Visit www.benjamins.com

Carrying, caring, and conversing

MyBook is a cheap paperback edition of the original book and will be sold at uniform, low price.

Buy this article

Price: £15.00+Taxes
Add to favourites

Constraints on the emergence of cooperation, conformity, and language

The full text of this article is not currently available.
/content/journals/10.1075/is.18.1.02hod
dcterms_subject,pub_keyword
6
3
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/is.18.1.02hod
Loading

Data & Media loading...

Loading

Full text loading...

References

Adornetti, I
(2015) Competition and cooperation in language evolution: A comparison between communication of apes and humans. In F. E’Errico , I. Poggi , J. Vinciarelli , & L. Vincze (Eds.), Conflict and multimodal communication: Social research and machine intelligence (pp. 91–101). New York: Springer.
Allen, J. M. , Worman, C. O. , Light, J. E. , & Reed, D. L
(2013) Parasitic lice help to fill in the gaps of early hominid history. In J. F. Brinkworth & K. Pechenkina (Eds.), Primates, pathogens and evolution (pp. 161–186). New York: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978‑1‑4614‑7181‑3_6
Amaral, L. Q
(2008) Mechanical analysis of infant carrying in hominoids. Naturwissenschaften, 95, 281–292. doi: 10.1007/s00114‑007‑0325‑0
Asch, S. E
(1956) Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs, 70, no. 9 (Whole No. 416). doi: 10.1037/h0093718
Berger, J. , & Heath, C
(2008) Who drives divergence? Identity signaling, outgroup similarity, and the abandonment of cultural tastes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 593–607. doi: 10.1037/0022‑3514.95.3.593
Boesch, C
(2005) Joint cooperative hunting among wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 692–693. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05230121
Bolhuis, J. J. , Tattersall, I. , Chomsky, N. , & Berwick, R. C
(2014) How could language have evolved?PLOS Biology, 12, 8, e1001934. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001934
Boyd, R. , Richerson, P. J. , & Henrich, J
(2011) The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 108, 10918–10925.
Bråten, S
(2000) Essays on dialogue in infant and adult. Bergen, Norway: Sigma.
(2004) Hominin infant decentration hypothesis: Mirror neurons system adapted to subserve mother-centered participation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 508–509. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0427011X
(2007) Participant perception of the speaker by conversation partners and infant learners: On the origins of (m)othercentred participation. Paper presented at theSymposium of the Distributed Language Group, Language dynamics and the phenomenology of individual experience, Grimstad, Norway.
(2009) The intersubjective mirror in infant learning and evolution of speech. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/aicr.76
Carvalho, S. , Biro, D. , Cunha, E. , Hockings, K. , McGrew, W. C. , Richmond, B. G. , & Matsuzawa, T
(2010) Chimpanzee carrying behaviour and the origins of human bipedality. Current Biology, 22(6), R180–R181. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.052
Cialdini, R. B. , & Goldstein, N. J
(2004) Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591–621. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015
Cialdini, R. , & Trost, M
(1998) Social influence: Social norms, conformity, and compliance. In D. Gilbert , S. Fiske , & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, 4th edition) (pp. 151–192). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Claidière, N. , & Whiten, A
(2012) Integrating the study of conformity and culture in human and nonhuman animals. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 126–145. doi: 10.1037/a0025868
Corriveau, K. H. , Fusaro, M. , & Harris, P. L
(2009) Going with the flow: Preschoolers prefer nondissenters as informants. Psychological Science, 20, 372–377. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.2009.02291.x
Corriveau, K. H. , & Harris, P. L
(2010) Preschoolers (sometimes) defer to the majority in making simple perceptual judgments. Developmental Psychology, 46, 437–445. doi: 10.1037/a0017553
Corriveau, K. H. , Kinzler, K. D. , & Harris, P. L
(2013) Accuracy trumps accent in children’s endorsement of object labels. Developmental Psychology, 49, 470–479. doi: 10.1037/a0030604
Clark, H. H
(1996) Using language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511620539
Cowley, S
(2004) Early hominins, utterance-activity, and niche construction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 509–510. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04290112
Crockford, C. , Wittig, R. M. , Mundry, R. , & Zuberbühler, K
(2012) Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.053
Crockford, C. , Wittig, R. M. , Langergraber, K. , Ziegler, T. E. , Zuberbühler, K. , & Deschner, T
(2013) Urinary oxytocin and social bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 280, 20122765. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2765
Duguid, S. , Wyman, E. , Bullinger, A. F. , Herfurth-Majstorovic, K. , & Tomasello, M
(2014), Coordination strategies of chimpanzees and human children in a Stag Hunt game. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 281, 20141973. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1973
