Communication without language

MyBook is a cheap paperback edition of the original book and will be sold at uniform, low price.

Buy this article

Price: $35.00+Taxes
Add to favourites

How great apes may cover crucial advantages of language without creating a system of symbolic communication

The full text of this article is not currently available.

Data & Media loading...


Full text loading...


Arnold, Kate & Klaus Zuberbühler
(2006a) The alarm-calling system of adult male putty-nosed monkeys (Cercopithecus nictitans martini). Animal Behaviour, 72, 643–653. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.11.017
(2006b) Language evolution: Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature, 441, 303. doi: 10.1038/441303a
Bard, Kim
(1992) Intentional behavior and intentional communication in young free-ranging orangutans. Child Development, 63, 1186–1197. doi: 10.2307/1131526
Boesch, Christophe
(1991a) Symbolic communication in wild chimpanzees?Human Evolution, 6 (1), 81–90. doi: 10.1007/BF02435610
(1991b) Teaching among wild chimpanzees. Animal Behaviour, 41, 530–532. doi: 10.1016/S0003‑3472(05)80857‑7
(1995) Innovation in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). International Journal of Primatology, 16 (1), 1–16. doi: 10.1007/BF02700150
(1996) Three approaches for assessing chimpanzee culture. In Anne E. Russon , Kim A. Bard , & Sue T. Parker (Eds.), Reaching into thought: The minds of the great apes (pp.404–429). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
(2003) Is culture a golden barrier between human and chimpanzee?Evolutionary Anthropology, 12, 82–91. doi: 10.1002/evan.10106
(2012a) From material to symbolic cultures: Culture in primates. In Jaan Valsiner (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of culture and psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
(2012b) Wild cultures. A comparison between chimpanzee and human cultures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9781139178532
Boesch, Christophe & Hedwige Boesch-Achermann
(1984) Mental map in wild chimpanzees: An analysis of hammer transports for nut cracking. Primates, 25, 160–170. doi: 10.1007/BF02382388
Byrne, Richard W
(1993) A formal notation to aid analysis of complex behaviour – Understanding the tactical deception of primates. Behaviour, 127, 231–246. doi: 10.1163/156853993X00038
(2003) Tracing the evolutionary path of cognition: Tactical deception in primates. In Martin Brüne , Hedda Ribbert , & Wulf Schiefenhövel (Eds.), The social brain: Evolution and pathology (pp.43–609). Chichester, England: John Wiley. doi: 10.1002/0470867221.ch3
(2010) Deception: Competition by misleading behavior. In Michael D. Breed & Janice Moore (Eds.), Encyclopaedia of animal behavior (Vol. 1, pp.461–465). Oxford: Academic Press. doi: 10.1016/B978‑0‑08‑045337‑8.00097‑8
Byrne, Richard W. & Andrew Whiten
(1990) Tactical deception in primates: The 1990 database. Primate report, 27, 1101.
(1992) Cognitive evolution in primates: Evidence from Tactical Deception. Man, 27 (3), 609–627. doi: 10.2307/2803931
Call, Josep
(2011) How artificial communication affects the communication and cognition of the great apes. Mind & Language, 26, 1–20. doi: 10.1111/j.1468‑0017.2010.01408.x
Call, Josep , Brian Agnetta , & Michael Tomasello
(2000) Cues that chimpanzees do and do not use to find hidden objects. Animal Cognition, 3, 23–34. doi: 10.1007/s100710050047
Call, Josep & Michael Tomasello
(2008) Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? 30 years later. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12 (5), 187–192. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.010
Cheney, Dorothy L. & Robert M. Seyfarth
(2007) Baboon metaphysics. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. doi: 10.7208/chicago/9780226102429.001.0001
Crockford, Catherine & Christophe Boesch
(2003) Context-specific calls in wild chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus: analysis of barks. Animal Behaviour, 66, 115–125. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2003.2166
(2005) Call combinations in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 142, 397–421. doi: 10.1163/1568539054012047
Crockford, Catherine , Ilka Herbinger , Linda Vigilant , & Christophe Boesch
(2004) Wild chimpanzees produce group-specific calls: A case for vocal learning?Ethology, 110, 221–243. doi: 10.1111/j.1439‑0310.2004.00968.x
Crockford, Catherine , Roman M. Wittig , Roger Mundry , & Klaus Zuberbühler
(2012) Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22, 142–146. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.053
Deacon, Terrence W
(1997) The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: W.W. Norton & Company.
Dennett, Daniel C
(1987) The intentional stance. Cambridge, MA: MIT/Bradford Books.
