1887
Volume 18, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1572-0373
  • E-ISSN: 1572-0381
GBP
Buy:£15.00 + Taxes
  • Combinatory rules and chunk structure in male Mueller’s gibbon songs

  • Author(s): Yoichi Inoue 1 , Waidi Sinun 2 , Shigeto Yosida 3  and Kazuo Okanoya 4
  • View Affiliations Hide Affiliations
    Affiliations:
    1 The School of Arts and Sciences, The University of Tokyo Laboratory for Biolinguistics, RIKEN Brain Science Institute
    2 Research and Development Division Yayasan Sabah Group
    3 Laboratory for Biolinguistics, RIKEN Brain Science Institute
    4 Cognition and Behavior Joint Research Laboratory, RIKEN Brain Science Institute Department of Life Sciences, Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, The University of Tokyo
  • Source: Interaction Studies, Volume 18, Issue 1, Jan 2017, p. 1 - 25
  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1075/is.18.1.01ino
    • Version of Record published : 28 Jul 2017

Abstract

Understanding whether the long and elaborate songs of male gibbons ( have syntax and hierarchical structures (chunks) is an interesting question in the evolution of language, because gibbons are near humans in the phylogenetic tree and a hierarchically organized syntax is considered to be a basic component of human language. We conducted field research at Danum Valley Conservation Area in northern Borneo to test the hypothesis that gibbon songs have syntax and chunks. We followed one Mueller’s gibbon group for 1 week in the dry and rainy seasons every year from 2001 to 2009, collecting vocal and behavioral data. Results show that songs emitted by the studied male gibbon were governed by combinatory rules. Some context-dependent songs had different combinatory rules, although they overlapped with the songs whose contexts were uncertain. The male Mueller’s songs had characteristics that suggest existence of chunk structure. These results provided an important perspective in the study of language origin.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/is.18.1.01ino
2017-07-28
2024-04-18
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Arnold, K. , & Zuberbühler, K
    (2006) Language evolution: Semantic combinations in primate calls. Nature, 441, 303. doi: 10.1038/441303a
    https://doi.org/10.1038/441303a [Google Scholar]
  2. (2012) Call combinations in monkeys: Compositional or idiomatic expressions?Brain and Language, 120, 303–309. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.10.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.10.001 [Google Scholar]
  3. Beer, C
    (1976) Some complexities in the communication behavior of gulls. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 280, 413–432. doi: 10.1111/j.1749‑6632.1976.tb25505.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1976.tb25505.x [Google Scholar]
  4. Berwick, R. C. , Okanoya, K. , Beckers, G.J. L. , & Bolhuis, J. J
    (2011) Songs to syntax: The linguistics of birdsong. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 113–121. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.01.002 [Google Scholar]
  5. Brockelman, W. Y. , & Schilling, D
    (1984) Inheritance of stereotyped gibbon calls. Nature, 312, 634–636. doi: 10.1038/312634a0
    https://doi.org/10.1038/312634a0 [Google Scholar]
  6. Chew, L
    (1981) Geographic and individual variation in the morphology and sequential organization of the song of the savannah sparrow (Passerculus sandwichensis). Canadian Journal of Zoology, 59, 702–713. doi: 10.1139/z81‑099
    https://doi.org/10.1139/z81-099 [Google Scholar]
  7. Clarke, E. , Reichard, U. H. , & Zuberbühler, K
    (2006) The syntax and meaning of wild gibbon songs. PLoS One, 1, e73. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0000073
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000073 [Google Scholar]
  8. Cleveland, J. , & Snowdon, C. T
    (1982) The complex vocal repertoire of the adult cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus oedipus). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie., 58, 231–270. doi: 10.1111/j.1439‑0310.1982.tb00320.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1982.tb00320.x [Google Scholar]
  9. Crockford, C. , & Boesch, C
    (2005) Call combinations in wild chimpanzees. Behaviour, 142, 397–421. doi: 10.1163/1568539054012047
    https://doi.org/10.1163/1568539054012047 [Google Scholar]
  10. Darwin, C
    (1859) On the origin of species. London: John Murray.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Geissmann, T
