1887

Learning conventions and conventionality through conversation

Despite assumptions that children learn linguistic and behavioral conventions through socialization, a systematic account of this developmental process is lacking. We ask how well existing evidence supports a sociocultural account explaining how children learn what is conventional, and that meanings are conventional. Specifically, we consider empirical support for two hypotheses: that parents provide systematic cues regarding conventions and conventionality to children, and that children learn from these cues. Considering research involving conventional word meanings, object uses, and social behaviors, we find impressive support for both hypotheses. Parent-child conversations may support children’s expectation that people within a community tend to know and use the same words, and have similar knowledge and expectations. Finally we discuss ongoing controversies and future research ideas.

References

  1. Akhtar, N. , & Menjivar, J
    (2012) Cognitive and linguistic correlates of early exposure to more than one language. In J.B. Benson (Ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior , Vol.42. Oxford: Elsevier.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Bialystok, E. , Craik, F.I.M. , & Luk, G
    (2012) Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 16, 240–250. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001 [Google Scholar]
  3. Brennan, S.E. , & Clark, H.H
    (1996) Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 22, 1482–1493. doi: 10.1037/0278‑7393.22.6.1482
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1482 [Google Scholar]
  4. Buresh, J. & Woodward, A
    (2007) Infants track action goals within and across agents. Cognition , 104, 287–314. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.07.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.07.001 [Google Scholar]
  5. Callanan, M
    (2000) Les noms d’objet dans les conversations entre parents et jeune enfants (Names for objects in conversations between young children and their parents). Psychologie Française , 45, 187–192.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Callanan, M. , Akhtar, N. , & Sussman, L
    (2012)  Children Learning Words from Ostensive and Directive Speech . Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Santa Cruz.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Callanan, M. , & Jipson, J.L
    (2001) Explanatory conversations and young children’s developing scientific literacy. In K. Crowley , C.D. Schuun , & T. Okada (Eds.), Designing for Science: Implications from Everyday, Classroom, and Professional Settings (pp.21–49). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Callanan, M. , Rigney, J. , Nolan-Reyes, C. , & Solis, G
    (2012) Beyond pedagogy: How children’s knowledge develops in the context of everyday parent-child conversations. In A. Pinkham , T. Kaefer , & S. Neuman . (Eds.), Knowledge Development in Early Childhood: How Young Children Build Knowledge and Why it Matters . New York, NY: Guilford.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Callanan, M. , & Sabbagh, M
    (2004) Multiple labels for objects in conversations with young children: Parents’ language and children’s developing expectations about word meanings. Developmental Psychology, 40, 746–763. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.40.5.746
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.746 [Google Scholar]
  10. Callanan, M.A. , Siegel, D. , & Luce, M.R
    (2007) Conventionality in family conversations about everyday objects. In M. Sabbagh & C. Kalish (Eds.), New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development (pp.83–97). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Cimpian, A. , & Scott, R
    (2012) Children expect generic knowledge to be widely shared. Cognition , 123, 419–433. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.003 [Google Scholar]
  12. Clark, E.V
    (1992) Conventionality and contrast: Pragmatic principles with lexical consequences. In A. Lehrer & E.F. Kittay (Eds.), Frames, Fields, and Contrasts (pp.171–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. (1993)  The Lexicon in Acquisition . Cambridge: CUP. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511554377
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511554377 [Google Scholar]
  14. (2007) Conventionality and contrast in language and language acquisition. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development , 115, 11–23. doi: 10.1002/cd.179
    https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.179 [Google Scholar]
  15. Csibra, G. , & Gergely, G
    (2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Science , 13(4), 148–153. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005 [Google Scholar]
  16. Diesendruck, G
    (2005) The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: An empirical examination. Developmental Psychology , 41, 451–463. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.41.3.451
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.3.451 [Google Scholar]
  17. Diesendruck, G. , & Markson, L
    (2001) Children’s avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology , 37, 630–641. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.37.5.630
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.630 [Google Scholar]
  18. (2011) Children’s assumption of the conventionality of culture. Child Development Perspectives , 5, 189–195. doi: 10.1111/j.1750‑8606.2010.00156.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00156.x [Google Scholar]