Enfield, Nick J
(2010) Human sociality at the heart of language [Inaugural lecture]. Nijmegen: Radboud University Nijmegen.
Fitch, W. Tecumseh
(2005) The evolution of language: A comparative review. Biology and Philosophy, 20, 193–230. doi: 10.1007/s10539‑005‑5597‑1
Genty, Emilie , Thomas Breuer , Catherine Hobaiter , & Richard W. Byrne
(2009) Gestural communication of the gorilla (Gorilla gorilla): Repertoire, intentionality and possible origins. Animal Cognition, 12, 527–546. doi: 10.1007/s10071‑009‑0213‑4
Goodall, Jane
(1986) The chimpanzees of Gombe, patterns of behavior. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.
Greenfield, Patricia M. & E. Sue Savage-Rumbaugh
(1990) Grammatical combination in Pan paniscus: Processes of learning and invention in the evolution and development of language. In Sue T. Parker & Kathleen R. Gibson (Eds.), ‘Language’ and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives (pp.540–578). New York: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511665486.022
Grice, Herbert P
(1957) Meaning. The Philosophical Review, 66 (3), 377–388. doi: 10.2307/2182440
Griffin, Donald R
(1984) Animal thinking. Cambridge, MA & London: Harvard University Press.
Hammerschmidt, Kurt & Julia Fischer
(2008) Constraints in primate vocal production. In Ulrike Griebel & D. Kimbrough Oller (Eds.), The evolution of communicative creativity: From fixed signals to contextual flexibility (pp.93–119). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hare, Brian , Josep Call , Brian Agnetta , & Michael Tomasello
(2000) Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see. Animal Behaviour, 59, 771–785. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1377
Hare, Brian , Josep Call , & Michael Tomasello
(2001) Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know?Animal Behaviour, 61, 139–151. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1518
Hauser, Marc D
(1990) Do chimpanzee copulatory calls incite male–male competition?Animal Behaviour, 39, 596–597. doi: 10.1016/S0003‑3472(05)80427‑0
Heyes, Cecilia M
(1994) Social learning in animals: Categories and mechanisms. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society, 69, 207–231. doi: 10.1111/j.1469‑185X.1994.tb01506.x
(2012) What’s social about social learning?Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126 (2), 193–202. doi: 10.1037/a0025180
Hobaiter, Catherine & Richard W. Byrne
(2011) The gestural repertoire of the wild chimpanzee. Animal Cognition, 14 (5), 745–767. doi: 10.1007/s10071‑011‑0409‑2
(2014) The meanings of chimpanzee gestures. Current Biology, 24, 1596–1600. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.05.066
Hockett, Charles F
(1960) The origin of speech. Scientific American, 203 (3), 89–96. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0960‑88
Hoppitt, William J.E. & Kevin N. Laland
(2008) Social processes influencing learning in animals: A review of the evidence. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 38, 105–165. doi: 10.1016/S0065‑3454(08)00003‑X
(2013) Social learning: An introduction to mechanisms, methods and models. Princeton: Princeton University Press. doi: 10.1515/9781400846504
Jaeggi, Adrain V. , Maria A. van Noordwijk , & Carel P. van Schaik
(2008) Begging for information: mother–offspring food sharing among wild Bornean orangutans. American Journal of Primatology, 70, 533–541. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20525
Janmaat, Karline R.L. , Simone Dagui Ban , & Christophe Boesch
(2013) Chimpanzees use long-term spatial memory to monitor large fruit trees and remember feeding experiences across seasons. Animal Behaviour, 86 (6), 1183–1205. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.09.021
Kita, Sotaro
(2003) Pointing: A foundational building block of human communication. In Sotaro Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp.93–119). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kummer, Hans
(1968) Social organization of Hamadryas baboons: A field study. (Bibliotheca Primatologica, 6). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
(1971) Primate societies: Group techniques of ecological adaptation. Chicago & New York: Aldine-Atherton.
Laland, Kevin N
(2004) Social learning strategies. Learning & Behavior, 32 (1), 4–14. doi: 10.3758/BF03196002
Liebal, Katja , Simone Pika , & Michael Tomasello
(2006) Gestural communication of orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus). Gesture, 6, 1–38. doi: 10.1075/gest.6.1.02lie
Liebal, Katja & Josep Call
(2012) The origins of non-human primates’ manual gestures. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367 (1585), 118–128. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0044
