1887

Production and comprehension of pronouns and reflexives in atypical populations

image of Production and comprehension of pronouns and reflexives in atypical populations

The production and comprehension of pronouns and reflexives has been extensively studied in typically-developing children, but has received less attention in children with neurodevelopmental disorders in which language impairment is the primary or secondary deficit, such as in children with specific language impairment, autism spectrum disorders, Williams syndrome and Down syndrome. Research on the acquisition of these core grammatical structures by atypical populations is important for understanding the linguistic nature of the disorders. The present chapter provides a review of experimental studies on the production and comprehension of these structures in different groups of atypical child and adolescent populations from a cross-linguistic perspective. The purpose of the chapter is to unravel similarities and differences between the affected and unaffected groups, and to investigate patterns of delayed and/or deviant performance across modalities and populations.

References

  1. Arnold, J. , Bennetto, L. , & Diehl, J
    (2009) Reference production in young speakers with and without autism: Effects of discourse status and processing constraints. Cognition, 110, 131–146. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.016
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.016 [Google Scholar]
  2. Baauw, S
    (2002) Grammatical features and the acquisition of reference: A comparative study of Dutch and Spanish. New York, NY: Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Baauw, S. , & Cuetos, F
    (2003) The interpretation of pronouns in Spanish language acquisition and breakdown: Evidence for the “Principle B delay” as a non-unitary phenomenon. Language Acquisition, 11, 219–275. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la1104_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la1104_2 [Google Scholar]
  4. Baauw, S. , Escobar, L. , & Philip, W
    (1997) A delay of Principle B effect in Spanish-speaking children: The role of lexical feature acquisition. In A. Sorace , C. Heycock , & R. Shillcock (Eds.), Proceedings of the GALA ‘97 Conference on Language Acquisition (pp. 81–92). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bedore, L. , & Leonard, L
    (2001) Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 905–924. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2001/072)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/072) [Google Scholar]
  6. Bellugi, U. , Bihrle, A. , Neville, H. , Jernigan, T. , & Doherty, S
    (1992) Language, cognition, and brain organization in a neurodevelopmental disorder. In M. Gunnar & C. Nelson (Eds.), Developmental behavioral neuroscience (pp. 201–232). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Bellugi, U. , Lichtenberger, L. , Jones, W. , Lai, Z. , & St. George, M
    . (2000) The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 1–29. doi: 10.1162/089892900561940
    https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900561940 [Google Scholar]
  8. Bergmann, C. , Paulus, M. , & Fikkert, P
    (2012) Preschoolers’ comprehension of pronouns and reflexives: The impact of the task. Journal of Child Language, 34, 777–803. doi: 10.1017/S0305000911000298
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000911000298 [Google Scholar]
  9. Blom, P. , Barss, A. , Nicol, J. , & Conway, L
    (1994) Children’s knowledge of binding and coreference: Evidence from spontaneous speech. Language, 70, 53–71. doi: 10.2307/416740
    https://doi.org/10.2307/416740 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bortolini, U. , Caselli, M.C. , Deevy, P. , & Leonard, L
    (2002) Specific language impairment in Italian: The first steps in the search for a clinical marker. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37, 77–93. doi: 10.1080/13682820110116758
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820110116758 [Google Scholar]
  11. Chien, Y.-C. , & Wexler, K
    (1990) Children’s knowledge of locality conditions in binding as evidence for the modularity of syntax and pragmatics. Language Acquisition, 1, 225–295. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0103_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0103_2 [Google Scholar]
  12. Chomsky, N
    (1981) Lectures on Government and Binding. Foris: Dordrecht.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. (1995) The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Chondrogianni, V. , Marinis, T. , Edwards, S. , & Blom, E
    (2015) Production and on-line comprehension of definite articles and clitic pronouns in Greek-speaking sequential bilingual children and children with SLI. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 1155–1191.. doi: 10.1017/S0142716414000101
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716414000101 [Google Scholar]
  15. Cipriani, P. , Bottari, P. , Chilosi, A.M. , & Pfanner, L
    (1998) A longitudinal perspective on the study of specific language impairment: The long-term follow-up of an Italian child. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 33, 245–280. doi: 10.1080/136828298247749
    https://doi.org/10.1080/136828298247749 [Google Scholar]
  16. Clahsen, H. , & Almazan, M
    (1998) Syntax and morphology in Williams syndrome. Cognition, 68, 167–198. doi: 10.1016/S0010‑0277(98)00049‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00049-3 [Google Scholar]
  17. Déchaine, R.-M. , & Wiltschko, M
    (2002) Decomposing pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry, 33, 409–422. doi: 10.1162/002438902760168554
    https://doi.org/10.1162/002438902760168554 [Google Scholar]
  18. de Villiers, J. , Cahillane, J. , & Altreuter, E
    (2006) What can production reveal about Principle B?In K.U. Deen , J. Nomura , B. Schulz , & B.D. Schwartz (Eds.), Proceedings of the Inaugural Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition – North America (GALANA) (pp. 89–100). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Fodor, J