Efferson, C. , Lalive, R. , Richerson, P. J. , McElreath, R. , & Lubell, M
(2008) Conformists and mavericks: the empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. Evolution of Human Behavior, 29, 56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.08.003
Efferson, C. , Richerson, P. J. , McElreath, R. , Lubell, M. , Edsten, E. , Waring, T. M. , Paciotti, B. , & Baum, W
(2007) Learning, productivity, and noise: an experimental study of cultural transmission on the Bolivian Altiplano. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 11–17. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.05.005
Enquist, M. , Eriksson, K. , & Ghirlanda, S
(2007) Critical social learning: A solution to Roger’s paradox of nonadaptive culture. American Anthropologist, 109, 727–734. doi: 10.1525/aa.2007.109.4.727
Eriksson, K. , & Coultas, J. C
(2009) Are people really conformist-biased? An empirical test and a new mathematical model. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 5–21. doi: 10.1556/JEP.7.2009.1.3
Eriksson, K. , Enquist, M. , & Ghirlanda, S
(2007) Critical points in current theory of conformist social learning. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 5, 67–87. doi: 10.1556/JEP.2007.1009
Eriksson, K. , & Strimling, P
(2009) Biases for acquiring information individually rather than socially. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 309–329. doi: 10.1556/JEP.7.2009.4.4
Falk, D
(2004) Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: Whence motherese?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 491–541. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04000111
(2009) Finding our tongues: Mothers, infants, and the origins of language. New York: Basic Books.
Francis, W. N
(1983) Dialectology. London: Longmans.
Fusaroli, R. , Bahrami, B. , Olsen, K. , Roepstorff, A. , Rees, G. , Frith, C. , & Tylén, K
(2012) Coming to terms: quantifying the benefits of linguistic coordination. Psychological Science, 23931–939. doi: 10.1177/0956797612436816
Haun, D.B. M. , Rekers, Y. , & Tomasello, M
(2014) Children conform to the behavior of peers; Other great apes stick with what they know. Psychological Science, 25, 2160–2167. doi: 10.1177/0956797614553235
Haun, D.B. M. , & Tomasello, M
(2011) Conformity to peer pressure in preschool children. Child Development, 82, 1759–1767. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2011.01666.x
Healey, P.G. T. , Purver, M. , & Howes, C
(2014) Divergence in dialogue. PLOS One, 9, 6, e98598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098598
Heft, H
(1983) Wayfinding as the perception of information over time. Population & Environment, 6, 133–150. doi: 10.1007/BF01258956
Heyes, C. M
(2011) Automatic imitation. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 463–483. doi: 10.1037/a0022288
Hodges, B. H
(2007a) Good prospects: Ecological and social perspectives on conforming, creating, and caring in conversation. Language Sciences, 29, 584–604. doi: 10.1016/j.langsci.2007.01.003
(2007b) Values define fields: The intentional dynamics of driving, carrying, leading, negotiating, and conversing. Ecological Psychology, 19, 153–178.
(2009) Ecological pragmatics: Values, dialogical arrays, complexity, and caring. Pragmatics & Cognition, 17, 628–652. doi: 10.1075/pc.17.3.08hod
(2014) Rethinking conformity and imitation: Divergence, convergence, and social understanding. Frontiers in Psychology: Cognitive Science, 5, 726.
(2017) Conformity and divergence in interactions, groups, and culture. In S. Harkins , K. Williams , & J. Burger (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social influence (pp.87–105). New York: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199859870.013.3
Hodges, B. H. , & Baron, R. M
(1992) Values as constraints on affordances: Perceiving and acting properly. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 22, 263–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1468‑5914.1992.tb00220.x
Hodges, B. H. , & Geyer, A
(2006) A nonconformist account of the Asch experiments: Values, pragmatics, and moral dilemmas. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 2–19. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_1
Hodges, B. H. , & Lindhiem, O
(2006) Carrying babies and groceries: The effect of moral and social weight on caring. Ecological Psychology, 18, 93–111. doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco1802_2
Hodges, B. H. , Meagher, B. R. , Norton, D. J. , McBain, R. , & Sroubek, A
(2014) Speaking from ignorance: Not agreeing with others we believe are correct. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 218–234. doi: 10.1037/a0034662
Hofman, D. , Bos, P. A. , Schutter, J.L. G. , & van Honk, J
(2012) Fairness modulates non-conscious facial mimicry in women. Proceedings of the Royal Society B , 279, 3535–3539.
Hrdy, S. B
(1999) Mother nature: A history of mothers, infants and natural selection. New York: Pantheon.
(2009) Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Hunt, K. D
(1994) The evolution of human bipedality: Ecology and functional morphology. Journal of Human Evolution, 26, 183–202. doi: 10.1006/jhev.1994.1011
Isbell, L. A. , & Young, T. P
(1996) The evolution of bipedalism in hominids and reduced group size in chimpanzees: Alternative responses to decreasing resource availability. Journal of Human Evolution, 30, 289–297. doi: 10.1006/jhev.1996.0034