  19. Gauvain, M
    (2001)  The Social Context of Cognitive Development . New York, NY: Guilford.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Gelman, S
    (2003)  The Essentialist Child . Oxford: OUP. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195154061.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195154061.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  21. (2009) Learning from others: Children’s construction of concepts. Annual Review of Psychology , 60, 115–140. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093659
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093659 [Google Scholar]
  22. Gelman, S. , Coley, J. , Rosengren, K. , Hartman, E. , & Pappas, A
    (1998) Beyond labeling: The role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly structured categories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , 63 (Serial No. 253). doi: 10.2307/1166211
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1166211 [Google Scholar]
  23. German, T.P. , & Johnson, S.C
    (2002) Function and the origins of the design. Journal of Cognition and Development , 3, 279–300. doi: 10.1207/S15327647JCD0303_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0303_2 [Google Scholar]
  24. Grusec, J.E. , & Davidov, M
    (2010) Integrating different perspectives on socialization theory and research: A domain-specific approach. Child Development , 81, 687–709. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2010.01426.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01426.x [Google Scholar]
  25. Harris, P.L. , & Koenig, M.A
    (2006) Trust in testimony: How children learn about science and religion. Child Development , 77, 505–524. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2006.00886.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00886.x [Google Scholar]
  26. Hart, B. , & Risley, T
    (1995)  Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children . Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Henderson, A. , & Graham, S.A
    (2005) Two-year-olds’ appreciation of the shared nature of novel object labels. Journal of Cognition and Development , 6, 381–402. doi: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0603_4
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0603_4 [Google Scholar]
  28. Henderson, A. , & Sabbagh, M
    (2010) Parents’ use of conventional and unconventional labels in conversations with their preschoolers. Journal of Child Language , 37, 793–816. doi: 10.1017/S0305000909990122
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000909990122 [Google Scholar]
  29. Horner, V. , & Whiten, A
    (2005) Causal knowledge and emulation/imitation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition , 8, 164–181. doi: 10.1007/s10071‑004‑0239‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004-0239-6 [Google Scholar]
  30. Huttenlocher, J. , Haight, W. , Bryk, A. , Seltzer, M. , & Lyons, T
    (1991) Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology , 27, 236–248. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.27.2.236
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236 [Google Scholar]
  31. Jaswal, V.K
    (2004) Don’t believe everything you hear: Preschoolers’ sensitivity to speaker intent in category induction. Child Development , 75, 1871–1885. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2004.00822.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00822.x [Google Scholar]
  32. Jaswal, V.K. , & Neely, L
    (2006) Adults don’t always know best: Preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychological Science , 9, 757–758. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.2006.01778.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x [Google Scholar]
  33. Kalish, C.W. , & Cornelius, R
    (2007) What is to be done: Children’s ascriptions of conventional obligations. Child Development, 78 , 859–878. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2007.01037.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01037.x [Google Scholar]
  34. Kalish, C.W. , & Lawson, C.A
    (2008) Development of social category representations: Early appreciation of roles and deontic relations. Child Development , 79, 577–593. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2008.01144.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01144.x [Google Scholar]
  35. Kalish, C.W. , & Sabbagh, M.A
    (2007) Conventionality and cognitive development: Learning to think the right way. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development , 115, 1–10. doi: 10.1002/cd.178
    https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.178 [Google Scholar]
  36. Kelemen, D
    (1999) The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition , 70, 241–272. doi: 10.1016/S0010‑0277(99)00010‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00010-4 [Google Scholar]
  37. Kellman, P.J. , Massey, C.M. , & Son, J.Y
    (2010) Perceptual learning modules in mathematics: Enhancing students’ pattern recognition, structure extraction, and fluency. Topics in Cognitive Science , 2, 285–305. doi: 10.1111/j.1756‑8765.2009.01053.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01053.x [Google Scholar]
  38. Kidd, C. , White, K.S. , & Aslin, R.N
    (2011) Toddlers use speech disfluencies to predict speakers’ referential intentions. Developmental Science , 14, 925–934. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2011.01049.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01049.x [Google Scholar]