    (1984) Inheritance of song parameters in the gibbon song, analyzed in 2 hybrid gibbons (Hylobates pileatus × H. lar). Folia Primatologica, 42, 216–235. doi: 10.1159/000156165
    https://doi.org/10.1159/000156165 [Google Scholar]
  12. (1999) Duet songs of the siamang, Hylobates syndactylus: II, Testing the pair-bonding hypothesis during a partner exchange. Behaviour, 136, 1005–1039. doi: 10.1163/156853999501694
    https://doi.org/10.1163/156853999501694 [Google Scholar]
  13. Geissmann, T. , & Orgeldinger, M
    (2000) The relationship between duet songs and pair bonds in siamangs, Hylobates syndactylus. Animal Behaviour, 60, 805–809. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2000.1540
    https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2000.1540 [Google Scholar]
  14. Geissmann, T. , Bohlen-Eyring, S. , & Heuck, A
    (2005) The male song of the Javan silvery gibbon (Hylobates moloch). Contributions to Zoology, 74, 1–25.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Groves, C. P
    (1972) Systematics and phylogeny of gibbons. In D. M. Rumbaugh (Eds.), Gibbon and siamang, vol. 1 (pp. 1–89). Basel: Karger.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Hailman, J. P. , Ficken, M. S. , & Ficken, R. W
    (1985) The “chick-a-dee” calls of Parus atricapillus: a recombinant system of animal communication compared with written English. Semiotica, 56, 191–224. doi: 10.1515/semi.1985.56.3‑4.191
    https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1985.56.3-4.191 [Google Scholar]
  17. Haimoff, E. H
    (1984) Acoustic and organizational features of gibbon songs. In: H. Preuschoft , D. J. Chivers , W. Y. Brockelman , & N. Creel (Eds.), The lesser apes. Evolutionary and behavioural biology (pp. 333–353). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. (1985) The organization of song in Mueller’s gibbon (Hylobates muelleri). International Journal of Primatology, 6, 173–192. doi: 10.1007/BF02693652
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02693652 [Google Scholar]
  19. Hope, A.C. A
    (1968) A simplified Monte Carlo significance test procedure. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society . Series B, 30, 582–598.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Hurford, J. R
    (2011) The origins of grammar: language in the light of evolution II (Vol. 2). Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Inoue, Y. , Waidi, S. , Yosida, S. , & Okanoya, K
    (2013) Intergroup and intragroup antiphonal songs in wild male Mueller’s gibbons (Hylobates muelleri). Interaction Studies, 14, 24–43. doi: 10.1075/is.14.1.03ino
    https://doi.org/10.1075/is.14.1.03ino [Google Scholar]
  22. Jackendoff, R
    (1999) Possible stages in the evolution of the language capacity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 272–279. doi: 10.1016/S1364‑6613(99)01333‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01333-9 [Google Scholar]
  23. Leighton, D. R
    (1987) Gibbons: Territoriality and monogamy. In B. B. Smuts , D. L. Cheney , R. M. Seyfarth , R. W. Wrangham , & T. T. Struhsaker (Eds.), Primate societies (pp. 135–145). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Manser, M. B. , Seyfarth, R. M. , & Cheney, D. L
    (2002) Suricate alarm calls signal predator class and urgency. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 55–57. doi: 10.1016/S1364‑6613(00)01840‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01840-4 [Google Scholar]
  25. Marshall, J. T. , & Marshall, E. R
    (1976) Gibbons and their territorial songs. Science, 193, 235–237. doi: 10.1126/science.193.4249.235
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.193.4249.235 [Google Scholar]
  26. Matsudaira, K. , Ishida, T. , Reichard, U. H. , & Malaivijitnond, S
    (2015) Male kin network and dispersal pattern of white-handed gibbons. The 31st Congress of the Primate Society of Japan .
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Meyer, P. E
    (2014) infotheo: Information-Theoretic Measures. R package version 1.2.0. URLhttps://CRAN.R-project.org/package=infotheo.
  28. Miller, G. A
    (1955) Note on the bias of information estimates. In H. Quastler (Eds.), Information theory in psychology; problems and methods II-B (pp. 95–100). Glencoe, IL: Free Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. (1956) The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits of our capacity for processing information. Psychology Review, 63, 81–97. doi: 10.1037/h0043158
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043158 [Google Scholar]
  30. Mitani, J. C