    (1983) The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Friedmann, N. , & Novogrodsky, R
    (2004) The acquisition of relative clause comprehension in Hebrew: A study of SLI and normal development. Journal of Child Language, 31, 661–681. doi: 10.1017/S0305000904006269
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000904006269 [Google Scholar]
  21. Gathercole, S. , & Baddeley, A
    (1990) Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection?Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360. doi: 10.1016/0749‑596X(90)90004‑J
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)90004-J [Google Scholar]
  22. Gavarró, A. , Torrens, V. , & Wexler, K
    (2010) Object clitic omission: Two language types. Language Acquisition, 17, 192–219. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2010.509262
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2010.509262 [Google Scholar]
  23. Grodzinsky, Y. , & Reinhart, T
    (1993) The innateness of binding and coreference. Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 69–101.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Hendriks, P. , & Koster, C
    (2010) Production/comprehension asymmetries in language acquisition. Lingua, 120, 1887–2094. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2010.02.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.02.002 [Google Scholar]
  25. Hestvik, A. , Schwartz, R. , Tornyova, L. , Almodovar, D. , Love, T. , & Swinney, D
    (2007) Binding theory and syntactic movement are dissociated in SLI. Paper presented atthe 20th Annual CUNY Sentence Processing Conference, University of California at San Diego, USA.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Hulme, C. , & Snowling, M.J
    (2009) Developmental disorders of language learning and cognition. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Jakubowicz, C. , & Nash, L
    . (in press). Why accusative clitics are avoided in normal and impaired language development. In C. Jakubowicz , L. Nash , & K. Wexler (Eds.) Essays in syntax, morphology and phonology in SLI. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Jakubowicz, C. , Nash, L. , Rigaut, C. , & Gerard, C
    (1998) Determiners and clitic pronouns in French-speaking children with SLI. Language Acquisition, 7, 113–160. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0702‑4_3
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0702-4_3 [Google Scholar]
  29. Kemper, T.L
    (1988) Neuropathology of Down syndrome. In L. Nadel (Ed.), The psychobiology of Down syndrome (pp. 269–289). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Kjelgaard, M. , & Tager-Flusberg, H
    (2001) An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 287–308. doi: 10.1080/01690960042000058
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960042000058 [Google Scholar]
  31. Korenberg, J.R. , Chen, X.N. , Hirota, H. , Lai, Z. , Bellugi, U. , Burian, D. , Roe, B. , & Matsuoka, R
    (2000) Genome structure and cognitive map of Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 (Issue supplement 1), 89–107. doi: 10.1162/089892900562002
    https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900562002 [Google Scholar]
  32. Lee, A. , Hobson, R.P. , & Chiat, S
    (1994) I, you, me and autism: An experimental study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 155–176. doi: 10.1007/BF02172094
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172094 [Google Scholar]
  33. Leonard, L
    (1997) Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Manika, S. , Varlokosta, S. , & Wexler, K
    (2011) The lack of omission of clitics in Greek children with SLI. In N. Danis , K. Mesh , & H. Sung (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 429–437). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Marinis, T. , & Saddy, D
    (2013) Parsing the passive: Comparing children with specific language impairment to sequential bilingual children. Language Acquisition, 20, 155–179. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2013.766743
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2013.766743 [Google Scholar]
  36. Matthews, D. , Lieven, E. , Theakston, A. , & Tomasello, M
    (2009) Pronoun co-referencing errors: Challenges for generativist and usage-based accounts. Cognitive Linguistics, 20, 599–626. doi: 10.1515/COGL.2009.026
    https://doi.org/10.1515/COGL.2009.026 [Google Scholar]
  37. Mavrogiorgos, M
    (2010) Clitics in Greek: A minimalist account of proclisis and enclisis. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/la.160
    https://doi.org/10.1075/la.160 [Google Scholar]
  38. McKee, C
    (1992) A comparison of pronouns and anaphors in Italian and English acquisition. Language Acquisition, 2, 21–54. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0201_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0201_2 [Google Scholar]
  39. Miller, J
    (1988) The developmental asynchrony of language development in children with Down syndrome. In L. Nadel (Ed.), The psychobiology of Down syndrome (pp. 167–199). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Montgomery, J.W. , & Leonard, L.B
    (1998) Real-time inflectional processing by children with specific language impairment: Effects of phonetic substance. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1432–1443. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4106.1432
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4106.1432 [Google Scholar]
  41. (2006) Effects of acoustic manipulation on the real-time inflectional processing of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 1238–1256. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2006/089)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/089) [Google Scholar]
  42. Novogrodsky, R
    (2013) Subject pronoun use by children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 27, 85–93. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2012.742567