  39. Kinzler, K. , Corriveau, K. , & Harris, P
    (2011) Children’s selective trust in native-accented speakers. Developmental Science , 14, 106–111. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2011.01049.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01049.x [Google Scholar]
  40. Koenig, M.A. , & Harris, P.L
    (2005) Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development , 76, 1261–1277. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2005.00849.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x [Google Scholar]
  41. Koenig, M.A. , & Woodward, A.L
    (2010) 24-month-olds’ sensitivity to the prior inaccuracy of the source: Possible mechanisms. Developmental Psychology , 46, 815–826. doi: 10.1037/a0019664
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019664 [Google Scholar]
  42. Lewis, D.K
    (1969)  Convention: A Philosophical Study . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Lieven, E.V.M
    (1994) Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children. In C. Gallaway & B.J. Richards ( Eds .), Input and Interaction in Language Acquisition (pp. 56–73). Cambridge: CUP. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511620690.005
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620690.005 [Google Scholar]
  44. Luce, M. , & Callanan, M
    (2010) Parents’ object labeling: Possible links to conventionality of word meaning? First Language , 30, 270–286. doi: 10.1177/0142723710370543
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723710370543 [Google Scholar]
  45. Matthews, D. , Lieven, E. , & Tomasello, M
    (2010) What’s in a manner of speaking? Children’s sensitivity to partner-specific referential precedents. Developmental Psychology , 46, 749–760. doi: 10.1037/a0019657
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019657 [Google Scholar]
  46. Meltzoff, A
    (1995) Understanding the intention of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology , 31, 838–850. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.31.5.838
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838 [Google Scholar]
  47. Nucci, L. & Weber, E.K
    (1995) Social interactions in the home and the development of young children’s conceptions of the personal. Child Development , 66, 1438–1452. doi: 10.2307/1131656
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1131656 [Google Scholar]
  48. Perez-Granados, D.R
    (2002) Normative scripts for object labeling during a play activity: Mother-child and sibling conversations in Mexican-descent families. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences , 24, 164–190. doi: 10.1177/0739986302024002004
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0739986302024002004 [Google Scholar]
  49. Rakoczy, H. , Tomasello, M. , & Striano, T
    (2005) On tools and toys: How children learn to act on and pretend with ‘virgin’ objects. Developmental Science , 8(1), 57–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2005.00393.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00393.x [Google Scholar]
  50. Rogoff, B
    (2003)  The Cultural Nature of Human Development . Oxford: OUP.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Rogoff, B. , Paradise, R. , Mejía Arauz, R. , Correa-Chávez, M. , & Angelillo, C
    (2003) Firsthand learning through intent participation. Annual Review of Psychology , 54, 175–203. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145118
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145118 [Google Scholar]
  52. Sabbagh, M. , & Baldwin, D
    (2001) Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers’ theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development , 72, 1054–1070. doi: 10.1111/1467‑8624.00334
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00334 [Google Scholar]
  53. Sabbagh, M. , & Henderson, A
    (2007) How an appreciation of conventionality shapes early word learning. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development , 115, 28–38.
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Sabbagh, M.A. , & Shafman, D
    (2009) How children block learning from ignorant speakers. Cognition , 112, 415–422. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.005 [Google Scholar]
  55. Saffran, J.R. , Aslin, R.N. , & Newport, E.L
    (1996) Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science , 274, 1926–1928. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5294.1926
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.274.5294.1926 [Google Scholar]
  56. Schieffelin, B. & Ochs, E
    (1986)  Language Socialization across Cultures . Cambridge: CUP.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Siegel, D.R
    (2010)  Parent-Child Interactions with Artifacts in Everyday Activities . Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Siegel, D.R. , & Callanan, M.A
    (2007) Artifacts as conventional objects. Journal of Cognition and Development , 8, 182–203. doi: 10.1080/15248370701202414
    https://doi.org/10.1080/15248370701202414 [Google Scholar]
  59. Silva, K
    (2011)  Teaching Children through “Little Dramas”: Opinions about Instructional Ribbing from Mexican-heritage and European-American Mothers . Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Tennie, C. , Call, J. , & Tomasello, M
    (2006) Push or pull: Imitation vs. emulation in great apes and human children. Ethology , 112, 1159–1169. doi: 10.1111/j.1439‑0310.2006.01269.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01269.x [Google Scholar]
