1887
Volume 12, Issue 1
  • ISSN 2213-8722
  • E-ISSN: 2213-8730
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

The conceptualization of time is based on the physical experience in space (Vyvyan Evans 2004). Linguistically and psychologically, time is represented in two deictic spacetime metaphors: the ego-moving metaphor and the time-moving metaphor (Herbert H. Clark 1973). In the current research, two studies were conducted to investigate a novel relationship between taste and time. In Study 1, participants drank honey water and black coffee separately before interpreting a temporally ambiguous question. The results showed that whereas sweet taste tilted participants toward the ego-moving perspective in disambiguation, bitter taste slanted participants toward the time-moving perspective. To find out whether the influence of taste on time could be reversed, Study 2 primed participants with the ego-moving- or the time-moving-framed contextual statements before asking them to gauge the sweetness of honey water and the bitterness of black coffee independently. It was observed that in contrast to the ego-moving prime that prompted higher sweetness scores, the contrary prime begot higher bitterness ratings. Across the two studies, emotion was shown to correlate with taste and time perspective. Taken together, our findings buttress the embodied cognition theory by furnishing preliminary evidence that taste can affect and be affected by time. Implications for the interplay between sensation, emotion, culture, and temporal cognition are discussed.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/cogls.22015.qin
2025-06-02
2025-06-24
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Avery, J. A., Liu, A. G., Carrington, M., & Martin, A.
    (2022) Taste metaphors ground emotion concepts through the shared attribute of valence. Frontiers in Psychology, 131, 1–9. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.938663
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.938663 [Google Scholar]
  2. Banaruee, H., Khoshsima, H., Zare-Behtash, E., & Yarahmadzehi, N.
    (2019) Reasons behind using metaphor: A cognitive perspective on metaphoric Language. NeuroQuantology, 17(3), 108–113. 10.14704/nq.2019.17.3.2001
    https://doi.org/10.14704/nq.2019.17.3.2001 [Google Scholar]
  3. Barbosa Escobar, F., Velasco, C., Motoki, K., Byrne, D. V., & Wang, Q. J.
    (2021) The temperature of emotions. PLoS ONE, 16(6), e0252408. 10.1371/journal.pone.0252408
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0252408 [Google Scholar]
  4. Barsalou, L. W.
    (2008) Grounded Cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 591, 617–645. 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639 [Google Scholar]
  5. Bartlett, J. J., & Ruangjaroon, S.
    (2022) A war of words: Dissecting the foundational claims of CMT. Axiomathes, 321, 435–451. 10.1007/s10516‑021‑09612‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10516-021-09612-0 [Google Scholar]
  6. Bender, A., & Beller, S.
    (2014) Mapping spatial frames of reference onto time: A review of theoretical accounts and empirical findings. Cognition, 132(3), 342–382. 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.016
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.016 [Google Scholar]
  7. Bender, A., Beller, S., & Bennardo, G.
    (2010) Temporal frames of reference: Conceptual analysis and empirical evidence from German, English, Mandarin Chinese, and Tongan. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 10(3–4), 283–307. 10.1163/156853710X531195
    https://doi.org/10.1163/156853710X531195 [Google Scholar]
  8. Boroditsky, L.
    (2000) Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 1–28. 10.1016/S0010‑0277(99)00073‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00073-6 [Google Scholar]
  9. Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M.
    (2002) The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science, 13(2), 185–189. 10.1111/1467‑9280.00434
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00434 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bredie, W. L. P., Tan, H. S. G., & Wendin, K.
    (2014) A comparative study on facially expressed emotions in response to basic tastes. Chemsensory Perception, 71, 1–9. 10.1007/s12078‑014‑9163‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-014-9163-6 [Google Scholar]
  11. Chan, K. Q., Tong, E. M. W., Tan, D. H., & Koh, A. H. Q.
    (2013) What do love and jealousy taste like?. Emotion, 13(6), 1142–1149. 10.1037/a0033758
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033758 [Google Scholar]
  12. Chapman, H. A., Kim, D. A., Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K.
    (2009) In bad taste: Evidence for the oral origins of moral disgust. Science, 323(5918), 1222–1226. 10.1126/science.1165565
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1165565 [Google Scholar]
  13. Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A.