    https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2012.742567 [Google Scholar]
  43. Novogrodsky, R. , & Friedmann, N
    (2010) Not all dependencies are impaired in syntactic-SLI: Binding in children with a deficit in Wh-movement. In K. Franich , K. Mesh , & L. Keil (Eds.), Online supplement to the Proceedings of the 34th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. www.bu.edu/bucld/proceedings/supplement/vol34/ [29 April, 2014]
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Perovic, A
    (2006) Syntactic deficit in Down syndrome: More evidence for the modular organization of language. Lingua, 116, 1616–1630. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2005.05.011
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2005.05.011 [Google Scholar]
  45. Perovic, A. , Modyanova, N. , & Wexler, K
    (2013a) Comprehension of reflexive and personal pronouns in children with autism: A syntactic or pragmatic deficit?Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 813–835. doi: 10.1017/S0142716412000033
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000033 [Google Scholar]
  46. (2013b) Comparison of grammar in neurodevelopmental disorders: The case of binding in Williams syndrome and autism with and without language impairment. Language Acquisition, 20, 133–154. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2013.766742
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2013.766742 [Google Scholar]
  47. Perovic, A. , & Wexler, K
    (2007) Complex grammar in Williams syndrome. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21, 729–745. doi: 10.1080/02699200701541409
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200701541409 [Google Scholar]
  48. Philip, W. , & Coopmans, P
    (1996) The role of lexical feature acquisition in the development of pronominal anaphora. In W. Philip & F. Wijnen (Eds.), Amsterdam Series on Child Language Development (Vol. 5). Amsterdam: Instituut Algemene Taalwetenschap.
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Prévost, P
    (2009) The acquisition of French: The development of inflectional morphology and syntax in L1 acquisition, bilingualism and L2 acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/lald.51
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.51 [Google Scholar]
  50. Reilly, J. , Losh, M. , Bellugi, U. , & Wulfeck, B
    (2004) Frog, where are you? Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury and Williams syndrome. Brain and Language, 88, 229–247. doi: 10.1016/S0093‑934X(03)00101‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00101-9 [Google Scholar]
  51. Reinhart, T. , & Reuland, E
    (1993) Reflexivity. Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 657–720.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Reuland, E
    (2001) Primitives of binding. Linguistic Inquiry, 32, 439–492. doi: 10.1162/002438901750372522
    https://doi.org/10.1162/002438901750372522 [Google Scholar]
  53. Rice, M.L. , Warren, S.F. , & Betz, S.K
    (2005) Language symptoms of developmental language disorders: An overview of autism, Down syndrome, fragile X, specific language impairment, and Williams syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 7–27. doi: 10.1017/S0142716405050034
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716405050034 [Google Scholar]
  54. Rice, M.L. , & Wexler, K
    (1996) Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1239–1257. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3906.1239
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3906.1239 [Google Scholar]
  55. (2001) Rice/Wexler Test of Early Grammatical Impairment. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Ring, M. , & Clahsen, H
    (2005) Distinct patterns of language impairment in Down’s syndrome and Williams syndrome: The case of syntactic chains. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 18, 479–501. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2005.06.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2005.06.002 [Google Scholar]
  57. Roberts, J. , Rice, M. , & Tager-Flusberg, H
    (2004) Tense marking in children with autism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 429–448. doi: 10.1017/S0142716404001201
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001201 [Google Scholar]
  58. Sanoudaki, E. , & Varlokosta, S
    (2014) Pronoun comprehension in individuals with Down syndrome: Deviance or delay? Journal of Speech , Language and Hearing Research, 57, 1442–1452. doi: 10.1044/2014_JSLHR‑L‑13‑0035
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0035 [Google Scholar]
  59. Schaeffer, J
    (2000) The acquisition of direct object scrambling and clitic placement: Syntax and pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/lald.22
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.22 [Google Scholar]
  60. Schwartz, R
    (2009) Handbook of child language disorders. London: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Sheng, L. , & McGregor, K.K
    (2010) Object and action naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 1704–1719. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2010/09‑0180)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0180) [Google Scholar]
  62. Smith, S.D. , & Morris, C.A
    (2005) Planning studies of etiology. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 97–110. doi: 10.1017/S0142716405050083
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716405050083 [Google Scholar]
  63. Spenader, J. , Smits, E.J. , & Hendriks, P
    (2009) Coherent discourse solves the Pronoun Interpretation Problem. Journal of Child Language, 36, 23–52. doi: 10.1017/S0305000908008854
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008854 [Google Scholar]
  64. Stathopoulou, N
    (2009) The linguistic profile of Greek individuals with Down syndrome: Evidence from syntactic and morphological phenomena (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Essex, Colchester, UK.