  61. Tomasello, M
    (1999)  The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Turiel, E
    (1983)  The Development of Social Knowledge: Morality and Convention . Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]

References

  1. Akhtar, N. , & Menjivar, J
    (2012) Cognitive and linguistic correlates of early exposure to more than one language. In J.B. Benson (Ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior , Vol.42. Oxford: Elsevier.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Bialystok, E. , Craik, F.I.M. , & Luk, G
    (2012) Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 16, 240–250. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001 [Google Scholar]
  3. Brennan, S.E. , & Clark, H.H
    (1996) Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition , 22, 1482–1493. doi: 10.1037/0278‑7393.22.6.1482
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1482 [Google Scholar]
  4. Buresh, J. & Woodward, A
    (2007) Infants track action goals within and across agents. Cognition , 104, 287–314. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.07.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.07.001 [Google Scholar]
  5. Callanan, M
    (2000) Les noms d’objet dans les conversations entre parents et jeune enfants (Names for objects in conversations between young children and their parents). Psychologie Française , 45, 187–192.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Callanan, M. , Akhtar, N. , & Sussman, L
    (2012)  Children Learning Words from Ostensive and Directive Speech . Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Santa Cruz.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Callanan, M. , & Jipson, J.L
    (2001) Explanatory conversations and young children’s developing scientific literacy. In K. Crowley , C.D. Schuun , & T. Okada (Eds.), Designing for Science: Implications from Everyday, Classroom, and Professional Settings (pp.21–49). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Callanan, M. , Rigney, J. , Nolan-Reyes, C. , & Solis, G
    (2012) Beyond pedagogy: How children’s knowledge develops in the context of everyday parent-child conversations. In A. Pinkham , T. Kaefer , & S. Neuman . (Eds.), Knowledge Development in Early Childhood: How Young Children Build Knowledge and Why it Matters . New York, NY: Guilford.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Callanan, M. , & Sabbagh, M
    (2004) Multiple labels for objects in conversations with young children: Parents’ language and children’s developing expectations about word meanings. Developmental Psychology, 40, 746–763. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.40.5.746
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.40.5.746 [Google Scholar]
  10. Callanan, M.A. , Siegel, D. , & Luce, M.R
    (2007) Conventionality in family conversations about everyday objects. In M. Sabbagh & C. Kalish (Eds.), New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development (pp.83–97). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Cimpian, A. , & Scott, R
    (2012) Children expect generic knowledge to be widely shared. Cognition , 123, 419–433. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.003 [Google Scholar]
  12. Clark, E.V
    (1992) Conventionality and contrast: Pragmatic principles with lexical consequences. In A. Lehrer & E.F. Kittay (Eds.), Frames, Fields, and Contrasts (pp.171–188). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. (1993)  The Lexicon in Acquisition . Cambridge: CUP. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511554377
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511554377 [Google Scholar]
  14. (2007) Conventionality and contrast in language and language acquisition. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development , 115, 11–23. doi: 10.1002/cd.179
    https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.179 [Google Scholar]
  15. Csibra, G. , & Gergely, G
    (2009) Natural pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Science , 13(4), 148–153. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.005 [Google Scholar]
  16. Diesendruck, G
    (2005) The principles of conventionality and contrast in word learning: An empirical examination. Developmental Psychology , 41, 451–463. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.41.3.451
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.41.3.451 [Google Scholar]
  17. Diesendruck, G. , & Markson, L
    (2001) Children’s avoidance of lexical overlap: A pragmatic account. Developmental Psychology , 37, 630–641. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.37.5.630
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.630 [Google Scholar]
  18. (2011) Children’s assumption of the conventionality of culture. Child Development Perspectives , 5, 189–195. doi: 10.1111/j.1750‑8606.2010.00156.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00156.x [Google Scholar]
  19. Gauvain, M
    (2001)  The Social Context of Cognitive Development . New York, NY: Guilford.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Gelman, S
    (2003)  The Essentialist Child . Oxford: OUP. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195154061.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195154061.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  21. (2009) Learning from others: Children’s construction of concepts. Annual Review of Psychology , 60, 115–140. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093659