Jetten, J. , & Hornsey, M. J
(2014) Deviance and dissent in groups. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 461–485. doi: 10.1146/annurev‑psych‑010213‑115151
Johansson, S. , Zlatev, J. , & Gardenfors, P
(2006) Why don’t chimps talk and humans sing like canaries?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 287–288. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X06319061
Karremans, J. C. , & Verwijmeren, T
(2008) Mimicking attractive opposite-sex others: The role of romantic relationship status. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 939–950. doi: 10.1177/0146167208316693
Laks, B
(2013) Why is there variation rather than nothing?Language Sciences, 39, 31–53. doi: 10.1016/j.langsci.2013.02.009
Lewis, D
(1969) Convention. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Locke, J. L. , & Bogin, B
(2006) Language and life history: A new perspective on the development and evolution of human language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 259–325.
Lovejoy, C. O
(1981) The origin of man. Science, 211, 341–350. doi: 10.1126/science.211.4480.341
(2009) Reexamining human origins in light of Ardipithecus ramidus . Science, 326, 74e1-74e8. doi: 10.1126/science.1175834
Lovejoy, C. O. , Suwa, G. , Spurlock, L. , Asfaw, B. , & White, T. D
(2009) The pelvis and femur of Ardipithecus ramidus: The emergence of upright walking. Science, 326, 71e1-71e6. doi: 10.1126/science.1175831
Lumsden, J. , Miles, L. K. , Richardson, M. J. , Smith, C. A. , & Macrae, C. N
(2012) Who syncs? Social motives and interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 746–751. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.007
Marsh, K. L. , Richardson, M. J. , & Schmidt, R. C
(2009) Social connection through joint action and interpersonal coordination. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 320–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1756‑8765.2009.01022.x
McBride, G
(2014) Storytelling, behavior planning, and language evolution in context. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1131. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01131
McElreath, R. , Lubell, M. , Richerson, P. J. , Waring, T. M. , Baum, W. , Edsten, E. , Efferson, C. , & Paciotti, B
(2005) Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 483–508. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.04.003
Mercier, H. , & Sperber, D
(2011) Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 57–111. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X10000968
Mesoudi, A
(2011) An experimental comparison of human social learning strategies: Payoff-biased social learning is adaptive but underused. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 334–342. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.12.001
Millikan, R. G
(2005) Language: A biological model. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/0199284768.001.0001
Mills, G. J
(2014) Dialogue in joint activity: Complementarity, convergence and conventionalization. New Ideas in Psychology, 32, 158–173. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2013.03.006
Moll, H. , & Tomasello, M
(2007) Co-operation and human cognition: The Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 362, 639–648. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.2000
(2012) Three-year-olds understand appearance and reality - Just not about the same object at the same time. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1124–1132. doi: 10.1037/a0025915
Moore, R
(2013) Imitation and conventional communication. Biology & Philosophy, 28, 481–500. doi: 10.1007/s10539‑012‑9349‑8
Nielsen, M
(2006) Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 555–565. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.42.3.555
Nielsen, M. , Mushin, I. , Tomaselli, K. , & Whiten, A
(2014) Where culture takes hold: “Overimitation” and its flexible deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen children. Child Development, 85, 2169–2184.
Nielsen, M. , & Tomaselli, K
(2010) Overimitation in Kalahari bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 21, 729–736. doi: 10.1177/0956797610368808
Niemitz, C
(2010) The evolution of upright posture and gait—a review and a new synthesis. Naturwissenschaften, 97, 241–263. doi: 10.1007/s00114‑009‑0637‑3
Over, H. , & Carpenter, M
(2012) Putting the social into social learning: Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 182–192. doi: 10.1037/a0024555
Pasquini, E. S. , Corriveau, K. H. , Koenig, M. , & Harris, P. L
(2007) Preschoolers monitor the relative accuracy of informants. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1216–1226. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.43.5.1216
Pickering, M. J. , & Garrod, S
(2004) Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169–190. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.43.5.1216
Pontzer, H. , Raichlen, D. A. , & Sockol, M. D
(2009) The metabolic cost of walking in humans, chimpanzees, and early hominins. Journal of Human Evolution, 56, 43–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.09.001
Provine, R. H
(2004) Walkie-talkie evolution: Bipedalism and vocal production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 520–521. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.09.001
Raubal, M