    (1999) Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(2), 215–224. 10.1177/0146167299025002007
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167299025002007 [Google Scholar]
  14. Clark, H. H.
    (1973) Space, time, semantics and the child. InT. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp.27–63). New York: Academic Press. 10.1016/B978‑0‑12‑505850‑6.50008‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-505850-6.50008-6 [Google Scholar]
  15. Crist, C. A., Duncan, S. E., Arnade, E. A., Leitch, K. A., O’Keefe, S. F., & Gallagher, D. L.
    (2018) Automated facial expression analysis for emotional responsivity using an aqueous bitter model. Food Quality and Preference, 681, 349–359. 10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.04.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.foodqual.2018.04.004 [Google Scholar]
  16. Danziger, S., Levav, J., & Avnaim-Pesso, L.
    (2011) Extraneous factors in judicial decisions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(17), 6889–6892. 10.1073/pnas.1018033108
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1018033108 [Google Scholar]
  17. Del Maschio, N., Fedeli, D., Garofalo, G., & Buccino, G.
    (2021) Evidence for the concreteness of abstract language: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Brain Sciences, 12(1), 32. 10.3390/brainsci12010032
    https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci12010032 [Google Scholar]
  18. Ding, Y., Ji, T.-T., & Chen, X.
    (2016) The way evaluation tastes: Tasting as an embodied cue of evaluation. Current Psychology, 351, 309–315. 10.1007/s12144‑014‑9295‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-014-9295-5 [Google Scholar]
  19. Duffy, S. E.
    (2014) The role of cultural artifacts in the interpretation of metaphorical expressions about time. Metaphor and Symbol, 29(2): 94–112. 10.1080/10926488.2014.889989
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2014.889989 [Google Scholar]
  20. Duffy, S. E., & Feist, M. I.
    (2014) Individual differences in the interpretation of ambiguous statements about time. Cognitive Linguistics, 25(1), 29–54. 10.1515/cog‑2013‑0030
    https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2013-0030 [Google Scholar]
  21. Duffy, S. E., Feist, M. I., & McCarthy, S.
    (2014) Moving through time: The role of personality in three real-life contexts. Cognitive Science, 38(8), 1662–1674. 10.1111/cogs.12124
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12124 [Google Scholar]
  22. Duffy, S. E., & Evans, V.
    (2016) The top trumps of time: Factors motivating the resolution of temporal ambiguity. Language and Cognition, 9(2), 293–315. 10.1017/langcog.2016.8
    https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2016.8 [Google Scholar]
  23. Duffy, S. E., & Feist, M. I.
    (2017) Power in time: The influence of power posing on metaphoric perspectives on time. Language and Cognition, 9(4), 637–647. 10.1017/langcog.2016.33
    https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2016.33 [Google Scholar]
  24. Elvevåg, B., Helsen, K., De Hert, M., Sweers, K., & Storms, G.
    (2011) Metaphor interpretation and use: A window into semantics in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 1331, 205–211. 10.1016/j.schres.2011.07.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2011.07.009 [Google Scholar]
  25. Eskine, K. J., Kacinik, N. A., & Prinz, J. J.
    (2011) A bad taste in the mouth: Gustatory disgust influences moral judgment. Psychological Science, 22(3), 295–299. 10.1177/0956797611398497
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611398497 [Google Scholar]
  26. Eskine, K. J., Kacinik, N. A., & Webster, G. D.
    (2012) The bitter truth about morality: Virtue, not vice, makes a bland beverage taste nice. PLoS ONE, 7(7), e41159. 10.1371/journal.pone.0041159
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041159 [Google Scholar]
  27. Evans, V.
    (2004) The structure of time: Language, meaning and temporal cognition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 10.1075/hcp.12
    https://doi.org/10.1075/hcp.12 [Google Scholar]
  28. Garg, N., & Lerner, J. S.
    (2013) Sadness and consumption. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 23(1), 106–113. 10.1016/j.jcps.2012.05.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcps.2012.05.009 [Google Scholar]
  29. Gayler, T., & Sas, C.
    (2017) An exploration of taste-emotion mappings from the perspective of food design practitioners. InMHFI 2017: Proceedings of the 2nd ACM SIGCHI International Workshop on Multisensory Approaches to Human-Food Interaction (pp.23–28). New York: Association for Computing Machinery. 10.1145/3141788.3141793
    https://doi.org/10.1145/3141788.3141793 [Google Scholar]
  30. Gayler, T., Sas, C., & Kalnikaitė, V.
    (2019) Taste your emotions: An exploration of the relationship between taste and emotional experience for HCI. InDIS’19: Proceedings of the 2019 on Designing Interactive Systems Conference (pp.1279–1291). New York: Association for Computing Machinery. 10.1145/3322276.3322336
    https://doi.org/10.1145/3322276.3322336 [Google Scholar]
