1887
Volume 10, Issue 2
  • ISSN 1876-1933
  • E-ISSN: 1876-1941
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Prior studies suggest that language users perform motoric simulations when construing action sentences and that verbs and constructions each contribute to simulation-based representation (Glenberg & Kaschak 2002Richardson et al. 2003Bergen et al. 2007Bergen & Wheeler 2010). This raises the possibility that motorically grounded verb and construction meanings can interact during sentence understanding. In this experiment, we use the action-sentence compatibility effect methodology to investigate how a verb’s lexical-class membership, constructional context, and constructional bias modulate motor simulation effects. Stimuli represent two classes of transfer verbs and two constructions that encode transfer events, Ditransitive and Oblique Goal (Goldberg 1995). Findings reveal two kinds of verb-construction interactions. First, verbs in their preferred construction generate stronger simulation effects overall than those in their dispreferred construction. Second, verbs that entail change of possession generate strong motor-simulation effects irrespective of constructional context, while those entailing causation of motion exert such effects only when enriched up to change-of-possession verbs in the semantically mismatched Ditransitive context. We conclude that simulation effects are not isolable to either verbs or constructions but instead arise from the interplay of verb and construction meaning.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/cf.00020.gou
2019-01-21
2025-02-19
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Anderson, S. E., & Spivey, M. J.
    (2009) The enactment of language: Decades of interactions between linguistic and motor processes. Language and Cognition, 1, 87–111. 10.1515/LANGCOG.2009.005
    https://doi.org/10.1515/LANGCOG.2009.005 [Google Scholar]
  2. Bates, E., Bretherton, I., & Snyder, L.
    (1988) From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Barsalou, L. W.
    (1999) Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Bergen, B., & Chang, N.
    (2005) Embodied construction grammar in simulation-based language understanding. InJ.-O. Östman & M. Fried (Eds.), Construction grammars: Cognitive grounding and theoretical extensions, 3, 147–190. 10.1075/cal.3.08ber
    https://doi.org/10.1075/cal.3.08ber [Google Scholar]
  5. (2013) Embodied Construction Grammar. InT. Hoffmann, & G. Trousdale (Eds.), Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar (pp.168–190). New York: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Bergen, B. K., & Han, W.-W.
    (2008) Constructional meaning and mental simulation. Paper presented at theFifth International Conference on Construction Grammar. University of Texas, Austin.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Bergen, B. K., Lindsay, S., Matlock, T., & Narayanan, S.
    (2007) Spatial and linguistic aspects of visual imagery in sentence comprehension. Cognitive Science, 31, 733–764. 10.1080/03640210701530748
    https://doi.org/10.1080/03640210701530748 [Google Scholar]
  8. Bergen, B., & Wheeler, K.
    (2010) Grammatical aspect and mental simulation. Brain and Language, 112, 150–158. 10.1016/j.bandl.2009.07.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2009.07.002 [Google Scholar]
  9. Bresnan, J.
    (1994) Locative inversion and the architecture of universal grammar. Language, 70, 2–131. 10.2307/416741
    https://doi.org/10.2307/416741 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bresnan, J., Cueni, A., Nikitina, T., & Baayen, R. H.
    (2007) Predicting the dative alternation. InG. Bouma, I. Krämer, & J. Zwarts (Eds.), Cognitive foundations of interpretation (pp.69–94). Amsterdam: Royal Academy of Science.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Bybee, J.
    (2001) Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511612886
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511612886 [Google Scholar]
  12. (2010) Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511750526
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511750526 [Google Scholar]
  13. Campbell, A., & Tomasello, M.
    (2001) The acquisition of English dative constructions. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 253–267. 10.1017/S0142716401002065
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716401002065 [Google Scholar]
  14. Croft, W.
    (2012) Verbs: Aspect and causal structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199248582.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199248582.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  15. Erteschik-Shir, N.
    (1979) Discourse constraints on dative movement in discourse and syntax. Syntax and Semantics, 12, 441–467.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Fellbaum, C.
    (Ed.) (1998) WordNet: An electronic lexical database. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 10.7551/mitpress/7287.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/7287.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  17. Fillmore, C. J., & Kay, P.
    (1995) Construction Grammar coursebook. Unpublished ms., University of California at Berkeley.
  18. Fillmore, C. J., Ruppenhofer, J., & Baker, C. F.
    (2004) Framenet and representing the link between semantic and syntactic relations. InC. Huang & W. Lenders (Eds.), Computational Linguistics and Beyond (pp.19–62). Taipei: Institute of Linguistics, Academia Sinica.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Fischer, M. H., & Zwaan, R. A.