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Stavrakaki, S. , & van der Lely, H.K.J
    (2010) Production and comprehension of pronouns by Greek children with specific language impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 189–216. doi: 10.1348/026151010X485269
    https://doi.org/10.1348/026151010X485269 [Google Scholar]
  66. Tager-Flusberg, H
    (1981) On the nature of linguistic functioning in early infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 45–56. doi: 10.1007/BF01531340
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01531340 [Google Scholar]
  67. (1995) “Once upon a ribbit”: Stories narrated by autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 45–59. doi: 10.1111/j.2044‑835X.1995.tb00663.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1995.tb00663.x [Google Scholar]
  68. (1999) Language development in atypical children. In M. Barrett (Ed.), The development of language (pp. 311–348). Hove, Sussex: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Terzi, A. , Marinis, T. , Francis, K. , & Kotsopoulou, A
    (2012) Crosslinguistic differences in autistic children’s comprehension of pronouns: English vs. Greek. In K. Alia Biller , Y.E. Chung , & A.E. Kimball (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 607–619). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Terzi, A. , Marinis, T. , Kotsopoulou, A. , & Francis, K
    (2014) Grammatical abilities of Greek-speaking children with autism, Language Acquisition, 21, 4–44. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2013.855216
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2013.855216 [Google Scholar]
  71. Thornton, R. , & Wexler, K
    (1999) Principle B, VP ellipsis, and interpretation in child grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Tomasello, M
    (2003) Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Tsimpli, I.M. , & Stavrakaki, S
    (1999) The effects of a morphosyntactic deficit in the determiner system: The case of a Greek SLI child. Lingua, 108, 31–85. doi: 10.1016/S0024‑3841(98)00041‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00041-2 [Google Scholar]
  74. Tuller, L. , Delage, H. , Monjauze, C. , Piller, A-G. , & Barthez, M.-A
    (2011) Clitic pronoun production as a measure of atypical language development in French. Lingua, 121, 421–441. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2010.10.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.10.008 [Google Scholar]
  75. Van der Lely, H.K.J
    (1997) Narrative discourse in grammatical specific language impaired children: A modular language deficit?Journal of Child Language, 24, 221–256. doi: 10.1017/S0305000996002966
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000996002966 [Google Scholar]
  76. (1998) SLI in children: Movement, economy and deficits in the computational-syntactic system. Language Acquisition, 7, 161–192. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0702‑4_4
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0702-4_4 [Google Scholar]
  77. (2005) Domain-specific cognitive systems: Evidence from grammatical SLI. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 53–59. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  78. Van der Lely, H.K.J. , & Stollwerk, L
    (1997) Binding theory and grammatical specific language impairment in children. Cognition, 62, 245–290. doi: 10.1016/S0010‑0277(96)00783‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00783-4 [Google Scholar]
  79. Varlokosta, S
    (2000) Lack of clitic pronoun distinctions in the acquisition of principle B in child Greek. In S.C. Howell , S. Fish , & T. Keith-Lucas (Eds.), Proceedings of the 24th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 738–748). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Varlokosta, S. , & Nerantzini, M
    (2012) Pronominal and anaphoric reference in Greek specific language impairment. In Z. Gavrilidou , A. Efthymiou , E. Thomadaki , & P. Kambakis-Vougiouklis (Eds.), Selected Papers of the 10th International Conference of Greek Linguistics (pp. 584–591). Komotini: Democritus University of Thrace.