    (1985) Location-specific responses of gibbons (Hylobates muelleri) to male songs. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie., 70, 219–224. doi: 10.1111/j.1439‑0310.1985.tb00513.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1985.tb00513.x [Google Scholar]
  31. Mitani, J. C. , & Marler, P
    (1989) A phonological analysis of male gibbon singing behavior. Behaviour, 109, 20–45. doi: 10.1163/156853989X00141
    https://doi.org/10.1163/156853989X00141 [Google Scholar]
  32. Morton, E. S
    (1977) Occurrence and significance of motivation structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. American Naturalist, 111, 855–869. doi: 10.1086/283219
    https://doi.org/10.1086/283219 [Google Scholar]
  33. Okanoya, K. , & Merker, B
    (2007) Neural substrates for string-context mutual segmentation: A path to human language. In C. Lyon , L. Nehaniv , A. Cangelosi & (Eds.), Emergence of communication and language (pp. 421–434). Springer-Verlag. doi: 10.1007/978‑1‑84628‑779‑4_22
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-84628-779-4_22 [Google Scholar]
  34. Ouattara, K. , Lemasson, A. , & Zuberbuhler, K
    (2009) Campbell’s monkeys concatenate vocalizations into context-specific call sequences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United Staes of America, 106(51), 222026–22031.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. R. Core Team
    (2016) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. URLhttps://www.R-project.org/.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Reichard, U. , & Sommer, V
    (1997) Group encounters in wild gibbons (Hylobates lar): Agonism, affiliation, and the concept of infanticide. Behaviour, 134, 1135. doi: 10.1163/156853997X00106
    https://doi.org/10.1163/156853997X00106 [Google Scholar]
  37. Robinson, J. G
    (1979) An analysis of the organization of vocal communication in the titi monkey Callicebus moloch. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie., 49, 381–405. doi: 10.1111/j.1439‑0310.1979.tb00300.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1979.tb00300.x [Google Scholar]
  38. (1984) Syntactic structures in the vocalisations of wedge-capped capuchin monkeys, Cebus olivaceus. Behaviour, 90, 46–78. doi: 10.1163/156853984X00551
    https://doi.org/10.1163/156853984X00551 [Google Scholar]
  39. Simon, H. A
    (1974) How big is a chunk? By combining data from several experiments, a basic human memory unit can be identified and measured. Science, 183, 482–488. doi: 10.1126/science.183.4124.482
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.183.4124.482 [Google Scholar]
  40. Suzuki, R. , Buck, J. R. , & Tyack, P. L
    (2006) Information entropy of humpback whale songs. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119, 1849–1866. doi: 10.1121/1.2161827
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2161827 [Google Scholar]
  41. Templeton, C. N. , Green, E. , & Davis, K
    (2005) Allometry of alarm calls: Black-capped chickadees encode information about predator size. Science, 308, 1934–1937. doi: 10.1126/science.1108841
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1108841 [Google Scholar]
  42. Ten Cate, C. , & Slater, P.J. B
    (1991) Song learning in zebra finches: how are elements from two tutors integrated?Animal Behaviour, 42, 150–152. doi: 10.1016/S0003‑3472(05)80617‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(05)80617-7 [Google Scholar]
  43. Venables, W. N. , & Ripley, B. D
    (2002) Modern Applied Statistics with S. Fourth edition. Springer. doi: 10.1007/978‑0‑387‑21706‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-21706-2 [Google Scholar]
  44. Williams, H. , & Staples, K
    (1992) Syllable chunking in zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) song. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 106, 278–286. doi: 10.1037/0735‑7036.106.3.278
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.106.3.278 [Google Scholar]
  45. Zuberbühler, K
    (2002) A syntactic rule in forest monkey comunication. Animal Behaviour, 63, 293–299. doi: 10.1006/anbe.2001.1914
    https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1914 [Google Scholar]
http://instance.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1075/is.18.1.01ino
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/is.18.1.01ino
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): chunk; combinatory rule; Danum Valley Conservation Area; male song; Mueller’s gibbon

Most Cited

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error