Lyn, Heidi , Jamie L. Russell , David A. Leavens , Kim A. Bard , Sarah T. Boysen , Jennifer A. Schaeffer , & William D. Hopkins
(2014) Apes communicate about absent and displaced objects: methodology matters. Animal Cognition, 17 (1), 85–94. doi: 10.1007/s10071‑013‑0640‑0
MacKinnon, John R
(1974) The behaviour and ecology of wild orang-utans (Pongo pygmaeus). Animal Behaviour, 22, 3–74. doi: 10.1016/S0003‑3472(74)80054‑0
Menzel, Emil W. Jr
(1971) Communication about the environment in a group of young chimpanzees. Folia Primatologica, 15, 220–232. doi: 10.1159/000155381
Miles, H. Lyn
(1990) The cognitive foundations for reference in a signing orangutan. In Sue T. Parker & Kathleen R. Gibson (Eds.), Language and intelligence in monkeys and apes: Comparative developmental perspectives (pp.511–539). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511665486.021
Milton, Katharine
(1981) Distribution patterns of tropical plant foods as an evolutionary stimulus to primate mental development. American Anthropologist, 83, 534–548. doi: 10.1525/aa.1981.83.3.02a00020
Moore, Richard
(forthcoming). Enacting and understanding communicative intent.
Mulcahy, Nicholas J. & Josep Call
(2006) Apes save tools for future use. Science, 312, 1038–1040. doi: 10.1126/science.1125456
Nishida, Toshisada
(1980) The leaf-clipping display: A newly-discovered expressive gesture in wild chimpanzees. Journal of Human Evolution, 9, 117–128. doi: 10.1016/0047‑2484(80)90068‑8
(1987) Local traditions and cultural transmission. In Barbara B. Smuts , Dorothy L. Cheney , Robert M. Seyfarth , Richard W. Wrangham , & Thomas T. Struhsaker (Eds.), Primate societies (pp.462–474). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
(1997) Sexual behavior of adult male chimpanzees of the Mahale Mountains National Park, Tanzania. Primates, 38, 379–398. doi: 10.1007/BF02381879
Normand, Emanuelle , Simone Dagui Ban , & Christophe Boesch
(2009) Forest chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) remember the location of numerous fruit trees. Animal Cognition, 12 (6), 797–807. doi: 10.1007/s10071‑009‑0239‑7
Osvath, Mathias
(2009) Spontaneous planning for future stone throwing by a male chimpanzee. Current Biology, 19, R190–R191. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.01.010
Osvath, Mathias & Helena Osvath
(2008) Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: Self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use. Animal Cognition, 11, 661–674. doi: 10.1007/s10071‑008‑0157‑0
Ouattara, Karim , Alban Lemasson , & Klaus Zuberbühler
(2009a) Campbell’s monkeys use affixation to alter call meaning. PLoS One, 4, e7808. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0007808
(2009b) Campbell’s monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106 (51), 22026–22031. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0908118106
Owren, Michael J. , R. Toby Amoss , & Drew Rendall
(2010) Two organizing principles of vocal production: Implications for nonhuman and human primates. American Journal of Primatology, 73, 530–544. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20913
Patterson, Francine G
(1978) The gestures of a gorilla: Language acquisition in another Pongid. Brain and Language, 5, 72–97. doi: 10.1016/0093‑934X(78)90008‑1
Pika, Simone , Katja Liebal , & Michael Tomasello
(2003) Gestural communication in young gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): Gestural repertoire, learning, and use. American Journal of Primatology, 60, 95–111. doi: 10.1002/ajp.10097
(2005) Gestural communication in subadult bonobos (Pan paniscus): Repertoire and use. American Journal of Primatology, 65, 39–61. doi: 10.1002/ajp.20096
Pika, Simone , Katja Liebal , Josep Call , & Michael Tomasello
(2005) The gestural communication of apes. In Katja Liebal , Cornelia Müller , & Simone Pika (Eds.), Gestural communication in nonhuman and human primates (pp.37–51). Gesture, 5 (1/2). doi: 10.1075/gest.5.1‑2.05pik
Pika, Simone & John C. Mitani
(2008) The directed scratch: Evidence for a referential gesture in chimpanzees?In Rudolph Botha & Chris Knight (Eds.), The prehistory of language (pp.166–180). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Russon, Anne E. & David R. Begun
(2004) Evolutionary origins of great ape intelligence: An integrated view. In Anne E. Russon & David R. Begun (Eds.), The evolution of thought (pp.353–368). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511542299.023
Saussure, Ferdinand de
(2003) Cours de linguistique générale. Charles Bally & Albert Séchehaye (Eds.). Paris: Payot & Rivages (original work published 1916).
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. Sue , Kelly McDonald , Rose A. Sevcik , William D. Hopkins , & Elizabeth Rubert