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Wexler, K
    (1998) Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua, 106, 23–79. doi: 10.1016/S0024‑3841(98)00029‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00029-1 [Google Scholar]

References

  1. Arnold, J. , Bennetto, L. , & Diehl, J
    (2009) Reference production in young speakers with and without autism: Effects of discourse status and processing constraints. Cognition, 110, 131–146. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.016
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.016 [Google Scholar]
  2. Baauw, S
    (2002) Grammatical features and the acquisition of reference: A comparative study of Dutch and Spanish. New York, NY: Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Baauw, S. , & Cuetos, F
    (2003) The interpretation of pronouns in Spanish language acquisition and breakdown: Evidence for the “Principle B delay” as a non-unitary phenomenon. Language Acquisition, 11, 219–275. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la1104_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la1104_2 [Google Scholar]
  4. Baauw, S. , Escobar, L. , & Philip, W
    (1997) A delay of Principle B effect in Spanish-speaking children: The role of lexical feature acquisition. In A. Sorace , C. Heycock , & R. Shillcock (Eds.), Proceedings of the GALA ‘97 Conference on Language Acquisition (pp. 81–92). Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bedore, L. , & Leonard, L
    (2001) Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 905–924. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2001/072)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/072) [Google Scholar]
  6. Bellugi, U. , Bihrle, A. , Neville, H. , Jernigan, T. , & Doherty, S
    (1992) Language, cognition, and brain organization in a neurodevelopmental disorder. In M. Gunnar & C. Nelson (Eds.), Developmental behavioral neuroscience (pp. 201–232). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Bellugi, U. , Lichtenberger, L. , Jones, W. , Lai, Z. , & St. George, M
    . (2000) The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12, 1–29. doi: 10.1162/089892900561940
    https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900561940 [Google Scholar]
  8. Bergmann, C. , Paulus, M. , & Fikkert, P
    (2012) Preschoolers’ comprehension of pronouns and reflexives: The impact of the task. Journal of Child Language, 34, 777–803. doi: 10.1017/S0305000911000298
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000911000298 [Google Scholar]
  9. Blom, P. , Barss, A. , Nicol, J. , & Conway, L
    (1994) Children’s knowledge of binding and coreference: Evidence from spontaneous speech. Language, 70, 53–71. doi: 10.2307/416740
    https://doi.org/10.2307/416740 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bortolini, U. , Caselli, M.C. , Deevy, P. , & Leonard, L
    (2002) Specific language impairment in Italian: The first steps in the search for a clinical marker. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 37, 77–93. doi: 10.1080/13682820110116758
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820110116758 [Google Scholar]
  11. Chien, Y.-C. , & Wexler, K
    (1990) Children’s knowledge of locality conditions in binding as evidence for the modularity of syntax and pragmatics. Language Acquisition, 1, 225–295. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0103_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0103_2 [Google Scholar]
  12. Chomsky, N
    (1981) Lectures on Government and Binding. Foris: Dordrecht.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. (1995) The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Chondrogianni, V. , Marinis, T. , Edwards, S. , & Blom, E
    (2015) Production and on-line comprehension of definite articles and clitic pronouns in Greek-speaking sequential bilingual children and children with SLI. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36, 1155–1191.. doi: 10.1017/S0142716414000101
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716414000101 [Google Scholar]
  15. Cipriani, P. , Bottari, P. , Chilosi, A.M. , & Pfanner, L
    (1998) A longitudinal perspective on the study of specific language impairment: The long-term follow-up of an Italian child. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 33, 245–280. doi: 10.1080/136828298247749
    https://doi.org/10.1080/136828298247749 [Google Scholar]
  16. Clahsen, H. , & Almazan, M
    (1998) Syntax and morphology in Williams syndrome. Cognition, 68, 167–198. doi: 10.1016/S0010‑0277(98)00049‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00049-3 [Google Scholar]
  17. Déchaine, R.-M. , & Wiltschko, M
    (2002) Decomposing pronouns. Linguistic Inquiry, 33, 409–422. doi: 10.1162/002438902760168554
    https://doi.org/10.1162/002438902760168554 [Google Scholar]
  18. de Villiers, J. , Cahillane, J. , & Altreuter, E
    (2006) What can production reveal about Principle B?In K.U. Deen , J. Nomura , B. Schulz , & B.D. Schwartz (Eds.), Proceedings of the Inaugural Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition – North America (GALANA) (pp. 89–100). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Fodor, J
    (1983) The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Friedmann, N. , & Novogrodsky, R
    (2004) The acquisition of relative clause comprehension in Hebrew: A study of SLI and normal development. Journal of Child Language, 31, 661–681. doi: 10.1017/S0305000904006269
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000904006269 [Google Scholar]
  21. Gathercole, S. , & Baddeley, A
    (1990) Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection?Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360. doi: 10.1016/0749‑596X(90)90004‑J