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093659 [Google Scholar]
  22. Gelman, S. , Coley, J. , Rosengren, K. , Hartman, E. , & Pappas, A
    (1998) Beyond labeling: The role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly structured categories. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , 63 (Serial No. 253). doi: 10.2307/1166211
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1166211 [Google Scholar]
  23. German, T.P. , & Johnson, S.C
    (2002) Function and the origins of the design. Journal of Cognition and Development , 3, 279–300. doi: 10.1207/S15327647JCD0303_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0303_2 [Google Scholar]
  24. Grusec, J.E. , & Davidov, M
    (2010) Integrating different perspectives on socialization theory and research: A domain-specific approach. Child Development , 81, 687–709. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2010.01426.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01426.x [Google Scholar]
  25. Harris, P.L. , & Koenig, M.A
    (2006) Trust in testimony: How children learn about science and religion. Child Development , 77, 505–524. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2006.00886.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00886.x [Google Scholar]
  26. Hart, B. , & Risley, T
    (1995)  Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children . Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Henderson, A. , & Graham, S.A
    (2005) Two-year-olds’ appreciation of the shared nature of novel object labels. Journal of Cognition and Development , 6, 381–402. doi: 10.1207/s15327647jcd0603_4
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0603_4 [Google Scholar]
  28. Henderson, A. , & Sabbagh, M
    (2010) Parents’ use of conventional and unconventional labels in conversations with their preschoolers. Journal of Child Language , 37, 793–816. doi: 10.1017/S0305000909990122
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000909990122 [Google Scholar]
  29. Horner, V. , & Whiten, A
    (2005) Causal knowledge and emulation/imitation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition , 8, 164–181. doi: 10.1007/s10071‑004‑0239‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004-0239-6 [Google Scholar]
  30. Huttenlocher, J. , Haight, W. , Bryk, A. , Seltzer, M. , & Lyons, T
    (1991) Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology , 27, 236–248. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.27.2.236
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236 [Google Scholar]
  31. Jaswal, V.K
    (2004) Don’t believe everything you hear: Preschoolers’ sensitivity to speaker intent in category induction. Child Development , 75, 1871–1885. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2004.00822.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00822.x [Google Scholar]
  32. Jaswal, V.K. , & Neely, L
    (2006) Adults don’t always know best: Preschoolers use past reliability over age when learning new words. Psychological Science , 9, 757–758. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.2006.01778.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01778.x [Google Scholar]
  33. Kalish, C.W. , & Cornelius, R
    (2007) What is to be done: Children’s ascriptions of conventional obligations. Child Development, 78 , 859–878. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2007.01037.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01037.x [Google Scholar]
  34. Kalish, C.W. , & Lawson, C.A
    (2008) Development of social category representations: Early appreciation of roles and deontic relations. Child Development , 79, 577–593. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2008.01144.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01144.x [Google Scholar]
  35. Kalish, C.W. , & Sabbagh, M.A
    (2007) Conventionality and cognitive development: Learning to think the right way. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development , 115, 1–10. doi: 10.1002/cd.178
    https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.178 [Google Scholar]
  36. Kelemen, D
    (1999) The scope of teleological thinking in preschool children. Cognition , 70, 241–272. doi: 10.1016/S0010‑0277(99)00010‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00010-4 [Google Scholar]
  37. Kellman, P.J. , Massey, C.M. , & Son, J.Y
    (2010) Perceptual learning modules in mathematics: Enhancing students’ pattern recognition, structure extraction, and fluency. Topics in Cognitive Science , 2, 285–305. doi: 10.1111/j.1756‑8765.2009.01053.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01053.x [Google Scholar]
  38. Kidd, C. , White, K.S. , & Aslin, R.N
    (2011) Toddlers use speech disfluencies to predict speakers’ referential intentions. Developmental Science , 14, 925–934. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2011.01049.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01049.x [Google Scholar]
  39. Kinzler, K. , Corriveau, K. , & Harris, P
    (2011) Children’s selective trust in native-accented speakers. Developmental Science , 14, 106–111. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2011.01049.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01049.x [Google Scholar]
  40. Koenig, M.A. , & Harris, P.L
    (2005) Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development , 76, 1261–1277. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2005.00849.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00849.x [Google Scholar]