  31. Gilead, M., Gal, O., Polak, M., & Cholow, Y.
    (2015) The role of nature and nurture in conceptual metaphors: The case of gustatory priming. Social Psychology, 46(3), 167–173. 10.1027/1864‑9335/a000238
    https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000238 [Google Scholar]
  32. Greimel, E., Macht, M., Krumhuber, E., & Ellgring, H.
    (2006) Facial and affective reactions to tastes and their modulation by sadness and joy. Physiology & Behavior, 89(2), 261–269. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.06.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2006.06.002 [Google Scholar]
  33. Harmon-Jones, E.
    (2007) Asymmetrical frontal cortical activity, affective valence, and motivational direction. InE. Harmon-Jones & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Social neuroscience: Integrating biological and psychological explanations of social behavior (pp.137–156). New York: The Guilford Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Hauser, D. J., Carter, M. S., & Meier, B. P.
    (2009) Mellow Monday and furious Friday: The approach-related link between anger and time representation. Cognition and Emotion, 23(6), 1166–1180. 10.1080/02699930802358424
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930802358424 [Google Scholar]
  35. Heath, T. P., Melichar, J. K., Nutt, D. J., & Donaldson, L. F.
    (2006) Human taste thresholds are modulated by serotonin and noradrenaline. The Journal of Neuroscience, 26(49): 12664–12671. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3459‑06.2006
    https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3459-06.2006 [Google Scholar]
  36. Higgins, E. T.
    (1997) Beyond pleasure and pain. American Psychologist, 52(12), 1280–1300. 10.1037/0003‑066X.52.12.1280
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.52.12.1280 [Google Scholar]
  37. Kaneko, D., Brouwer, A., Hogervorst, M., Toet, A., Kallen, V., & van Erp, J. B. F.
    (2020) Emotional state during tasting affects emotional experience differently and robustly for novel and familiar foods. Frontiers in Psychology, 111, 558172. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.558172
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.558172 [Google Scholar]
  38. Kashima, H., & Hayashi, N.
    (2011) Basic taste stimuli elicit unique responses in facial skin blood flow. PLoS ONE, 6(12): e28236. 10.1371/journal.pone.0028236
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0028236 [Google Scholar]
  39. Khatin-Zadeh, O., Eskandari, Z., Vahdat, S., & Banaruee, H.
    (2019) Why are motions effective in describing emotions. Polish Psychological Bulletin, 50(2), 119–124. 10.24425/ppb.2019.126027
    https://doi.org/10.24425/ppb.2019.126027 [Google Scholar]
  40. Kranjec, A., & Chatterjee, A.
    (2010) Are temporal concepts embodied? A challenge for cognitive neuroscience. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 240. 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00240
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00240 [Google Scholar]
  41. Krieglmeyer, R., Deutsch, R., De Houwer, J., & De Raedt, R.
    (2010) Being moved: Valence activates approach-avoidance behavior independently of evaluation and approach-avoidance intentions. Psychological Science, 21(4), 607–613. 10.1177/0956797610365131
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610365131 [Google Scholar]
  42. Lai, V. T. & Boroditsky, L.
    (2013) The immediate and chronic influence of spatio-temporal metaphors on the mental representations of time in English, Mandarin, and Mandarin-English speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 411421. 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00142
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00142 [Google Scholar]
  43. Lakoff, G.
    (2012) Explaining embodied cognition results. Topics in Cognitive Science, 4(4), 773–785. 10.1111/j.1756‑8765.2012.01222.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2012.01222.x [Google Scholar]
  44. Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M.
    (1980) Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. (1999) Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to western thought. New York: Basic Books.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Lang, P. J., & Bradley, M. M.
    (2013) Appetitive and defensive motivation: Goal-directed or goal-determined?. Emotion Review, 5(3), 230–234. 10.1177/1754073913477511
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913477511 [Google Scholar]
  47. Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N.
    (1997) International affective picture system (IAPS): Technical manual and affective ratings. NIMH Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention, Gainesville, 39–58.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Lee, A., & Ji, L.-J.
    (2014) Moving away from a bad past and toward a good future: Feelings influence the metaphorical understanding of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 21–26. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/a0032233. 10.1037/a0032233
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032233 [Google Scholar]
  49. Lee, S. W. S., & Schwarz, N.
    (2012) Bidirectionality, mediation, and moderation of metaphorical effects: The embodiment of social suspicion and fishy smells. Journal of personality and social psychology, 103(5): 737–749. 10.1037/a0029708
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029708 [Google Scholar]
  50. Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., & Weber, E. U.
