1887

De-confounding the neural constitution of phenomenal consciousness from attention, report and memory

image of De-confounding the neural constitution of phenomenal consciousness from attention, report and memory

In this chapter, we discuss possible confounds that need to be carefully controlled for when the neural constitution of phenomenal conscious perception is studied. First, we discuss previous psychological or neural studies that clearly dissociated confounding factors (in particular, attention, report and memory) from phenomenal consciousness. We discuss several important phenomena and experimental methods (e.g., aftereffects, change blindness and inattentional blindness, and brain-imaging results) whose implications for the theory of consciousness might be contaminated by the influence of attention, report and memory. Finally, we discuss necessity and sufficiency of report and memory for phenomenal consciousness.

  • Affiliations: 1: Monash University, Melbourne

References

  1. Baars, B.J
    (2005) Global workspace theory of consciousness: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience. Progress in Brain Research, 150, 45–53. doi: 10.1016/S0079‑6123(05)50004‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50004-9 [Google Scholar]
  2. Bachmann, T
    (2006) A single metatheoretical framework for a number of conscious-vision phenomena. In Q. Jing , M.R. Rosenzweig , G. d’Ydewalle , H. Zhang , H.-C. Chen , & K. Zhang (Eds.), Progress in psychological science around the world. Volume 1 Neural, cognitive and developmental issues (pp.229–242). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Bachmann, T. , & Murd, C
    (2010) Covert spatial attention in search for the location of a color-afterimage patch speeds up its decay from awareness: Introducing a method useful for the study of neural correlates of visual awareness. Vision Research, 50(11), 1048–1053. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.03.013
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.03.013 [Google Scholar]
  4. Bauby, J.-B
    (1997) The diving bell and the butterfly ( J. Leggatt , Trans.). New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Blake, R. , & Logothetis, N.K
    (2002) Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(1), 13–21. doi: 10.1038/nrn701
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn701 [Google Scholar]
  6. Block, N
    (2005) Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 46–52. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.006 [Google Scholar]
  7. (2007) Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(5–6), 481–499.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. (2011) Perceptual consciousness overflows cognitive access. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(12), 567–575. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.001 [Google Scholar]
  9. Brascamp, J.W. , van Boxtel, J.J.A. , Knapen, T.H.J. , & Blake, R
    (2010) A dissociation of attention and awareness in phase-sensitive but not phase-insensitive visual channels. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(10), 2326–2344. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21397
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21397 [Google Scholar]
  10. Braun, J
    (2001) It’s great but not necessarily about attention. Psyche, 7(6). Retrieved fromwww.theassc.org/files/assc/2500.pdf
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown, R
    (2011) The myth of phenomenological overflow. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(2), 599–604. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.06.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.06.005 [Google Scholar]
  12. Cartwright-Finch, U. , & Lavie, N
    (2007) The role of perceptual load in inattentional blindness. Cognition, 102(3), 321–340. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.002 [Google Scholar]
  13. Castelo-Branco M. , Kozak, L.R. , Formisano E. , Teixeira J. , Xavier J. , & Goebel R
    (2009) Type of featural attention differentially modulates hMT + responses to illusory motion aftereffects. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102(5), 3016–3025. doi: 10.1152/jn.90812.2008
    https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90812.2008 [Google Scholar]
  14. Cattaneo, Z. , Vecchi, T. , Pascual-Leone, A. , & Silvanto, J
    (2009) Contrasting early visual cortical activation states causally involved in visual imagery and short-term memory. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(7), 1393–1400. doi: 10.1111/j.1460‑9568.2009.06911.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06911.x [Google Scholar]
  15. Chalmers, D.J
    (1996) The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Chun, M.M. , & Wolfe, J.M
    (2001) Visual attention. In E.B. Goldstein (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of perception (pp.272–310). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Cohen, M.A. , Alvarez, G.A. , & Nakayama, K
    (2011) Natural-scene perception requires attention. Psychological Science, 22(9), 1165–1172. doi: 10.1177/0956797611419168
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611419168 [Google Scholar]
  18. Coltheart, M
    (1980a) Iconic memory and visible persistence. Perception & Psychophysics, 27(3), 183–228. doi: 10.3758/BF03204258
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03204258 [Google Scholar]
  19. (1980b) The persistences of vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 290(1038), 57–69. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1980.0082
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1980.0082 [Google Scholar]
  20. De Brigard, F. , & Prinz, J
    (2010) Attention and consciousness. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(1), 51–59. doi: 10.1002/wcs.27
    https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.27 [Google Scholar]
  21. de Gardelle, V. , Sackur, J. , & Kouider, S
    (2009) Perceptual illusions in brief visual presentations. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(3), 569–577. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.03.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.03.002 [Google Scholar]
  22. de Graaf, T.A. , de Jong, M.C. , Goebel, R. , van Ee, R. , & Sack, A.T
    (2011) On the functional relevance of frontal cortex for passive and voluntarily controlled bistable vision. Cerebral Cortex, 21(10), 2322–2331. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr015
    https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr015 [Google Scholar]
  23. Deco, G. , & Rolls, E.T
    (2005) Attention, short-term memory, and action selection: A unifying theory. Progress in Neurobiology, 76(4), 236–256. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.08.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.08.004 [Google Scholar]
  24. Dehaene, S. , Changeux, J.-P. , Naccache, L. , Sackur, J. , & Sergent, C
    (2006) Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204–211. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007 [Google Scholar]
  25. Enoksson, P
    (1968) Studies in optokinetic binocular rivalry with a new device. Acta Ophthalmologica, 46(1), 71–74. doi: 10.1111/j.1755‑3768.1968.tb02496.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1755-3768.1968.tb02496.x [Google Scholar]
  26. Fei-Fei, L. , VanRullen, R. , Koch, C. , & Perona, P
    (2002) Rapid natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99(14), 9596–9601. doi: 10.1073/pnas.092277599
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.092277599 [Google Scholar]
  27. Fernández-Espejo, D. , & Owen, A.M
    (2013) Detecting awareness after severe brain injury. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(11), 801–809. doi: 10.1038/nrn3608
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3608 [Google Scholar]
  28. Fox, R. , Todd, S. , & Bettinger, L.A
    (1975) Optokinetic nystagmus as an objective indicator of binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 15(7), 849–853. doi: 10.1016/0042‑6989(75)90265‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(75)90265-5 [Google Scholar]
  29. Frässle, S. , Sommer, J. , Jansen, A. , Naber, M. , & Einhäuser, W
    (2014) Binocular rivalry: Frontal activity relates to introspection and action but not to perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(5), 1738–1747. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4403‑13.2014
    https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4403-13.2014 [Google Scholar]
  30. Fries, P. , Roelfsema, P.R. , Engel, A.K. , König, P. , & Singer, W
    (1997) Synchronization of oscillatory responses in visual cortex correlates with perception in interocular rivalry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 94(23), 12699–12704. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.23.12699
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.94.23.12699 [Google Scholar]
  31. Hampshire, A. , Chamberlain, S.R. , Monti, M.M. , Duncan, J. , & Owen, A.M
    (2010) The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: Inhibition and attentional control. Neuroimage, 50(3), 1313–1319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.109
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.109 [Google Scholar]
  32. Hardcastle, V.G
    (1997) Attention versus consciousness: A distinction with a difference. Cognitive Studies, 4(3), 56–66.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Hayashi, R. , & Tanifuji, M