  41. Koenig, M.A. , & Woodward, A.L
    (2010) 24-month-olds’ sensitivity to the prior inaccuracy of the source: Possible mechanisms. Developmental Psychology , 46, 815–826. doi: 10.1037/a0019664
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019664 [Google Scholar]
  42. Lewis, D.K
    (1969)  Convention: A Philosophical Study . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Lieven, E.V.M
    (1994) Crosslinguistic and crosscultural aspects of language addressed to children. In C. Gallaway & B.J. Richards ( Eds .), Input and Interaction in Language Acquisition (pp. 56–73). Cambridge: CUP. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511620690.005
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620690.005 [Google Scholar]
  44. Luce, M. , & Callanan, M
    (2010) Parents’ object labeling: Possible links to conventionality of word meaning? First Language , 30, 270–286. doi: 10.1177/0142723710370543
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723710370543 [Google Scholar]
  45. Matthews, D. , Lieven, E. , & Tomasello, M
    (2010) What’s in a manner of speaking? Children’s sensitivity to partner-specific referential precedents. Developmental Psychology , 46, 749–760. doi: 10.1037/a0019657
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019657 [Google Scholar]
  46. Meltzoff, A
    (1995) Understanding the intention of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology , 31, 838–850. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.31.5.838
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838 [Google Scholar]
  47. Nucci, L. & Weber, E.K
    (1995) Social interactions in the home and the development of young children’s conceptions of the personal. Child Development , 66, 1438–1452. doi: 10.2307/1131656
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1131656 [Google Scholar]
  48. Perez-Granados, D.R
    (2002) Normative scripts for object labeling during a play activity: Mother-child and sibling conversations in Mexican-descent families. Hispanic Journal of Behavioral Sciences , 24, 164–190. doi: 10.1177/0739986302024002004
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0739986302024002004 [Google Scholar]
  49. Rakoczy, H. , Tomasello, M. , & Striano, T
    (2005) On tools and toys: How children learn to act on and pretend with ‘virgin’ objects. Developmental Science , 8(1), 57–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2005.00393.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00393.x [Google Scholar]
  50. Rogoff, B
    (2003)  The Cultural Nature of Human Development . Oxford: OUP.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Rogoff, B. , Paradise, R. , Mejía Arauz, R. , Correa-Chávez, M. , & Angelillo, C
    (2003) Firsthand learning through intent participation. Annual Review of Psychology , 54, 175–203. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145118
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145118 [Google Scholar]
  52. Sabbagh, M. , & Baldwin, D
    (2001) Learning words from knowledgeable versus ignorant speakers: Links between preschoolers’ theory of mind and semantic development. Child Development , 72, 1054–1070. doi: 10.1111/1467‑8624.00334
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00334 [Google Scholar]
  53. Sabbagh, M. , & Henderson, A
    (2007) How an appreciation of conventionality shapes early word learning. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development , 115, 28–38.
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Sabbagh, M.A. , & Shafman, D
    (2009) How children block learning from ignorant speakers. Cognition , 112, 415–422. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.005 [Google Scholar]
  55. Saffran, J.R. , Aslin, R.N. , & Newport, E.L
    (1996) Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science , 274, 1926–1928. doi: 10.1126/science.274.5294.1926
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.274.5294.1926 [Google Scholar]
  56. Schieffelin, B. & Ochs, E
    (1986)  Language Socialization across Cultures . Cambridge: CUP.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Siegel, D.R
    (2010)  Parent-Child Interactions with Artifacts in Everyday Activities . Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Siegel, D.R. , & Callanan, M.A
    (2007) Artifacts as conventional objects. Journal of Cognition and Development , 8, 182–203. doi: 10.1080/15248370701202414
    https://doi.org/10.1080/15248370701202414 [Google Scholar]
  59. Silva, K
    (2011)  Teaching Children through “Little Dramas”: Opinions about Instructional Ribbing from Mexican-heritage and European-American Mothers . Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Cruz.
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Tennie, C. , Call, J. , & Tomasello, M
    (2006) Push or pull: Imitation vs. emulation in great apes and human children. Ethology , 112, 1159–1169. doi: 10.1111/j.1439‑0310.2006.01269.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01269.x [Google Scholar]
  61. Tomasello, M
    (1999)  The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Turiel, E
    (1983)  The Development of Social Knowledge: Morality and Convention . Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/books/9789027270443-tilar.10.08cal
dcterms_subject,pub_keyword
-contentType:Journal
10
5
Chapter
content/books/9789027270443
Book
false
Loading
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error