    (2013) The financial costs of sadness. Psychological Science, 24(1), 72–79. 10.1177/0956797612450302
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612450302 [Google Scholar]
  51. Lerner, J. S., Small, D. A., & Loewenstein, G.
    (2004) Heart strings and purse strings: Carryover effects of emotions on economic decisions. Psychological Science, 15(5), 337–341. 10.1111/j.0956‑7976.2004.00679.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00679.x [Google Scholar]
  52. Li, H.
    (2020) Will it really happen? Disambiguating of the hypothetical and real “next wednesday’s meeting” question in Mandarin speakers. Lingua, 2371, 102806. 10.1016/j.lingua.2020.102806
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2020.102806 [Google Scholar]
  53. Li, H., & Cao, Y.
    (2018) 时间焦点对前后方向上内隐时空映射的影响——来自汉族和羌族的证据[The influence of temporal focus on implicit space-time mappings on the front-back axis: Evidence from Han and Qiang Chinese]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 50(10), 1083–1093. 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01083
    https://doi.org/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2018.01083 [Google Scholar]
  54. Liang, P., Jiang, J., Chen, J., & Wei, L.
    (2021) Affective face processing modified by different tastes. Frontiers in Psychology, 121, 644704. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.644704
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.644704 [Google Scholar]
  55. Loermans, A. C., & Milfont, T. L.
    (2018) Time after time: A short-term longitudinal examination of the ego- and time-moving representations. Journal of Research in Personality, 741, 1–5. 10.1016/j.jrp.2017.12.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2017.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  56. Loermans, A. C., de Koning, B., & Krabbendam, L.
    (2019) Agency and time representation in English and Dutch speakers. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 38(3): 353–375. 10.1177/0261927X18824619
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X18824619 [Google Scholar]
  57. Margolies, S. O., & Crawford, L. E.
    (2008) Event valance and spatial metaphors of time. Cognition and Emotion, 22(7), 1401–1414. 10.1080/02699930701810335
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701810335 [Google Scholar]
  58. Marmolejo-Ramos, F., Correa, J. C., Sakarkar, G., Ngo, G., Ruiz-Fernández, S., Butcher, N., & Yamada, Y.
    (2017) Placing joy, surprise and sadness in space: A cross-linguistic study. Psychological Research, 811, 750–763. 10.1007/s00426‑016‑0787‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-016-0787-9 [Google Scholar]
  59. Matlock, T.
    (2004) Fictive motion as cognitive simulation. Memory & Cognition, 321, 1389–1400. 10.3758/BF03206329
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206329 [Google Scholar]
  60. Matlock, T., Holmes, K. J., Srinivasan, M., & Ramscar, M.
    (2011) Even abstract motion influences the understanding of time. Metaphor and Symbol, 26(4), 260–271. 10.1080/10926488.2011.609065
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2011.609065 [Google Scholar]
  61. Matlock, T., Ramscar, M., & Boroditsky, L.
    (2005) On the experiential link between spatial and temporal language. Cognitive Science, 29(4), 655–664. 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_17
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog0000_17 [Google Scholar]
  62. McGlone, M. S., & Harding, J. L.
    (1998) Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(5), 1211–1223. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0278-7393.24.5.1211. 10.1037/0278‑7393.24.5.1211
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.24.5.1211 [Google Scholar]
  63. McGlone, M. S., & Pfiester, R. A.
    (2009) Does time fly when you’re having fun, or do you?: Affect, agency, and embodiment in temporal communication. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 28(1), 3–31. 10.1177/0261927X08325744
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X08325744 [Google Scholar]
  64. Mihafu, F. D., Issa, J. Y., & Kamiyango, M. W.
    (2020) Implication of sensory evaluation and quality assessment in food product development: A review. Current Research in Nutrition and Food Science, 8(3): 690–702. 10.12944/CRNFSJ.8.3.03
    https://doi.org/10.12944/CRNFSJ.8.3.03 [Google Scholar]
  65. Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Moll, F. T., Ignácio, F. A., Bramati, I. E., Caparelli-Dáquer, E. M., & Eslinger, P. J.