    (2012) Which image is in awareness during binocular rivalry? Reading perceptual status from eye movements. Journal of Vision, 12(3), 5. doi: 10.1167/12.3.5
    https://doi.org/10.1167/12.3.5 [Google Scholar]
  34. Henderson, J.M. , & Hollingworth, A
    (1999) High-level scene perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 243–271. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.243
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.243 [Google Scholar]
  35. Huk, A.C. , Ress, D. , & Heeger, D.J
    (2001) Neuronal basis of the motion aftereffect reconsidered. Neuron, 32(1), 161–172. doi: 10.1016/S0896‑6273(01)00452‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00452-4 [Google Scholar]
  36. Hulme, O.J. , Friston, K.F. , & Zeki, S
    (2009) Neural correlates of stimulus reportability. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(8), 1602–1610. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21119
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21119 [Google Scholar]
  37. Imamoglu, F. , Kahnt, T. , Koch, C. , & Haynes, J.-D
    (2012) Changes in functional connectivity support conscious object recognition. Neuroimage, 63(4), 1909–1917. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.056
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.056 [Google Scholar]
  38. Iwasaki, S
    (1993) Spatial attention and two modes of visual consciousness. Cognition, 49(3), 211–233. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0277(93)90005‑G
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90005-G [Google Scholar]
  39. Jack, A.I. , Shulman, G.L. , Snyder, A.Z. , McAvoy, M. , & Corbetta, M
    (2006) Separate modulations of human V1 associated with spatial attention and task structure. Neuron, 51(1), 135–147. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.003 [Google Scholar]
  40. Jackendoff, R
    (1996) How language helps us think. Pragmatics and Cognition, 4(1), 1–34. doi: 10.1075/pc.4.1.03jac
    https://doi.org/10.1075/pc.4.1.03jac [Google Scholar]
  41. Jonides, J. , Lewis, R.L. , Nee, D.E. , Lustig, C.A. , Berman, M.G. , & Moore, K.S
    (2008) The mind and brain of short-term memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 193–224. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093615
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093615 [Google Scholar]
  42. Kanai, R. , Carmel, D. , Bahrami, B. , & Rees, G
    (2011) Structural and functional fractionation of right superior parietal cortex in bistable perception. Current Biology, 21(3), R106–R107. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.12.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.12.009 [Google Scholar]
  43. Kanai, R. , & Rees, G
    (2011) The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(4), 231–242. doi: 10.1038/nrn3000
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3000 [Google Scholar]
  44. Kanai, R. , & Tsuchiya, N
    (2012) Qualia. Current Biology, 22(10), R392–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.033
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.033 [Google Scholar]
  45. Kanai, R. , Tsuchiya, N. , & Verstraten, F.A
    (2006) The scope and limits of top-down attention in unconscious visual processing. Current Biology, 16(23), 2332–2336. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.001 [Google Scholar]
  46. Kentridge, R.W. , Heywood, C.A. , & Weiskrantz, L
    (2004) Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight. Neuropsychologia, 42(6), 831–835. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.001 [Google Scholar]
  47. Keogh, R. , & Pearson, J
    (2011) Mental imagery and visual working memory. PLoS ONE, 6(12), e29221. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029221
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029221 [Google Scholar]
  48. Knapen, T. , Brascamp, J. , Pearson, J. , van Ee, R. , & Blake, R
    (2011) The role of frontal and parietal brain areas in bistable perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(28), 10293–10301. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1727‑11.2011
    https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1727-11.2011 [Google Scholar]
  49. Koch, C
    (2004) The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Englewood, CO: Roberts & Company.
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Koch, C. , & Tsuchiya, N
    (2007) Attention and consciousness: Two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.012
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.012 [Google Scholar]
  51. (2012) Attention and consciousness: Related yet different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 103–105. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.012
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.012 [Google Scholar]
  52. Koivisto, M. , & Silvanto, J
    (2011) Relationship between visual binding, reentry and awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1293–1303. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.02.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.02.008 [Google Scholar]
  53. Kok, P. , Rahnev, D. , Jehee, J.F.M. , Lau, H.C. , & de Lange, F.P
    (2012) Attention reverses the effect of prediction in silencing sensory signals. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 2197–2206. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr310
    https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr310 [Google Scholar]
  54. Kouider, S. , Stahlhut, C. , Gelskov, S.V. , Barbosa, L.S. , Dutat, M. , de Gardelle, V. , Christophe, A. , Dehaene, S. , & Dehaene-Lambertz, G
    (2013) A neural marker of perceptual consciousness in infants. Science, 340(6130), 376–380. doi: 10.1126/science.1232509
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1232509 [Google Scholar]
  55. Lamme, V.A.F
    (2003) Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 12–18. doi: 10.1016/S1364‑6613(02)00013‑X
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-X [Google Scholar]
  56. (2010) How neuroscience will change our view on consciousness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(3), 204–220. doi: 10.1080/17588921003731586
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17588921003731586 [Google Scholar]
  57. Landman, R. , Spekreijse, H. , & Lamme, V.A.F
    (2003) Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Research, 43(2), 149–164. doi: 10.1016/S0042‑6989(02)00402‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00402-9 [Google Scholar]
  58. Lau, H. , & Rosenthal, D
    (2011) Empirical support for higher-order theories of conscious awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(8), 365–373. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.009 [Google Scholar]
  59. Laureys, S
    (2005) The neural correlate of (un)awareness: Lessons from the vegetative state. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(12), 556–559. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.010 [Google Scholar]
  60. Lee, S.-H. , Blake, R. , & Heeger, D.J
    (2007) Hierarchy of cortical responses underlying binocular rivalry. Nature Neuroscience, 10(8), 1048–1054. doi: 10.1038/nn1939
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1939 [Google Scholar]
  61. Leopold, D.A. , Wilke, M. , Maier, A. , & Logothetis, N.K
    (2002) Stable perception of visually ambiguous patterns. Nature Neuroscience, 5(6), 605–609. doi: 10.1038/nn0602‑851
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nn0602-851 [Google Scholar]
  62. Logothetis, N.K
    (2002) The neural basis of the blood-oxygen-level-dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging signal. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 357(1424), 1003–1037. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1114
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1114 [Google Scholar]
  63. Lumer, E.D. , Friston, K.J. , & Rees, G
    (1998) Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. Science, 280(5371), 1930–1934. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5371.1930
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.280.5371.1930 [Google Scholar]
  64. Mack, A. , & Rock, I
    (1998) Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Marchetti, G
    (2012) Against the view that consciousness and attention are fully dissociable. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 36. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00036
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00036 [Google Scholar]
  66. Merikle, P.M. , & Joordens, S
    (1997) Parallels between perception without attention and perception without awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 6(2–3), 219–236. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1997.0310
    https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1997.0310 [Google Scholar]
  67. Miller, S.M
    (2007) On the correlation/constitution distinction problem (and other hard problems) in the scientific study of consciousness. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 19(3), 159–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1601‑5215.2007.00207.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-5215.2007.00207.x [Google Scholar]
  68. (Ed.) (2013) The constitution of visual consciousness: Lessons from binocular rivalry. Advances in Consciousness Research (Vol. 90). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company. doi: 10.1075/aicr.90
    https://doi.org/10.1075/aicr.90 [Google Scholar]
  69. Mole, C
    (2008) Attention and consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15(4), 86–104.