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)90004-J [Google Scholar]
  22. Gavarró, A. , Torrens, V. , & Wexler, K
    (2010) Object clitic omission: Two language types. Language Acquisition, 17, 192–219. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2010.509262
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2010.509262 [Google Scholar]
  23. Grodzinsky, Y. , & Reinhart, T
    (1993) The innateness of binding and coreference. Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 69–101.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Hendriks, P. , & Koster, C
    (2010) Production/comprehension asymmetries in language acquisition. Lingua, 120, 1887–2094. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2010.02.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.02.002 [Google Scholar]
  25. Hestvik, A. , Schwartz, R. , Tornyova, L. , Almodovar, D. , Love, T. , & Swinney, D
    (2007) Binding theory and syntactic movement are dissociated in SLI. Paper presented atthe 20th Annual CUNY Sentence Processing Conference, University of California at San Diego, USA.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Hulme, C. , & Snowling, M.J
    (2009) Developmental disorders of language learning and cognition. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Jakubowicz, C. , & Nash, L
    . (in press). Why accusative clitics are avoided in normal and impaired language development. In C. Jakubowicz , L. Nash , & K. Wexler (Eds.) Essays in syntax, morphology and phonology in SLI. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Jakubowicz, C. , Nash, L. , Rigaut, C. , & Gerard, C
    (1998) Determiners and clitic pronouns in French-speaking children with SLI. Language Acquisition, 7, 113–160. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0702‑4_3
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0702-4_3 [Google Scholar]
  29. Kemper, T.L
    (1988) Neuropathology of Down syndrome. In L. Nadel (Ed.), The psychobiology of Down syndrome (pp. 269–289). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Kjelgaard, M. , & Tager-Flusberg, H
    (2001) An investigation of language impairment in autism: Implications for genetic subgroups. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 287–308. doi: 10.1080/01690960042000058
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960042000058 [Google Scholar]
  31. Korenberg, J.R. , Chen, X.N. , Hirota, H. , Lai, Z. , Bellugi, U. , Burian, D. , Roe, B. , & Matsuoka, R
    (2000) Genome structure and cognitive map of Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12 (Issue supplement 1), 89–107. doi: 10.1162/089892900562002
    https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900562002 [Google Scholar]
  32. Lee, A. , Hobson, R.P. , & Chiat, S
    (1994) I, you, me and autism: An experimental study. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 155–176. doi: 10.1007/BF02172094
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172094 [Google Scholar]
  33. Leonard, L
    (1997) Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Manika, S. , Varlokosta, S. , & Wexler, K
    (2011) The lack of omission of clitics in Greek children with SLI. In N. Danis , K. Mesh , & H. Sung (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 429–437). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Marinis, T. , & Saddy, D
    (2013) Parsing the passive: Comparing children with specific language impairment to sequential bilingual children. Language Acquisition, 20, 155–179. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2013.766743
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2013.766743 [Google Scholar]
  36. Matthews, D. , Lieven, E. , Theakston, A. , & Tomasello, M
    (2009) Pronoun co-referencing errors: Challenges for generativist and usage-based accounts. Cognitive Linguistics, 20, 599–626. doi: 10.1515/COGL.2009.026
    https://doi.org/10.1515/COGL.2009.026 [Google Scholar]
  37. Mavrogiorgos, M
    (2010) Clitics in Greek: A minimalist account of proclisis and enclisis. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/la.160
    https://doi.org/10.1075/la.160 [Google Scholar]
  38. McKee, C
    (1992) A comparison of pronouns and anaphors in Italian and English acquisition. Language Acquisition, 2, 21–54. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0201_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0201_2 [Google Scholar]
  39. Miller, J
    (1988) The developmental asynchrony of language development in children with Down syndrome. In L. Nadel (Ed.), The psychobiology of Down syndrome (pp. 167–199). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Montgomery, J.W. , & Leonard, L.B
    (1998) Real-time inflectional processing by children with specific language impairment: Effects of phonetic substance. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1432–1443. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4106.1432
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4106.1432 [Google Scholar]
  41. (2006) Effects of acoustic manipulation on the real-time inflectional processing of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 1238–1256. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2006/089)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/089) [Google Scholar]
  42. Novogrodsky, R
    (2013) Subject pronoun use by children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD). Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 27, 85–93. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2012.742567
    https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2012.742567 [Google Scholar]
  43. Novogrodsky, R. , & Friedmann, N