    (2005) The moral affiliations of disgust: A functional MRI study. Cognitive and Behavioral Neurology, 18(1), 68–78. 10.1097/01.wnn.0000152236.46475.a7
    https://doi.org/10.1097/01.wnn.0000152236.46475.a7 [Google Scholar]
  66. Moseley, R., Carota, F., Hauk, O., Mohr, B., & Pulvermüller, F.
    (2012) A role for the motor system in binding abstract emotional meaning. Cerebral Cortex, 22(7), 1634–1647. 10.1093/cercor/bhr238
    https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr238 [Google Scholar]
  67. Niedenthal, P. M., Winkielman, P., Mondillon, L., & Vermeulen, N.
    (2009) Embodiment of emotion concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(6): 1120–1136. 10.1037/a0015574
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015574 [Google Scholar]
  68. Noel, C., & Dando, R.
    (2015) The effect of emotional state on taste perception. Appetite, 95(1), 89–95. 10.1016/j.appet.2015.06.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.06.003 [Google Scholar]
  69. Ostarek, M., & Huettig, F.
    (2019) Six challenges for embodiment research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(6): 593–599. 10.1177/0963721419866441
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721419866441 [Google Scholar]
  70. Ostarek, M., & Bottini, R.
    (2021) Towards strong inference in research on embodiment — Possibilities and limitations of causal paradigms. Journal of Cognition, 4(1), 5. 10.5334/joc.139
    https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.139 [Google Scholar]
  71. Payne, T., Kronenbuerger, M., & Wong, G. Gustatory Testing
    (2022) In: StatPearls [Internet]. Treasure Island (FL): StatPearls Publishing; 2022. Accessed on2022 Jan 24. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK567734/
  72. Poggi, I., & D’Errico, F.
    (2010) The mental ingredients of bitterness. Journal on Multimodal User Interfaces, 31, 79–86. 10.1007/s12193‑009‑0021‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s12193-009-0021-9 [Google Scholar]
  73. Ren, D., Tan, K., Arriaga, X. B., & Chan, K. Q.
    (2015) Sweet love: The effects of sweet taste experience on romantic perceptions. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 32(7), 905–921. 10.1177/0265407514554512
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0265407514554512 [Google Scholar]
  74. Richmond, J., Wilson, J. C., & Zinken, J.
    (2012) A feeling for the future: How does agency in time metaphors relate to feelings?. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42(7), 813–823. 10.1002/ejsp.1906
    https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.1906 [Google Scholar]
  75. Ritchie, L. D.
    (2006) Context and connection in metaphor. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan. 10.1057/9780230286825
    https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230286825 [Google Scholar]
  76. Robin, O., Rousmans, S., Dittmar, A., & Vernet-Maury, E.
    (2003) Gender influence on emotional responses to primary tastes. Physiology & behavior, 78(3), 385–393. 10.1016/S0031‑9384(02)00981‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(02)00981-2 [Google Scholar]
  77. Rothe-Wulf, A., Beller, S., & Bender, A.
    (2015) Temporal frames of reference in three Germanic languages: Individual consistency, interindividual consensus, and cross-linguistic variability. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(5), 917–939. 10.1080/17470218.2014.970205
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2014.970205 [Google Scholar]
  78. Rousmans, S., Robin, O., Dittmar, A., & Vernet-Maury, E.
    (2000) Automatic nervous system responses associated with primary tastes. Chemical Senses, 25(6), 709–718. 10.1093/chemse/25.6.709
    https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/25.6.709 [Google Scholar]
  79. Rucker, D. D., & Petty, R. E.
    (2004) Emotion specificity and consumer behavior: Anger, sadness, and preference for activity. Motivation and Emotion, 281, 3–21. 10.1023/B:MOEM.0000027275.95071.82
    https://doi.org/10.1023/B:MOEM.0000027275.95071.82 [Google Scholar]
  80. Ruscher, J. B.
    (2011) Moving forward: The effect of spatiotemporal metaphors on perceptions about grief. Social Psychology, 42(3), 225–230. 10.1027/1864‑9335/a000066
    https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000066 [Google Scholar]
  81. Russell, J. A.
    (2003) Core affect and the psychological construction of emotion. Psychological Review, 110(1): 145–172. 10.1037/0033‑295X.110.1.145
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.145 [Google Scholar]
  82. Sagioglou, C., & Greitemeyer, T.
    (2014) Bitter taste causes hostility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(12): 1589–1597. 10.1177/0146167214552792
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167214552792 [Google Scholar]
  83. Schienle, A., Osmani, F., & Schlintl, C.
    (2020) Disgust propensity and the bitter aftertaste response. Chemosensory Perception, 141, 57–63. 10.1007/s12078‑020‑09283‑y
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s12078-020-09283-y [Google Scholar]