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Monti, M.M. , Vanhaudenhuyse, A. , Coleman, M.R. , Boly, M. , Pickard, J.D. , Tshibanda, L. , Owen, A.M. , & Laureys, S
    (2010) Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. New England Journal of Medicine, 362(7), 579–589. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0905370
    https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa0905370 [Google Scholar]
  71. Murd, C. , & Bachmann, T
    (2011) Spatially localized motion aftereffect disappears faster from awareness when selectively attended to according to its direction. Vision Research, 51(10), 1157–1162. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.03.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.03.008 [Google Scholar]
  72. Naccache, L. , Blandin, E. , & Dehaene, S
    (2002) Unconscious masked priming depends on temporal attention. Psychological Science, 13(5), 416–424. doi: 10.1111/1467‑9280.00474
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00474 [Google Scholar]
  73. Oberauer, K
    (2002) Access to information in working memory: Exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(3), 411–421. doi: 10.1037/0278‑7393.28.3.411
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.411 [Google Scholar]
  74. Oizumi, M. , Albantakis, L. , & Tononi, G
    (2014) From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: Integrated Information Theory 3.0. PLoS Computational Biology, 10(5), e1003588. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003588
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003588 [Google Scholar]
  75. O’Regan, J.K
    (1992) Solving the “real” mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46(3), 461–488. doi: 10.1037/h0084327
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084327 [Google Scholar]
  76. O’Regan, J.K. , & Noë, A
    (2001) A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 939–973. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000115
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01000115 [Google Scholar]
  77. Owen, A.M. , Coleman, M.R. , Boly, M. , Davis, M.H. , Laureys, S. , & Pickard, J.D
    (2006) Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science, 313(5792), 1402. doi: 10.1126/science.1130197
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1130197 [Google Scholar]
  78. Pessoa, L. , & Padmala, S
    (2005) Quantitative prediction of perceptual decisions during near-threshold fear detection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102(15), 5612–5617. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500566102
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0500566102 [Google Scholar]
  79. Phillips, I.B
    (2011) Perception and iconic memory: What Sperling doesn’t show. Mind & Language, 26(4), 381–411. doi: 10.1111/j.1468‑0017.2011.01422.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2011.01422.x [Google Scholar]
  80. Pinto, Y. , Sligte, I.G. , Shapiro, K.L. , & Lamme, V.A.F
    (2013) Fragile visual short-term memory is an object-based and location-specific store. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(4), 732–739. doi: 10.3758/s13423‑013‑0393‑4
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0393-4 [Google Scholar]
  81. Polonsky, A. , Blake, R. , Braun, J. , & Heeger, D.J
    (2000) Neuronal activity in human primary visual cortex correlates with perception during binocular rivalry. Nature Neuroscience, 3(11), 1153–1159. doi: 10.1038/80676
    https://doi.org/10.1038/80676 [Google Scholar]
  82. Posner, M.I
    (1994) Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 91(16), 7398–7403.
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Prinz, J
    (2010) When is perception conscious?In B. Nanay (Ed.), Perceiving the world: New essays on perception (pp.310–332). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195386196.003.0011
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195386196.003.0011 [Google Scholar]
  84. Ramsøy, T.Z. , & Overgaard, M
    (2004) Introspection and subliminal perception. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 3(1), 1–23. doi: 10.1023/B:PHEN.0000041900.30172.e8
    https://doi.org/10.1023/B:PHEN.0000041900.30172.e8 [Google Scholar]
  85. Rensink, R.A
    (2002) Change detection. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 245–277. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135125
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135125 [Google Scholar]
  86. Rensink, R.A. , O’Regan, J.K. , & Clark, J.J
    (1997) To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8(5), 368–373. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.1997.tb00427.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00427.x [Google Scholar]
  87. Ress, D. , & Heeger, D.J
    (2003) Neuronal correlates of perception in early visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 6(4), 414–420. doi: 10.1038/nn1024
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1024 [Google Scholar]
  88. Ricci, C. , & Blundo, C
    (1990) Perception of ambiguous figures after focal brain lesions. Neuropsychologia, 28(11), 1163–1173. doi: 10.1016/0028‑3932(90)90052‑P
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(90)90052-P [Google Scholar]
  89. Sampanes, A.C. , Tseng, P. , & Bridgeman, B
    (2008) The role of gist in scene recognition. Vision Research, 48(21), 2275–2283. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.07.011
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2008.07.011 [Google Scholar]
  90. Siclari, F. , Larocque, J.J. , Postle, B.R. , & Tononi, G
    (2013) Assessing sleep consciousness within subjects using a serial awakening paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 542. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00542
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00542 [Google Scholar]
  91. Sligte, I.G. , Scholte, H.S. , & Lamme, V.A.F
    (2008) Are there multiple visual short-term memory stores?PLoS ONE, 3(2), e1699. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001699
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001699 [Google Scholar]
  92. Sperling, G
    (1960) The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 74(11), 1–29. doi: 10.1037/h0093759
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093759 [Google Scholar]
  93. Srinivasan, N. , Srivastava, P. , Lohani, M. , & Baijal, S
    (2009) Focused and distributed attention. Progress in Brain Research, 176, 87–100. doi: 10.1016/S0079‑6123(09)17606‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17606-9 [Google Scholar]
  94. Sterzer, P. , & Kleinschmidt, A
    (2007) A neural basis for inference in perceptual ambiguity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104(1), 323–328. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609006104
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0609006104 [Google Scholar]
  95. Sterzer, P. , Kleinschmidt, A. , & Rees, G
    (2009) The neural bases of multistable perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(7), 310–318. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.006 [Google Scholar]
  96. Summerfield, C. , & Egner, T
    (2009) Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(9), 403–409. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.003 [Google Scholar]
  97. Swick, D. , Ashley, V. , & Turken, A.U
    (2008) Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neuroscience, 9, 102. doi: 10.1186/1471‑2202‑9‑102
    https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-9-102 [Google Scholar]
  98. Tong, F. , & Engel, S.A
    (2001) Interocular rivalry revealed in the human cortical blind-spot representation. Nature, 411(6834), 195–199. doi: 10.1038/35075583
    https://doi.org/10.1038/35075583 [Google Scholar]
  99. Tong, F. , Meng, M. , & Blake, R
    (2006) Neural bases of binocular rivalry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(11), 502–511. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.003 [Google Scholar]
  100. Tononi, G
    (2010) Information integration: Its relevance to brain function and consciousness. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 148(3), 299–322.