    (2010) Not all dependencies are impaired in syntactic-SLI: Binding in children with a deficit in Wh-movement. In K. Franich , K. Mesh , & L. Keil (Eds.), Online supplement to the Proceedings of the 34th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. www.bu.edu/bucld/proceedings/supplement/vol34/ [29 April, 2014]
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Perovic, A
    (2006) Syntactic deficit in Down syndrome: More evidence for the modular organization of language. Lingua, 116, 1616–1630. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2005.05.011
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2005.05.011 [Google Scholar]
  45. Perovic, A. , Modyanova, N. , & Wexler, K
    (2013a) Comprehension of reflexive and personal pronouns in children with autism: A syntactic or pragmatic deficit?Applied Psycholinguistics, 34, 813–835. doi: 10.1017/S0142716412000033
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000033 [Google Scholar]
  46. (2013b) Comparison of grammar in neurodevelopmental disorders: The case of binding in Williams syndrome and autism with and without language impairment. Language Acquisition, 20, 133–154. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2013.766742
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2013.766742 [Google Scholar]
  47. Perovic, A. , & Wexler, K
    (2007) Complex grammar in Williams syndrome. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 21, 729–745. doi: 10.1080/02699200701541409
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200701541409 [Google Scholar]
  48. Philip, W. , & Coopmans, P
    (1996) The role of lexical feature acquisition in the development of pronominal anaphora. In W. Philip & F. Wijnen (Eds.), Amsterdam Series on Child Language Development (Vol. 5). Amsterdam: Instituut Algemene Taalwetenschap.
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Prévost, P
    (2009) The acquisition of French: The development of inflectional morphology and syntax in L1 acquisition, bilingualism and L2 acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/lald.51
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.51 [Google Scholar]
  50. Reilly, J. , Losh, M. , Bellugi, U. , & Wulfeck, B
    (2004) Frog, where are you? Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury and Williams syndrome. Brain and Language, 88, 229–247. doi: 10.1016/S0093‑934X(03)00101‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00101-9 [Google Scholar]
  51. Reinhart, T. , & Reuland, E
    (1993) Reflexivity. Linguistic Inquiry, 24, 657–720.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Reuland, E
    (2001) Primitives of binding. Linguistic Inquiry, 32, 439–492. doi: 10.1162/002438901750372522
    https://doi.org/10.1162/002438901750372522 [Google Scholar]
  53. Rice, M.L. , Warren, S.F. , & Betz, S.K
    (2005) Language symptoms of developmental language disorders: An overview of autism, Down syndrome, fragile X, specific language impairment, and Williams syndrome. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 7–27. doi: 10.1017/S0142716405050034
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716405050034 [Google Scholar]
  54. Rice, M.L. , & Wexler, K
    (1996) Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1239–1257. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3906.1239
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3906.1239 [Google Scholar]
  55. (2001) Rice/Wexler Test of Early Grammatical Impairment. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Ring, M. , & Clahsen, H
    (2005) Distinct patterns of language impairment in Down’s syndrome and Williams syndrome: The case of syntactic chains. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 18, 479–501. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2005.06.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2005.06.002 [Google Scholar]
  57. Roberts, J. , Rice, M. , & Tager-Flusberg, H
    (2004) Tense marking in children with autism. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 429–448. doi: 10.1017/S0142716404001201
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001201 [Google Scholar]
  58. Sanoudaki, E. , & Varlokosta, S
    (2014) Pronoun comprehension in individuals with Down syndrome: Deviance or delay? Journal of Speech , Language and Hearing Research, 57, 1442–1452. doi: 10.1044/2014_JSLHR‑L‑13‑0035
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0035 [Google Scholar]
  59. Schaeffer, J
    (2000) The acquisition of direct object scrambling and clitic placement: Syntax and pragmatics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/lald.22
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.22 [Google Scholar]
  60. Schwartz, R
    (2009) Handbook of child language disorders. London: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Sheng, L. , & McGregor, K.K
    (2010) Object and action naming in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 1704–1719. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2010/09‑0180)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2010/09-0180) [Google Scholar]
  62. Smith, S.D. , & Morris, C.A
    (2005) Planning studies of etiology. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 97–110. doi: 10.1017/S0142716405050083
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716405050083 [Google Scholar]
  63. Spenader, J. , Smits, E.J. , & Hendriks, P
    (2009) Coherent discourse solves the Pronoun Interpretation Problem. Journal of Child Language, 36, 23–52. doi: 10.1017/S0305000908008854
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008854 [Google Scholar]
  64. Stathopoulou, N
    (2009) The linguistic profile of Greek individuals with Down syndrome: Evidence from syntactic and morphological phenomena (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Essex, Colchester, UK.