  84. Spence, C.
    (2022) What is the link between personality and food behavior?. Current Research in Food Science, 51: 19–27. 10.1016/j.crfs.2021.12.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.crfs.2021.12.001 [Google Scholar]
  85. Spinelli, S.
    (2021) Emotions elicited by foods. InH. L. Meiselman (Ed.), Emotion measurement (2nd edition) (pp.707–730). Sawston: Woodhead Publishing. 10.1016/B978‑0‑12‑821124‑3.00022‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-821124-3.00022-3 [Google Scholar]
  86. Spinelli, S., & Jaeger, S. R.
    (2019) What do we know about the sensory drivers of emotions in foods and beverages?. Current Opinion in Food Science, 271, 82–89. 10.1016/j.cofs.2019.06.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cofs.2019.06.007 [Google Scholar]
  87. Stocker, K., & Hartmann, M.
    (2019) Next Wednesday’s meeting has been moved forward two days. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 78(1–2): 61–67. 10.1024/1421‑0185/a000220
    https://doi.org/10.1024/1421-0185/a000220 [Google Scholar]
  88. van der Wal, R. C., & van Dillen, L. F.
    (2013) Leaving a flat taste in your mouth: Task load reduces taste perception. Psychological Science, 24(7), 1277–1284. 10.1177/0956797612471953
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612471953 [Google Scholar]
  89. Vigliocco, G., Kousta, S., Della Rosa, P. A., Vinson, D. P., Tettamanti, M., Devlin, J. T., & Cappa, S. F.
    (2014) The neural representation of abstract words: The role of emotion. Cerebral Cortex, 24(7), 1767–1777. 10.1093/cercor/bht025
    https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bht025 [Google Scholar]
  90. Wang, L., Chen, Q., Chen, Y., & Zhong, R.
    (2019) The effect of sweet taste on romantic semantic processing: An ERP study. Frontiers in Psychology, 101,1573. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01573
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01573 [Google Scholar]
  91. Wang, Q. J., & Spence, C.
    (2016) ‘Striking a sour note’: Assessing the influence of consonant and dissonant music on taste perception. Multisensory Research, 29(1–3): 195–208. 10.1163/22134808‑00002505
    https://doi.org/10.1163/22134808-00002505 [Google Scholar]
  92. Wang, Q. J., Wang, S., & Spence, C.
    (2016) “Turn up the taste”: Assessing the role of taste intensity and emotion in mediating crossmodal correspondences between basic tastes and pitch. Chemical Senses, 41(4): 345–356. 10.1093/chemse/bjw007
    https://doi.org/10.1093/chemse/bjw007 [Google Scholar]
  93. Wang, Q. J., & Spence, C.
    (2018) Assessing the influence of music on wine perception among wine professionals. Food Science & Nutrition, 61: 295–301. 10.1002/fsn3.554
    https://doi.org/10.1002/fsn3.554 [Google Scholar]
  94. Xu, J., Wan, F., & Schwarz, N.
    (2020) “That’s bitter!”: Culture-specific effects of gustatory experience on judgments of fairness and advancement. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 10.1037/xge0000985
    https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000985 [Google Scholar]
  95. Xu, T., Liu, M., & Wang, X.
    (2022) How humor is experienced: An embodied metaphor account. Current Psychology, 1–13.
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Zhang, X., Li, Y., Chao, X., & Li, Y.
    (2022) Sourness impacts envy and jealousy in Chinese culture. Psychological Research, 891, 96–107. 10.1007/s00426‑022‑01652‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-022-01652-4 [Google Scholar]
  97. Zheng, W., Liu, Y., Liu, C., Chen, Y.-H., Cui, Q., & Fu, X.
    (2019) The influence of event valence and emotional states on the metaphorical comprehension of time. Frontiers in Psychology, 101, 410. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00410
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00410 [Google Scholar]
  98. Zhou, Y., & Tse, C.
    (2020) The taste of emotion: Metaphoric association between taste words and emotion/emotion-laden words. Frontiers in Psychology, 111, 986. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00986
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00986 [Google Scholar]
  99. (2021) Sweet taste brings happiness, but happiness does not taste sweet: The unidirectionality of taste-emotion metaphoric association. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 341, 339–361. 10.1080/20445911.2021.2020797
    https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2021.2020797 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/cogls.22015.qin
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/cogls.22015.qin
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): culture; embodied cognition; emotion; spacetime metaphors; taste
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error