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Tootell, R.B.H. , Reppas, J.B. , Dale, A.M. , Look, R.B. , Sereno, M.I. , Malach, R. , Brady, T.J. , & Rosen, B.R
    (1995) Visual motion aftereffects in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature, 375(6527), 139–141. doi: 10.1038/375139a0
    https://doi.org/10.1038/375139a0 [Google Scholar]
  102. Treisman, A. , & Schmidt, H
    (1982) Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects. Cognitive Psychology, 14(1), 107–141. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0285(82)90006‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(82)90006-8 [Google Scholar]
  103. Tse, P.U
    (2004) Mapping visual attention with change blindness: New directions for a new method. Cognitive Science, 28(2), 241–258. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2802_6
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2802_6 [Google Scholar]
  104. Tse, P.U. , Martinez-Conde, S. , Schlegel, A.A. , & Macknik, S.L
    (2005) Visibility, visual awareness, and visual masking of simple unattended targets are confined to areas in the occipital cortex beyond human V1/V2. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102(47), 17178–17183. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0508010102
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0508010102 [Google Scholar]
  105. Tsuchiya, N. , & Koch, C
    (2008a) Attention and consciousness. Scholarpedia, 3(5), 4173. doi: 10.4249/scholarpedia.4173
    https://doi.org/10.4249/scholarpedia.4173 [Google Scholar]
  106. (2008b) The relationship between consciousness and attention. In S. Laureys & G. Tononi (Eds.), The neurology of consciousness: Cognitive neuroscience and neuropathology (pp.63–77). London, UK: Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  107. van Boxtel, J.J.A. , Tsuchiya, N. , & Koch, C
    (2010a) Consciousness and attention: On sufficiency and necessity. Frontiers in Consciousness Research, 1, 217. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00217
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00217 [Google Scholar]
  108. (2010b) Opposing effects of attention and consciousness on afterimages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(19), 8883–8888. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913292107
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0913292107 [Google Scholar]
  109. Vandenbroucke, A.R.E. , Sligte, I.G. , Barrett, A.B. , Seth, A.K. , Fahrenfort, J.J. , & Lamme, V.A.F
    (2014) Accurate metacognition for visual sensory memory representations. Psychological Science, 25(4), 861–873. doi: 10.1177/0956797613516146
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613516146 [Google Scholar]
  110. Vandenbroucke, A.R.E. , Sligte, I.G. , & Lamme, V.A.F
    (2011) Manipulations of attention dissociate fragile visual short-term memory from visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1559–1568. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.044
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.044 [Google Scholar]
  111. Velmans, M
    (Ed.) (1996) The science of consciousness: Psychological, neuropsychological and clinical reviews. London, UK: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203360019
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203360019 [Google Scholar]
  112. Watanabe, M. , Cheng, K. , Murayama, Y. , Ueno, K. , Asamizuya, T. , Tanaka, K. , & Logothetis, N
    (2011) Attention but not awareness modulates the BOLD signal in the human V1 during binocular suppression. Science, 334(6057), 829–831. doi: 10.1126/science.1203161
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1203161 [Google Scholar]
  113. Wilke, M. , Logothetis, N.K. , & Leopold, D.A
    (2003) Generalized flash suppression of salient visual targets. Neuron, 39(6), 1043–1052. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2003.08.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2003.08.003 [Google Scholar]
  114. Wilke, M. , Mueller, K.M. , & Leopold, D.A
    (2009) Neural activity in the visual thalamus reflects perceptual suppression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 106(23), 9465–9470. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900714106
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0900714106 [Google Scholar]
  115. Wolfe, J.M
    (1999) Inattentional amnesia. In V. Coltheart (Ed.), Fleeting memories: Cognition of brief visual stimuli (pp.71–94). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Wolfe, J.M. , Horowitz, T.S. , & Kenner, N.M
    (2005) Cognitive psychology: Rare items often missed in visual searches. Nature, 435(7041), 439–440. doi: 10.1038/435439a
    https://doi.org/10.1038/435439a [Google Scholar]
  117. Wolfe, J.M. , Reinecke, A. , & Brawn, P
    (2006) Why don’t we see changes?: The role of attentional bottlenecks and limited visual memory. Visual Cognition, 14(4–8), 749–780. doi: 10.1080/13506280500195292
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280500195292 [Google Scholar]
  118. Woodman, G.F. , & Luck, S.J
    (2003) Dissociations among attention, perception, and awareness during object-substitution masking. Psychological Science, 14(6), 605–611. doi: 10.1046/j.0956‑7976.2003.psci_1472.x
    https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1472.x [Google Scholar]
  119. Wundt, W
    (1874) Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. Leipzig: Engelmann.