    (2008) Embodied language: A review of the role of the motor system in language comprehension. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 825–850. 10.1080/17470210701623605
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701623605 [Google Scholar]
  20. Gahl, S., & Garnsey, S. M.
    (2004) Knowledge of grammar, knowledge of usage: Syntactic probabilities affect pronunciation variation. Language, 80, 748–775. 10.1353/lan.2004.0185
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2004.0185 [Google Scholar]
  21. (2006) Knowledge of grammar includes knowledge of syntactic probabilities. Language, 82, 405–410. 10.1353/lan.2006.0089
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2006.0089 [Google Scholar]
  22. Gahl, S., Menn, L., Ramsberger, G., Jurafsky, D., Elder, E., Rewega, M., & Holland, A.
    (2003) Syntactic frame and verb bias in aphasia: Plausibility judgments of undergoer-subject sentences. Brain and Cognition, 53, 223–228. 10.1016/S0278‑2626(03)00114‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00114-3 [Google Scholar]
  23. Garnsey, S. M., Pearlmutter, N. J., Myers, E., & Lotocky, M. A.
    (1997) The contributions of verb bias and plausibility to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 58–93. 10.1006/jmla.1997.2512
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2512 [Google Scholar]
  24. Givón, T.
    (1984) Direct object and dative shifting: Semantic and pragmatic case. InF. Plank (Ed.), Objects: Towards a theory of grammatical relations (pp.151–182). London: Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Glenberg, A. M., & Kaschak, M. P.
    (2002) Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 558–565. 10.3758/BF03196313
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196313 [Google Scholar]
  26. Goldberg, A.
    (1995) Constructions: A Construction Grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. (2006) Constructions at work: The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Gropen, J., Pinker, S., Hollander, M., Goldberg, R., & Wilson, R.
    (1989) The learnability and acquisition of the dative alternation in English. Language, 65, 203–257. 10.2307/415332
    https://doi.org/10.2307/415332 [Google Scholar]
  29. Groefsema, M.
    (2001) The real-world colour of the dative alternation. Language Sciences, 23, 525–550. 10.1016/S0388‑0001(00)00035‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0388-0001(00)00035-8 [Google Scholar]
  30. Hauk, O., Johnsrude, I., & Pulvermüller, F.
    (2004) Somatotopic representation of action words in human motor and premotor cortex. Neuron, 41, 301–307. 10.1016/S0896‑6273(03)00838‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00838-9 [Google Scholar]
  31. Jackendoff, R.
    (1990) Semantic Structures. Cambridge: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Levin, B.
    (1993) Verb classes and alternations: A preliminary investigation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. (2008) Dative verbs: A crosslinguistic perspective. Lingvisticæ Investigationes, 31, 285–312. 10.1075/li.31.2.12lev
    https://doi.org/10.1075/li.31.2.12lev [Google Scholar]
  34. Mazurkewich, I., & White, L.
    (1984) The acquisition of the dative alternation: Unlearning overgeneralizations. Cognition, 16(3), 261–283. 10.1016/0010‑0277(84)90030‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(84)90030-1 [Google Scholar]
  35. Michaelis, L. A.
    (2004) Type shifting in Construction Grammar: An integrated approach to aspectual coercion. Cognitive Linguistics, 15, 1–67. 10.1515/cogl.2004.001
    https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2004.001 [Google Scholar]
  36. Michaelis, L. A., & Ruppenhofer, J.
    (2001) Beyond alternations: A constructional model of the German applicative pattern. Stanford: CSLI Publications.
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Mukherjee, J.
    (2005) English ditransitive verbs: Aspects of theory, description and a usage-based model. Amsterdam: Rodopi.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Pinker, S.
    (1989) Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Pulvermüller, F.
    (1999) Words in the brain’s language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(02), 253–279. 10.1017/S0140525X9900182X
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X9900182X [Google Scholar]
  40. (2001) Brain reflections of words and their meaning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(12), 517–524. 10.1016/S1364‑6613(00)01803‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01803-9 [Google Scholar]
  41. Rappaport Hovav, M., & Levin, B.
    (1998) Building verb meanings. InM. Butt & W. Geuder (Eds.), The projection of arguments (pp.97–134). Stanford: CSLI Publications.
    [Google Scholar]
  42. (2008) The English dative alternation: The case for verb sensitivity. Journal of Linguistics, 44, 129–167.
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Richardson, D. C., Spivey, M. J., Barsalou, L. W., & McRae, K.
    (2003) Spatial representations activated during real-time comprehension of verbs. Cognitive Science, 27, 767–780. 10.1207/s15516709cog2705_4
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2705_4 [Google Scholar]
  44. Ruppenhofer, J. K.
    (2004) The interaction of valence and information structure. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Berkeley.
  45. Thompson, S.
    (1995) The iconicity of “dative shift” in English: Considerations from information flow in discourse. InM. Landsberg (Ed.), Syntactic iconicity and linguistic freezes: The human dimension (pp.155–175). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. 10.1515/9783110882926.155
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110882926.155 [Google Scholar]
  46. Wasow, T.
    (2002) Postverbal behavior. Stanford: CSLI Publications.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Zwaan, R. A.
    (2014) Embodiment and language comprehension: Reframing the discussion. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 229–234. 10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.008 [Google Scholar]
  48. Zwaan, R. A., & Taylor, L. J.
    (2006) Seeing, acting, understanding: Motor resonance in language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 1–11. 10.1037/0096‑3445.135.1.1
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.135.1.1 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/cf.00020.gou
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/cf.00020.gou
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error