(1986) Spontaneous symbol acquisition and communicative use by pygmy chimpanzees (Pan paniscus). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115, 211–235. doi: 10.1037/0096‑3445.115.3.211
Schel, Anne M. , Simon W. Townsend , Zarin Machanda , Klaus Zuberbühler , & Katie E. Slocombe
(2013) Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLoS ONE, 01/2013; 8 (10), e76674. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076674
Schmelz, Martin , Josep Call , & Michael Tomasello
(2011) Chimpanzees know that others make inferences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 3077–3079. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1000469108
Scott-Phillips, Thomas C
(2015) Meaning in animal and human communication. Animal Cognition, 18 (3), 801–805. doi: 10.1007/s10071‑015‑0845‑5
Seyfarth, Robert M. & Dorothy L. Cheney
(2010) Production, usage, and comprehension in animal vocalizations. Brain & Language, 115, 92–100. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.10.003
Slocombe, Katie E. & Klaus Zuberbühler
(2007) Chimpanzees modify recruitment screams as a function of audience composition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104 (43), 17228–17233. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0706741104
Slocombe, Katie E. , Tanja Kaller , Laurel Turman , Simon W. Townsend , Sarah Papworth , Paul Squibbs , & Klaus Zuberbühler
(2010) Production of food-associated calls in wild male chimpanzees is dependent on the composition of the audience. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 64 (12), 1959–1966. doi: 10.1007/s00265‑010‑1006‑0
Sugiyama, Yukimaru
(1981) Observations on the population dynamics and behavior of wild chimpanzees of Bossou, Guinea, in 1979–1980. Primates, 22, 435–444. doi: 10.1007/BF02381236
Tomasello, Michael , Deborah Gust , & G. Thomas Frost
(1989) A longitudinal investigation of gestural communication in young chimpanzees. Primates, 30, 35–50. doi: 10.1007/BF02381209
Tomasello, Michael , Josep Call , & Brian Hare
(1998) Five primate species follow the visual gaze of conspecifics. Animal Behaviour, 55, 1063–1069. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1997.0636
Tomasello, Michael , Brian Hare , & Bryan Agnetta
(1999) Chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, follow gaze direction geometrically. Animal Behaviour, 58, 769–777. doi: 10.1006/anbe.1999.1192
Tomasello, Michael , Brian Hare , & Tara Fogleman
(2001) The ontogeny of gaze following in chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes, and rhesus macaques, Macaca mulatta . Animal Behaviour, 61, 335–343. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1598
Tomasello, Michael , Josep Call , & Brian Hare
(2003) Chimpanzees understand psychological states – the question is which ones and to what extent. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 153–156. doi: 10.1016/S1364‑6613(03)00035‑4
van Schaik, Carel P. , Laura Damerius , & Karin Isler
(2013) Wild orangutan males plan and communicate their travel direction one day in advance. PLoS ONE, 8 (9), e74896. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0074896
Watts, David & John C. Mitani
(2001) Boundary patrols and intergroup encounters in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 299–327. doi: 10.1163/15685390152032488
Whiten, Andrew , Jane Goodall , William C. McGrew , Toshisada Nishida , Vernon Reynolds , Yukimaru Sugiyama , Caroline E.G. Tutin , Richard W. Wrangham , & Christophe Boesch
(1999) Cultures in chimpanzees. Nature, 399, 682–685. doi: 10.1038/21415
(2001) Charting cultural variation in chimpanzees. Behaviour, 138, 1481–1516. doi: 10.1163/156853901317367717
Wich, Serge A. , Michael Krützen , Adriano R. Lameira , Alexander Nater , Natasha Arora , Meredith L. Bastian , Ellen Meulman , Helen C. Morrogh-Bernard , S. Suci Utami Atmoko , Joko Pamungkas , Dyah Perwitasari-Farajallah , Madelaine E. Hardus , Maria van Noordwijk , & Carel P. van Schaik
(2012) Call cultures in orang-utans?PLoS one, 7, e36180. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0036180
Wilson, Michael L. , Marc D. Hauser , & Richard W. Wrangham
(2007) Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) modify grouping and vocal behaviour in response to location-specific risk. Behaviour, 144, 1621–1653. doi: 10.1163/156853907782512137
Zuberbühler, Klaus
(2002) A syntactic rule in forest monkey communication. Animal Behaviour, 63, 293–299. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1914
Zuberbühler, Klaus & Karline R.L. Janmaat
(2010) Foraging cognition in non-human primates. In Michael L. Platt & Asif A. Ghazanfar (Eds.), Primate neuroethology (pp.64–83). Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195326598.003.0004
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address