    [Google Scholar]

References

  1. Baars, B.J
    (2005) Global workspace theory of consciousness: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience. Progress in Brain Research, 150, 45–53. doi: 10.1016/S0079‑6123(05)50004‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50004-9 [Google Scholar]
  2. Bachmann, T
    (2006) A single metatheoretical framework for a number of conscious-vision phenomena. In Q. Jing , M.R. Rosenzweig , G. d’Ydewalle , H. Zhang , H.-C. Chen , & K. Zhang (Eds.), Progress in psychological science around the world. Volume 1 Neural, cognitive and developmental issues (pp.229–242). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Bachmann, T. , & Murd, C
    (2010) Covert spatial attention in search for the location of a color-afterimage patch speeds up its decay from awareness: Introducing a method useful for the study of neural correlates of visual awareness. Vision Research, 50(11), 1048–1053. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.03.013
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2010.03.013 [Google Scholar]
  4. Bauby, J.-B
    (1997) The diving bell and the butterfly ( J. Leggatt , Trans.). New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Blake, R. , & Logothetis, N.K
    (2002) Visual competition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(1), 13–21. doi: 10.1038/nrn701
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn701 [Google Scholar]
  6. Block, N
    (2005) Two neural correlates of consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 46–52. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.006 [Google Scholar]
  7. (2007) Consciousness, accessibility, and the mesh between psychology and neuroscience. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(5–6), 481–499.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. (2011) Perceptual consciousness overflows cognitive access. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(12), 567–575. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.001 [Google Scholar]
  9. Brascamp, J.W. , van Boxtel, J.J.A. , Knapen, T.H.J. , & Blake, R
    (2010) A dissociation of attention and awareness in phase-sensitive but not phase-insensitive visual channels. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(10), 2326–2344. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21397
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21397 [Google Scholar]
  10. Braun, J
    (2001) It’s great but not necessarily about attention. Psyche, 7(6). Retrieved fromwww.theassc.org/files/assc/2500.pdf
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown, R
    (2011) The myth of phenomenological overflow. Consciousness and Cognition, 21(2), 599–604. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.06.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.06.005 [Google Scholar]
  12. Cartwright-Finch, U. , & Lavie, N
    (2007) The role of perceptual load in inattentional blindness. Cognition, 102(3), 321–340. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.01.002 [Google Scholar]
  13. Castelo-Branco M. , Kozak, L.R. , Formisano E. , Teixeira J. , Xavier J. , & Goebel R
    (2009) Type of featural attention differentially modulates hMT + responses to illusory motion aftereffects. Journal of Neurophysiology, 102(5), 3016–3025. doi: 10.1152/jn.90812.2008
    https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.90812.2008 [Google Scholar]
  14. Cattaneo, Z. , Vecchi, T. , Pascual-Leone, A. , & Silvanto, J
    (2009) Contrasting early visual cortical activation states causally involved in visual imagery and short-term memory. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30(7), 1393–1400. doi: 10.1111/j.1460‑9568.2009.06911.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2009.06911.x [Google Scholar]
  15. Chalmers, D.J
    (1996) The conscious mind: In search of a fundamental theory. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Chun, M.M. , & Wolfe, J.M
    (2001) Visual attention. In E.B. Goldstein (Ed.), Blackwell handbook of perception (pp.272–310). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Cohen, M.A. , Alvarez, G.A. , & Nakayama, K
    (2011) Natural-scene perception requires attention. Psychological Science, 22(9), 1165–1172. doi: 10.1177/0956797611419168
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611419168 [Google Scholar]
  18. Coltheart, M
    (1980a) Iconic memory and visible persistence. Perception & Psychophysics, 27(3), 183–228. doi: 10.3758/BF03204258
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03204258 [Google Scholar]
  19. (1980b) The persistences of vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 290(1038), 57–69. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1980.0082
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1980.0082 [Google Scholar]
  20. De Brigard, F. , & Prinz, J
    (2010) Attention and consciousness. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1(1), 51–59. doi: 10.1002/wcs.27
    https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.27 [Google Scholar]
  21. de Gardelle, V. , Sackur, J. , & Kouider, S
    (2009) Perceptual illusions in brief visual presentations. Consciousness and Cognition, 18(3), 569–577. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2009.03.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2009.03.002 [Google Scholar]
  22. de Graaf, T.A. , de Jong, M.C. , Goebel, R. , van Ee, R. , & Sack, A.T
    (2011) On the functional relevance of frontal cortex for passive and voluntarily controlled bistable vision. Cerebral Cortex, 21(10), 2322–2331. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr015
    https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr015 [Google Scholar]
  23. Deco, G. , & Rolls, E.T
    (2005) Attention, short-term memory, and action selection: A unifying theory. Progress in Neurobiology, 76(4), 236–256. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.08.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2005.08.004 [Google Scholar]
  24. Dehaene, S. , Changeux, J.-P. , Naccache, L. , Sackur, J. , & Sergent, C
    (2006) Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(5), 204–211. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007 [Google Scholar]
  25. Enoksson, P
    (1968) Studies in optokinetic binocular rivalry with a new device. Acta Ophthalmologica, 46(1), 71–74. doi: 10.1111/j.1755‑3768.1968.tb02496.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1755-3768.1968.tb02496.x [Google Scholar]
  26. Fei-Fei, L. , VanRullen, R. , Koch, C. , & Perona, P
    (2002) Rapid natural scene categorization in the near absence of attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 99(14), 9596–9601. doi: 10.1073/pnas.092277599
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.092277599 [Google Scholar]
  27. Fernández-Espejo, D. , & Owen, A.M
    (2013) Detecting awareness after severe brain injury. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(11), 801–809. doi: 10.1038/nrn3608
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3608 [Google Scholar]
  28. Fox, R. , Todd, S. , & Bettinger, L.A
    (1975) Optokinetic nystagmus as an objective indicator of binocular rivalry. Vision Research, 15(7), 849–853. doi: 10.1016/0042‑6989(75)90265‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(75)90265-5 [Google Scholar]
  29. Frässle, S. , Sommer, J. , Jansen, A. , Naber, M. , & Einhäuser, W
    (2014) Binocular rivalry: Frontal activity relates to introspection and action but not to perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(5), 1738–1747. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4403‑13.2014
    https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4403-13.2014 [Google Scholar]
  30. Fries, P. , Roelfsema, P.R. , Engel, A.K. , König, P. , & Singer, W
    (1997) Synchronization of oscillatory responses in visual cortex correlates with perception in interocular rivalry. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 94(23), 12699–12704. doi: 10.1073/pnas.94.23.12699
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.94.23.12699 [Google Scholar]
  31. Hampshire, A. , Chamberlain, S.R. , Monti, M.M. , Duncan, J. , & Owen, A.M
    (2010) The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: Inhibition and attentional control. Neuroimage, 50(3), 1313–1319. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.109
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.109 [Google Scholar]
  32. Hardcastle, V.G
    (1997) Attention versus consciousness: A distinction with a difference. Cognitive Studies, 4(3), 56–66.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Hayashi, R. , & Tanifuji, M
    (2012) Which image is in awareness during binocular rivalry? Reading perceptual status from eye movements. Journal of Vision, 12(3), 5. doi: 10.1167/12.3.5
    https://doi.org/10.1167/12.3.5 [Google Scholar]
  34. Henderson, J.M. , & Hollingworth, A
    (1999) High-level scene perception. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 243–271. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.243
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.50.1.243 [Google Scholar]
  35. Huk, A.C. , Ress, D. , & Heeger, D.J