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Stavrakaki, S. , & van der Lely, H.K.J
    (2010) Production and comprehension of pronouns by Greek children with specific language impairment. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 189–216. doi: 10.1348/026151010X485269
    https://doi.org/10.1348/026151010X485269 [Google Scholar]
  66. Tager-Flusberg, H
    (1981) On the nature of linguistic functioning in early infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 45–56. doi: 10.1007/BF01531340
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01531340 [Google Scholar]
  67. (1995) “Once upon a ribbit”: Stories narrated by autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13, 45–59. doi: 10.1111/j.2044‑835X.1995.tb00663.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1995.tb00663.x [Google Scholar]
  68. (1999) Language development in atypical children. In M. Barrett (Ed.), The development of language (pp. 311–348). Hove, Sussex: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Terzi, A. , Marinis, T. , Francis, K. , & Kotsopoulou, A
    (2012) Crosslinguistic differences in autistic children’s comprehension of pronouns: English vs. Greek. In K. Alia Biller , Y.E. Chung , & A.E. Kimball (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 607–619). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Terzi, A. , Marinis, T. , Kotsopoulou, A. , & Francis, K
    (2014) Grammatical abilities of Greek-speaking children with autism, Language Acquisition, 21, 4–44. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2013.855216
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2013.855216 [Google Scholar]
  71. Thornton, R. , & Wexler, K
    (1999) Principle B, VP ellipsis, and interpretation in child grammar. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Tomasello, M
    (2003) Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Tsimpli, I.M. , & Stavrakaki, S
    (1999) The effects of a morphosyntactic deficit in the determiner system: The case of a Greek SLI child. Lingua, 108, 31–85. doi: 10.1016/S0024‑3841(98)00041‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00041-2 [Google Scholar]
  74. Tuller, L. , Delage, H. , Monjauze, C. , Piller, A-G. , & Barthez, M.-A
    (2011) Clitic pronoun production as a measure of atypical language development in French. Lingua, 121, 421–441. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2010.10.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.10.008 [Google Scholar]
  75. Van der Lely, H.K.J
    (1997) Narrative discourse in grammatical specific language impaired children: A modular language deficit?Journal of Child Language, 24, 221–256. doi: 10.1017/S0305000996002966
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000996002966 [Google Scholar]
  76. (1998) SLI in children: Movement, economy and deficits in the computational-syntactic system. Language Acquisition, 7, 161–192. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0702‑4_4
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0702-4_4 [Google Scholar]
  77. (2005) Domain-specific cognitive systems: Evidence from grammatical SLI. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 53–59. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  78. Van der Lely, H.K.J. , & Stollwerk, L
    (1997) Binding theory and grammatical specific language impairment in children. Cognition, 62, 245–290. doi: 10.1016/S0010‑0277(96)00783‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00783-4 [Google Scholar]
  79. Varlokosta, S
    (2000) Lack of clitic pronoun distinctions in the acquisition of principle B in child Greek. In S.C. Howell , S. Fish , & T. Keith-Lucas (Eds.), Proceedings of the 24th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 738–748). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Varlokosta, S. , & Nerantzini, M
    (2012) Pronominal and anaphoric reference in Greek specific language impairment. In Z. Gavrilidou , A. Efthymiou , E. Thomadaki , & P. Kambakis-Vougiouklis (Eds.), Selected Papers of the 10th International Conference of Greek Linguistics (pp. 584–591). Komotini: Democritus University of Thrace.
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Wexler, K
    (1998) Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua, 106, 23–79. doi: 10.1016/S0024‑3841(98)00029‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00029-1 [Google Scholar]
/content/books/9789027267894-tilar.15.12cho
dcterms_subject,pub_keyword
-contentType:Journal
10
5
Chapter
content/books/9789027267894
Book
false
Loading
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error