1887
Volume 18, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1572-0373
  • E-ISSN: 1572-0381
GBP
Buy:£15.00 + Taxes

Abstract

I tested the hypothesis that, in infant-mother-sibling interactions, infants with older siblings aged 11 to 24 months produce deictic gestures when they are proximal to, or engaging in joint attention with, their mothers more frequently than same-aged infants without siblings. Fifteen infant-mother dyads and 10 infant-mother-sibling triads were individually observed for 15 minutes in a playroom full of toys. Infants involved in infant-mother-sibling interactions produced more deictic gestures when they were proximal to their mothers than infants in infant-mother interactions. Further, infants involved in infant-mother-sibling interactions accompanied their gestures with vocalizations at a higher rate than infants in infant-mother interactions. This result suggests that infants with older siblings monitor their mothers more carefully in interactions in which their sibling is also present, and that they produce deictic gestures in order to effectively elicit joint action with their mothers.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/is.18.1.04kis
2017-07-28
2024-04-16
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Benigno, J. P. , Clark, L. , & Farrar, M. J
    (2007) Three is not always a crowd: Contexts of joint attention and language. Journal of Child Language, 34(1), 175–187. doi: 10.1017/S0305000906007732
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000906007732 [Google Scholar]
  2. Butterworth, G
    (2003) Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In S. Kita (Ed.), PointingPointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 9–33). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Emery, N. J
    (2000) The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(6), 581–604. doi: 10.1016/S0149‑7634(00)00025‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00025-7 [Google Scholar]
  4. Fleiss, J. L. , Levin, B. , & Paik, M. C
    (2003) Statistical methods for rates and proportions (3rd ed.). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons. doi: 10.1002/0471445428
    https://doi.org/10.1002/0471445428 [Google Scholar]
  5. Franco, F
    (2005) Infant pointing: Harlequin, servant of two masters. In N. Eilan , C. Hoerl , T. McCormack , & J. Roessler (Eds.), Joint attention: Communication and other minds. Issues in philosophy and psychology (pp. 129–164). Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199245635.003.0007
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199245635.003.0007 [Google Scholar]
  6. Goldin-Meadow, S. , Goodrich, W. , Sauer, E. , & Iverson, J
    (2007) Young children use their hands to tell their mothers what to say. Developmental Science, 10(6), 778–785. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2007.00636.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00636.x [Google Scholar]
  7. Gräfenhain, M. , Behne, T. , Carpenter, M. , & Tomasello, M
    (2009) One-year-olds’ understanding of nonverbal gestures directed to a third person. Cognitive Development, 24(1), 23–33. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.10.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2008.10.001 [Google Scholar]
  8. Jones, C. P. , & Adamson, L. B
    (1987) Language use in mother-child and mother-child-sibling interactions. Child Development, 58(2), 356–366. doi: 10.2307/1130512
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1130512 [Google Scholar]
  9. Krueger, J
    (2011) Extended cognition and the space of social interaction. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 643–657. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.022
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2010.09.022 [Google Scholar]
  10. Legerstee, M. , & Barillas, Y
    (2003) Sharing attention and pointing to objects at 12 months: Is the intentional stance implied?Cognitive Development, 18(1), 91–110. doi: 10.1016/S0885‑2014(02)00165‑X
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00165-X [Google Scholar]
  11. Liszkowski, U. , Albrecht, K. , Carpenter, M. , & Tomasello, M
    (2008) Infants’ visual and auditory communication when a partner is or is not visually attending. Infant Behavior and Development, 31(2), 157–167. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.10.011.
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.10.011 [Google Scholar]
  12. Liszkowski, U. , Brown, P. , Callaghan, T. , Takada, A. , & de Vos, C
    (2012) A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication. Cognitive Science, 36(4), 698–713. doi: 10.1111/j.1551‑6709.2011.01228.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01228.x [Google Scholar]
  13. Liszkowski, U. , Carpenter, M. , Henning, A. , Striano, T. , & Tomasello, M
    (2004) Twelve‐month‐olds point to share attention and interest. Developmental Science, 7(3), 297–307. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2004.00349.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2004.00349.x [Google Scholar]
  14. Liszkowski, U. , & Tomasello, M
    (2011) Individual differences in social, cognitive, and morphological aspects of infant pointing. Cognitive Development, 26, 16–29. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.10.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2010.10.001 [Google Scholar]
  15. Martin, P. , & Bateson, P
    (1990) Measuring behaviour: An introductory guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Moore, C. , & D’Entremont, B
    (2001) Developmental changes in pointing as a function of attentional focus. Journal of Cognition and Development, 2, 109–129. doi: 10.1207/S15327647JCD0202_1
    https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327647JCD0202_1 [Google Scholar]
  17. Salomo, D. , & Liszkowski, U
    (2013) Sociocultural settings influence the emergence of prelinguistic deictic gestures. Child development, 84(4), 1296–1307. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12026.
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12026 [Google Scholar]
  18. Thorgrimsson, G. B. , Fawcett, C. , & Liszkowski, U
    (2015) 1- and 2-year-olds’ expectations about third-party communicative actions. Infant Behavior and Development, 39, 53–66. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.02.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.02.002 [Google Scholar]
  19. Tomasello, M. , Mannle, S. , & Kruger, A. C
    (1986) Linguistic environment of 1- to 2-year-old twins. Developmental Psychology, 22(2), 169–176. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.22.2.169
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.2.169 [Google Scholar]
  20. Yamada, K
    (2008) Mother-offspring interactions and social development of offspring in a free-ranging group of Japanese macaques at Katsuyama. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Osaka University, Osaka, Japan.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Zhao, S
    (2003) Toward a taxonomy of copresence. Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 12(5), 445–455. doi: 10.1162/105474603322761261
    https://doi.org/10.1162/105474603322761261 [Google Scholar]
http://instance.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1075/is.18.1.04kis
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/is.18.1.04kis
Loading

Data & Media loading...

Most Cited

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error