    (2001) Neuronal basis of the motion aftereffect reconsidered. Neuron, 32(1), 161–172. doi: 10.1016/S0896‑6273(01)00452‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00452-4 [Google Scholar]
  36. Hulme, O.J. , Friston, K.F. , & Zeki, S
    (2009) Neural correlates of stimulus reportability. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(8), 1602–1610. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21119
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21119 [Google Scholar]
  37. Imamoglu, F. , Kahnt, T. , Koch, C. , & Haynes, J.-D
    (2012) Changes in functional connectivity support conscious object recognition. Neuroimage, 63(4), 1909–1917. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.056
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.07.056 [Google Scholar]
  38. Iwasaki, S
    (1993) Spatial attention and two modes of visual consciousness. Cognition, 49(3), 211–233. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0277(93)90005‑G
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90005-G [Google Scholar]
  39. Jack, A.I. , Shulman, G.L. , Snyder, A.Z. , McAvoy, M. , & Corbetta, M
    (2006) Separate modulations of human V1 associated with spatial attention and task structure. Neuron, 51(1), 135–147. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.06.003 [Google Scholar]
  40. Jackendoff, R
    (1996) How language helps us think. Pragmatics and Cognition, 4(1), 1–34. doi: 10.1075/pc.4.1.03jac
    https://doi.org/10.1075/pc.4.1.03jac [Google Scholar]
  41. Jonides, J. , Lewis, R.L. , Nee, D.E. , Lustig, C.A. , Berman, M.G. , & Moore, K.S
    (2008) The mind and brain of short-term memory. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 193–224. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093615
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093615 [Google Scholar]
  42. Kanai, R. , Carmel, D. , Bahrami, B. , & Rees, G
    (2011) Structural and functional fractionation of right superior parietal cortex in bistable perception. Current Biology, 21(3), R106–R107. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.12.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.12.009 [Google Scholar]
  43. Kanai, R. , & Rees, G
    (2011) The structural basis of inter-individual differences in human behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(4), 231–242. doi: 10.1038/nrn3000
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3000 [Google Scholar]
  44. Kanai, R. , & Tsuchiya, N
    (2012) Qualia. Current Biology, 22(10), R392–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.033
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.033 [Google Scholar]
  45. Kanai, R. , Tsuchiya, N. , & Verstraten, F.A
    (2006) The scope and limits of top-down attention in unconscious visual processing. Current Biology, 16(23), 2332–2336. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.10.001 [Google Scholar]
  46. Kentridge, R.W. , Heywood, C.A. , & Weiskrantz, L
    (2004) Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight. Neuropsychologia, 42(6), 831–835. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.11.001 [Google Scholar]
  47. Keogh, R. , & Pearson, J
    (2011) Mental imagery and visual working memory. PLoS ONE, 6(12), e29221. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0029221
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029221 [Google Scholar]
  48. Knapen, T. , Brascamp, J. , Pearson, J. , van Ee, R. , & Blake, R
    (2011) The role of frontal and parietal brain areas in bistable perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(28), 10293–10301. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1727‑11.2011
    https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1727-11.2011 [Google Scholar]
  49. Koch, C
    (2004) The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Englewood, CO: Roberts & Company.
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Koch, C. , & Tsuchiya, N
    (2007) Attention and consciousness: Two distinct brain processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 16–22. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.012
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.012 [Google Scholar]
  51. (2012) Attention and consciousness: Related yet different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 103–105. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.012
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.11.012 [Google Scholar]
  52. Koivisto, M. , & Silvanto, J
    (2011) Relationship between visual binding, reentry and awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(4), 1293–1303. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.02.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.02.008 [Google Scholar]
  53. Kok, P. , Rahnev, D. , Jehee, J.F.M. , Lau, H.C. , & de Lange, F.P
    (2012) Attention reverses the effect of prediction in silencing sensory signals. Cerebral Cortex, 22(9), 2197–2206. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr310
    https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr310 [Google Scholar]
  54. Kouider, S. , Stahlhut, C. , Gelskov, S.V. , Barbosa, L.S. , Dutat, M. , de Gardelle, V. , Christophe, A. , Dehaene, S. , & Dehaene-Lambertz, G
    (2013) A neural marker of perceptual consciousness in infants. Science, 340(6130), 376–380. doi: 10.1126/science.1232509
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1232509 [Google Scholar]
  55. Lamme, V.A.F
    (2003) Why visual attention and awareness are different. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 12–18. doi: 10.1016/S1364‑6613(02)00013‑X
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00013-X [Google Scholar]
  56. (2010) How neuroscience will change our view on consciousness. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(3), 204–220. doi: 10.1080/17588921003731586
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17588921003731586 [Google Scholar]
  57. Landman, R. , Spekreijse, H. , & Lamme, V.A.F
    (2003) Large capacity storage of integrated objects before change blindness. Vision Research, 43(2), 149–164. doi: 10.1016/S0042‑6989(02)00402‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0042-6989(02)00402-9 [Google Scholar]
  58. Lau, H. , & Rosenthal, D
    (2011) Empirical support for higher-order theories of conscious awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(8), 365–373. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.05.009 [Google Scholar]
  59. Laureys, S
    (2005) The neural correlate of (un)awareness: Lessons from the vegetative state. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(12), 556–559. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.010 [Google Scholar]
  60. Lee, S.-H. , Blake, R. , & Heeger, D.J
    (2007) Hierarchy of cortical responses underlying binocular rivalry. Nature Neuroscience, 10(8), 1048–1054. doi: 10.1038/nn1939
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1939 [Google Scholar]
  61. Leopold, D.A. , Wilke, M. , Maier, A. , & Logothetis, N.K
    (2002) Stable perception of visually ambiguous patterns. Nature Neuroscience, 5(6), 605–609. doi: 10.1038/nn0602‑851
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nn0602-851 [Google Scholar]
  62. Logothetis, N.K
    (2002) The neural basis of the blood-oxygen-level-dependent functional magnetic resonance imaging signal. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 357(1424), 1003–1037. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1114
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2002.1114 [Google Scholar]
  63. Lumer, E.D. , Friston, K.J. , & Rees, G
    (1998) Neural correlates of perceptual rivalry in the human brain. Science, 280(5371), 1930–1934. doi: 10.1126/science.280.5371.1930
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.280.5371.1930 [Google Scholar]
  64. Mack, A. , & Rock, I
    (1998) Inattentional blindness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Marchetti, G
    (2012) Against the view that consciousness and attention are fully dissociable. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 36. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00036
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00036 [Google Scholar]
  66. Merikle, P.M. , & Joordens, S
    (1997) Parallels between perception without attention and perception without awareness. Consciousness and Cognition, 6(2–3), 219–236. doi: 10.1006/ccog.1997.0310
    https://doi.org/10.1006/ccog.1997.0310 [Google Scholar]
  67. Miller, S.M
    (2007) On the correlation/constitution distinction problem (and other hard problems) in the scientific study of consciousness. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 19(3), 159–176. doi: 10.1111/j.1601‑5215.2007.00207.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-5215.2007.00207.x [Google Scholar]
  68. (Ed.) (2013) The constitution of visual consciousness: Lessons from binocular rivalry. Advances in Consciousness Research (Vol. 90). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company. doi: 10.1075/aicr.90
    https://doi.org/10.1075/aicr.90 [Google Scholar]
  69. Mole, C
    (2008) Attention and consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15(4), 86–104.