(2008) Wayfinding: Affordances and agent simulation. Encyclopedia of GIS (pp. 1243–1246). New York: Springer. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04410115
Reddy, V
(2008) How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Redhead, G. , & Dunbar, R.I. M
(2013) The functions of language: An experimental study. Evolutionary Psychology, 11, 845–854. doi: 10.1177/147470491301100409
Reed, E. S
(1996) Encountering the world: Toward an ecological psychology. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1177/147470491301100409
Rekers, Y. , Haun, D.B. M. , & Tomasello, M
(2011) Children, but not chimpanzees, prefer to collaborate. Current Biology, 21, 1756–1758. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.066
Richerson, P. J. , & Boyd, R
(2005) Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.066
Riley, M. A. , & Turvey, M. T
(2002) Variability and determinism in motor behavior. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34, 99–125. doi: 10.1080/00222890209601934
Roche, J. M. , Dale, R. , & Caucci, G. M
(2012) Double up on double meanings: Pragmatic alignment. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 1–24. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2010.509929
Rost, G. C. , & McMurray, B
(2010) Finding the signal by adding noise: The role of noncontrastive phonetic variability in early world learning. Infancy, 15, 608–635. doi: 10.1111/j.1532‑7078.2010.00033.x
Schel, A. M. , Townsend, S. W. , Machanda, Z. , Zuberbühler, K. , & Slocombe, K. E
(2013) Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLOS One, 8, e76674. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076674
Shockley, K. , Santana, M. V. , & Fowler, C. A
(2003) Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 326–332. doi: 10.1037/0096‑1523.29.2.326
Sockol, M. D. , Raichlen, D. A. , & Pontzer, H
(2007) Chimpanzee locomotor energetics and the origin of human bipedalism. PNAS, 104, 12265–12269. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703267104
Sterelny, K
(2003) Thought in a hostile world. Malden, MA: Basil Blackwell. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703267104
Strigul, N
(2009) Can imitation explain dialect origins?Ecological Modeling, 220, 2624–2639. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2009.07.005
Suddendorf, T
(2013) The gap: The science of what separates us from other animals. New York: Basic Books. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2009.07.005
Sutou, S
(2012) Hairless mutation: A driving force of humanization from a human-ape common ancestor by enforcing upright walking while holding a baby with both hands. Genes to Cells, 17, 264–272. doi: 10.1111/j.1365‑2443.2012.01592.x
Tilsen, S
(2015) Structured nonstationarity in articulatory timing. InThe Scottish Consortium for ICPhS 2015 (Ed.), Proceedings of the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Glasgow: University of Glasgow. doi: 10.1111/j.1365‑2443.2012.01592.x
Tomasello, M
(1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
(2008) Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
(2009) Why we cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
(2014a) A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. doi: 10.4159/9780674726369
(2014b) The ultra-social animal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 187–194. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2015
Tomasello, M. , & Haberl, K
(2003) Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology, 39, 906–912. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.39.5.906
Tomasello, M. , Melis, A. P. , Tennie, C. , Wyman, E. , & Herrmann, E
(2012) Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53, 673–692. doi: 10.1086/668207
Tomasello, M. , & Vaish, A
(2013) Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 231–255. doi: 10.1146/annurev‑psych‑113011‑143812
Waters, D. P
(2012) From extended phenotype to extended affordance: Distributed language at the intersection of Gibson and Dawkins. Language Sciences, 34, 507–512. doi: 10.1016/j.langsci.2012.03.007
Watson, J. , Payne, R. , Chamberlain, A. , Jones, R. K. , & Sellers, W. I
(2008) The energetic costs of load-carrying and the evolution of bipedalism. Journal of Human Evolution, 54, 675–683. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.10.004
(2009) The kinematics of load carrying in humans and great apes: Implications for the evolution of human bipedalism. Folia Primatology, 80, 309–328. doi: 10.1159/000258646
Whalen, D
(2014) Deliberate variability in speech for flexibly engaging other speakers. Poster at conference, Finding Common Ground: Social, Ecological, and Cognitive Perspectives on Language Use , University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT. doi: 10.1159/000258646
Whiten, A. , McGuigan, N. , Marshall-Pescini, S. , & Hopper, L. M
(2009) Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2417–2428. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0069
Wittig, R. M. , Crockford, C. , Deschner, T. , Langergraber, K. , Ziegler, T. E. , & Zuberbühler, K
(2014) Food sharing is linked to urinary oxytocin levels and bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 281, 20133096. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.3096
http://jbenjamins.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1075/is.18.1.02hod
Loading
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address