1887
Volume 9, Issue 2
  • ISSN 1876-1933
  • E-ISSN: 1876-1941
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

The Dutch verb phrase (VP) is notorious for its syntactic intricacies. There are three main reasons why it is difficult to robustly handle its complexity in a processing model. First of all, a single VP can contain multiple modal auxiliaries (‘modal stacking’). Second, perfect auxiliaries can not only take a past participle as their argument but also a modal auxiliary in the infinitive form. Finally, there are various word orders in which the verb forms can appear. The first part of this paper presents a fully operational precision processing model of the Dutch VP in Fluid Construction Grammar. The model shows that the aforementioned challenges can be overcome by carefully managing the hierarchical relations between the elements of the VP. The second part introduces a robust comprehension method, which can process VPs containing morphological and word order errors. This method allows in many cases to recover the intended meaning of an erroneous VP, as well as to correct its form, using a strategy that exploits the deep semantic analyses and the bidirectional nature of the model.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/cf.00003.van
2017-12-30
2023-05-30
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Allen, J.
    (1984) Towards a general theory of action and time. Artificial intelligence, 23(2), 123–154. doi: 10.1016/0004‑3702(84)90008‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0004-3702(84)90008-0 [Google Scholar]
  2. Boogaart, R.
    (2013) De modaliteit van temporaliteit. Nederlandse Taalkunde, 18(3), 324–338. doi: 10.5117/NEDTAA2013.3.BOOG
    https://doi.org/10.5117/NEDTAA2013.3.BOOG [Google Scholar]
  3. Haeseryn, W. , Romijn, K. , Geerts, G. , de Rooij, J. , & van den Toorn, M.
    (1997) Algemene Nederlands Spraakkunst. Martinus Nijhoff/Wolters Plantyn, 2nd, revised edition.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Pani, A. , & Bhattacharjee, G.
    (2001) Temporal representation and reasoning in artificial intelligence: A review. Mathematical and Computer Modelling, 34(1–2), 55–80. doi: 10.1016/S0895‑7177(01)00049‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0895-7177(01)00049-8 [Google Scholar]
  5. Pardoen, J.
    (1986) Werkwoordclustering in de voltooide tijd. Voortgang, 7, 49–76.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Pickering, M. , & Garrod, S.
    (2007) Do people use language production to make predictions during comprehension?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(3), 105–110. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  7. (2013) An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(04), 329–347. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12001495
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12001495 [Google Scholar]
  8. Reichenbach, H.
    (1947) Elements of Symbolic Logic. Dover Publications Inc.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Steels, L. (2003) Language re-entrance and the inner voice. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10(4–5), 173–185.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. (Ed.) (2011) Design patterns in Fluid Construction Grammar. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/cal.11
    https://doi.org/10.1075/cal.11 [Google Scholar]
  11. (Ed.) (2012) Computational issues in Fluid Construction Grammar. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer Verlag. doi: 10.1007/978‑3‑642‑34120‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-34120-5 [Google Scholar]
  12. Verkuyl, H. , & Broekhuis, H.
    (2013) Temporaliteit en modaliteit. Nederlandse Taalkunde, 18(3), 306–323. doi: 10.5117/NEDTAA2013.3.VERK
    https://doi.org/10.5117/NEDTAA2013.3.VERK [Google Scholar]
http://instance.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1075/cf.00003.van
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/cf.00003.van
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error