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Monti, M.M. , Vanhaudenhuyse, A. , Coleman, M.R. , Boly, M. , Pickard, J.D. , Tshibanda, L. , Owen, A.M. , & Laureys, S
    (2010) Willful modulation of brain activity in disorders of consciousness. New England Journal of Medicine, 362(7), 579–589. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa0905370
    https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMoa0905370 [Google Scholar]
  71. Murd, C. , & Bachmann, T
    (2011) Spatially localized motion aftereffect disappears faster from awareness when selectively attended to according to its direction. Vision Research, 51(10), 1157–1162. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.03.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2011.03.008 [Google Scholar]
  72. Naccache, L. , Blandin, E. , & Dehaene, S
    (2002) Unconscious masked priming depends on temporal attention. Psychological Science, 13(5), 416–424. doi: 10.1111/1467‑9280.00474
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00474 [Google Scholar]
  73. Oberauer, K
    (2002) Access to information in working memory: Exploring the focus of attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(3), 411–421. doi: 10.1037/0278‑7393.28.3.411
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.28.3.411 [Google Scholar]
  74. Oizumi, M. , Albantakis, L. , & Tononi, G
    (2014) From the phenomenology to the mechanisms of consciousness: Integrated Information Theory 3.0. PLoS Computational Biology, 10(5), e1003588. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003588
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1003588 [Google Scholar]
  75. O’Regan, J.K
    (1992) Solving the “real” mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 46(3), 461–488. doi: 10.1037/h0084327
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084327 [Google Scholar]
  76. O’Regan, J.K. , & Noë, A
    (2001) A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 939–973. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000115
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01000115 [Google Scholar]
  77. Owen, A.M. , Coleman, M.R. , Boly, M. , Davis, M.H. , Laureys, S. , & Pickard, J.D
    (2006) Detecting awareness in the vegetative state. Science, 313(5792), 1402. doi: 10.1126/science.1130197
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1130197 [Google Scholar]
  78. Pessoa, L. , & Padmala, S
    (2005) Quantitative prediction of perceptual decisions during near-threshold fear detection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102(15), 5612–5617. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0500566102
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0500566102 [Google Scholar]
  79. Phillips, I.B
    (2011) Perception and iconic memory: What Sperling doesn’t show. Mind & Language, 26(4), 381–411. doi: 10.1111/j.1468‑0017.2011.01422.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0017.2011.01422.x [Google Scholar]
  80. Pinto, Y. , Sligte, I.G. , Shapiro, K.L. , & Lamme, V.A.F
    (2013) Fragile visual short-term memory is an object-based and location-specific store. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(4), 732–739. doi: 10.3758/s13423‑013‑0393‑4
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0393-4 [Google Scholar]
  81. Polonsky, A. , Blake, R. , Braun, J. , & Heeger, D.J
    (2000) Neuronal activity in human primary visual cortex correlates with perception during binocular rivalry. Nature Neuroscience, 3(11), 1153–1159. doi: 10.1038/80676
    https://doi.org/10.1038/80676 [Google Scholar]
  82. Posner, M.I
    (1994) Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 91(16), 7398–7403.
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Prinz, J
    (2010) When is perception conscious?In B. Nanay (Ed.), Perceiving the world: New essays on perception (pp.310–332). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195386196.003.0011
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195386196.003.0011 [Google Scholar]
  84. Ramsøy, T.Z. , & Overgaard, M
    (2004) Introspection and subliminal perception. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 3(1), 1–23. doi: 10.1023/B:PHEN.0000041900.30172.e8
    https://doi.org/10.1023/B:PHEN.0000041900.30172.e8 [Google Scholar]
  85. Rensink, R.A
    (2002) Change detection. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 245–277. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135125
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135125 [Google Scholar]
  86. Rensink, R.A. , O’Regan, J.K. , & Clark, J.J
    (1997) To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes. Psychological Science, 8(5), 368–373. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.1997.tb00427.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00427.x [Google Scholar]
  87. Ress, D. , & Heeger, D.J
    (2003) Neuronal correlates of perception in early visual cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 6(4), 414–420. doi: 10.1038/nn1024
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1024 [Google Scholar]
  88. Ricci, C. , & Blundo, C
    (1990) Perception of ambiguous figures after focal brain lesions. Neuropsychologia, 28(11), 1163–1173. doi: 10.1016/0028‑3932(90)90052‑P
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(90)90052-P [Google Scholar]
  89. Sampanes, A.C. , Tseng, P. , & Bridgeman, B
    (2008) The role of gist in scene recognition. Vision Research, 48(21), 2275–2283. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.07.011
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2008.07.011 [Google Scholar]
  90. Siclari, F. , Larocque, J.J. , Postle, B.R. , & Tononi, G
    (2013) Assessing sleep consciousness within subjects using a serial awakening paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 542. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00542
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00542 [Google Scholar]
  91. Sligte, I.G. , Scholte, H.S. , & Lamme, V.A.F
    (2008) Are there multiple visual short-term memory stores?PLoS ONE, 3(2), e1699. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0001699
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0001699 [Google Scholar]
  92. Sperling, G
    (1960) The information available in brief visual presentations. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 74(11), 1–29. doi: 10.1037/h0093759
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093759 [Google Scholar]
  93. Srinivasan, N. , Srivastava, P. , Lohani, M. , & Baijal, S
    (2009) Focused and distributed attention. Progress in Brain Research, 176, 87–100. doi: 10.1016/S0079‑6123(09)17606‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17606-9 [Google Scholar]
  94. Sterzer, P. , & Kleinschmidt, A
    (2007) A neural basis for inference in perceptual ambiguity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 104(1), 323–328. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0609006104
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0609006104 [Google Scholar]
  95. Sterzer, P. , Kleinschmidt, A. , & Rees, G
    (2009) The neural bases of multistable perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(7), 310–318. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.006 [Google Scholar]
  96. Summerfield, C. , & Egner, T
    (2009) Expectation (and attention) in visual cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(9), 403–409. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.06.003 [Google Scholar]
  97. Swick, D. , Ashley, V. , & Turken, A.U
    (2008) Left inferior frontal gyrus is critical for response inhibition. BMC Neuroscience, 9, 102. doi: 10.1186/1471‑2202‑9‑102
    https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2202-9-102 [Google Scholar]
  98. Tong, F. , & Engel, S.A
    (2001) Interocular rivalry revealed in the human cortical blind-spot representation. Nature, 411(6834), 195–199. doi: 10.1038/35075583
    https://doi.org/10.1038/35075583 [Google Scholar]
  99. Tong, F. , Meng, M. , & Blake, R
    (2006) Neural bases of binocular rivalry. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(11), 502–511. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.09.003 [Google Scholar]
  100. Tononi, G
    (2010) Information integration: Its relevance to brain function and consciousness. Archives Italiennes de Biologie, 148(3), 299–322.
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Tootell, R.B.H. , Reppas, J.B. , Dale, A.M. , Look, R.B. , Sereno, M.I. , Malach, R. , Brady, T.J. , & Rosen, B.R
    (1995) Visual motion aftereffects in human cortical area MT revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nature, 375(6527), 139–141. doi: 10.1038/375139a0
    https://doi.org/10.1038/375139a0 [Google Scholar]
  102. Treisman, A. , & Schmidt, H
    (1982) Illusory conjunctions in the perception of objects. Cognitive Psychology, 14(1), 107–141. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0285(82)90006‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(82)90006-8 [Google Scholar]
  103. Tse, P.U
    (2004) Mapping visual attention with change blindness: New directions for a new method. Cognitive Science, 28(2), 241–258. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2802_6
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2802_6 [Google Scholar]
  104. Tse, P.U. , Martinez-Conde, S. , Schlegel, A.A. , & Macknik, S.L
    (2005) Visibility, visual awareness, and visual masking of simple unattended targets are confined to areas in the occipital cortex beyond human V1/V2. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 102(47), 17178–17183. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0508010102
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0508010102 [Google Scholar]
  105. Tsuchiya, N. , & Koch, C
    (2008a) Attention and consciousness. Scholarpedia, 3(5), 4173. doi: 10.4249/scholarpedia.4173
    https://doi.org/10.4249/scholarpedia.4173 [Google Scholar]
  106. (2008b) The relationship between consciousness and attention. In S. Laureys & G. Tononi (Eds.), The neurology of consciousness: Cognitive neuroscience and neuropathology (pp.63–77). London, UK: Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  107. van Boxtel, J.J.A. , Tsuchiya, N. , & Koch, C
    (2010a) Consciousness and attention: On sufficiency and necessity. Frontiers in Consciousness Research, 1, 217. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00217
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00217 [Google Scholar]
  108. (2010b) Opposing effects of attention and consciousness on afterimages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 107(19), 8883–8888. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0913292107
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0913292107 [Google Scholar]
  109. Vandenbroucke, A.R.E. , Sligte, I.G. , Barrett, A.B. , Seth, A.K. , Fahrenfort, J.J. , & Lamme, V.A.F
    (2014) Accurate metacognition for visual sensory memory representations. Psychological Science, 25(4), 861–873. doi: 10.1177/0956797613516146
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613516146 [Google Scholar]
  110. Vandenbroucke, A.R.E. , Sligte, I.G. , & Lamme, V.A.F
    (2011) Manipulations of attention dissociate fragile visual short-term memory from visual working memory. Neuropsychologia, 49(6), 1559–1568. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.044
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.044 [Google Scholar]
  111. Velmans, M
    (Ed.) (1996) The science of consciousness: Psychological, neuropsychological and clinical reviews. London, UK: Routledge. doi: 10.4324/9780203360019
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203360019 [Google Scholar]
  112. Watanabe, M. , Cheng, K. , Murayama, Y. , Ueno, K. , Asamizuya, T. , Tanaka, K. , & Logothetis, N
    (2011) Attention but not awareness modulates the BOLD signal in the human V1 during binocular suppression. Science, 334(6057), 829–831. doi: 10.1126/science.1203161
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1203161 [Google Scholar]
  113. Wilke, M. , Logothetis, N.K. , & Leopold, D.A
    (2003) Generalized flash suppression of salient visual targets. Neuron, 39(6), 1043–1052. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2003.08.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2003.08.003 [Google Scholar]
  114. Wilke, M. , Mueller, K.M. , & Leopold, D.A
    (2009) Neural activity in the visual thalamus reflects perceptual suppression. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 106(23), 9465–9470. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0900714106
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0900714106 [Google Scholar]
  115. Wolfe, J.M
    (1999) Inattentional amnesia. In V. Coltheart (Ed.), Fleeting memories: Cognition of brief visual stimuli (pp.71–94). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Wolfe, J.M. , Horowitz, T.S. , & Kenner, N.M
    (2005) Cognitive psychology: Rare items often missed in visual searches. Nature, 435(7041), 439–440. doi: 10.1038/435439a
    https://doi.org/10.1038/435439a [Google Scholar]
  117. Wolfe, J.M. , Reinecke, A. , & Brawn, P
    (2006) Why don’t we see changes?: The role of attentional bottlenecks and limited visual memory. Visual Cognition, 14(4–8), 749–780. doi: 10.1080/13506280500195292
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280500195292 [Google Scholar]
  118. Woodman, G.F. , & Luck, S.J
    (2003) Dissociations among attention, perception, and awareness during object-substitution masking. Psychological Science, 14(6), 605–611. doi: 10.1046/j.0956‑7976.2003.psci_1472.x
    https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0956-7976.2003.psci_1472.x [Google Scholar]
  119. Wundt, W
    (1874) Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie. Leipzig: Engelmann.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/books/9789027268785-aicr.92.04box
dcterms_subject,pub_keyword
-contentType:Journal
10
5
Chapter
content/books/9789027268785
Book
false
Loading
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error