1887
Volume 4, Issue 1
  • ISSN 2589-1588
  • E-ISSN: 2589-1596
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Has language developed to serve as a system of communication or one of thought? While language is clearly an excellent tool for communication, the possible contribution of higher order cognitive processes other than language to communication may provide insights on how we think about language evolution. Studies show that bilingualism improves communication skills, possibly due to boosting domain general processes, thus suggesting a divide between communication and formal language. However, to date little attention has been paid to bilingual atypical child populations presenting asymmetric developmental delays in language and communication skills. In this study, we explore the language-communication divide in bilingual and monolingual children diagnosed with High Functioning Autism (HFA), displaying primary communication difficulties, and those with Specific Language Impairment (SLI), displaying primary language difficulties. Children were tested on language, cognitive control, Theory of Mind (ToM), social comprehension and verbal communication. Results demonstrate that bilingual children with HFA exhibit enhanced cognitive control, ToM and social comprehension, which in turn improve their verbal communication as compared to their monolingual peers with HFA. On the other hand, no boost emerges in bilingual children with SLI, be it in cognitive control or ToM, though a weaker advantage (relative to the children with ASD) can be observed in their verbal communication skills, with significant mediation from social comprehension. The overall evidence suggests that language, though useful for communication, is not sufficient for communication, and may arguably not have evolved primarily for communicative purposes.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/elt.00037.dur
2022-09-29
2024-10-11
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Alloway, T. P., & Gathercole, S. E.
    (2005) Working memory and short-term sentence recall in young children. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 17(2), 207–220. 10.1080/09541440440000005
    https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440440000005 [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force
    American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5™ (5th ed.). American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.. 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
    https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 [Google Scholar]
  3. Andreou, M.
    (2015) The effects of bilingualism on verbal and non verbal cognition: the micro- and macro-structure of narratives in the weak and the dominant language of the bilingual child. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Andrés-Roqueta, C., & Katsos, N.
    (2017) The Contribution of Grammar, Vocabulary and Theory of Mind in Pragmatic Language Competence in Children with Autistic Spectrum Disorders. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 996. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00996
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00996 [Google Scholar]
  5. Archibald, L. M., 7 Gathercole, S. E.
    (2006) Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 41(6), 675–93. 10.1080/13682820500442602
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820500442602 [Google Scholar]
  6. Asoulin, E.
    (2016) Language as an instrument of thought. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 1(1), 1–23. 10.5334/gjgl.34
    https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.34 [Google Scholar]
  7. Atonius, J.
    (2018) The Analysis of Illocutionary Acts in Narrative Text of Eleventh Grade Students at SMA Negeri 5 Medan. Doctoral dissertation, Nommensen HKBP University, Indonesia.
  8. Auza-Benavides, A., Peñaloza, C., & Murata, C.
    (2019) The influence of maternal education on the linguistic abilities of monolingual Spanish-speaking children with and without Specific Language Impairment. InE. Aguilar-Mediavilla, L. Buil-Legaz, R. López-Penadés, V. A. Sanchez-Azanza, & D. Adrover-Roig (Eds.), The Atypical Language Development in Romance (pp.93–112). Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. 10.1075/z.223.06ale
    https://doi.org/10.1075/z.223.06ale [Google Scholar]
  9. Bal, V. H., Wilkinson, E., White, L. C., Law, J. K., THE SPARK Consortium, Feliciano, P., & Chung, W. K.
    (2021) Early Pandemic Experiences of Autistic Adults: Predictors of Psychological Distress. Autism Research, 14(6), 1209–1219. 10.1002/aur.2480
    https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2480 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bang, J., & Nadig, A.
    (2015) Learning Language in Autism: Maternal Linguistic Input Contributes to Later Vocabulary. Autism Research, 8(2), 214–223. 10.1002/aur.1440
    https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1440 [Google Scholar]
  11. Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A.
    (1986) The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51(6), 1173–1182. 10.1037/0022‑3514.51.6.1173
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.51.6.1173 [Google Scholar]
  12. Baron-Cohen, S.
    (1995) Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. The MIT Press. 10.7551/mitpress/4635.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/4635.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  13. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U.
    (1985) Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind?”Cognition, 21, 37–46. 10.1016/0010‑0277(85)90022‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8 [Google Scholar]
  14. Bedore, L. M., & Peña, E. D.
    (2008) Assessment of Bilingual Children for Identification of Language Impairment: Current Findings and Implications for Practice. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11(1), 1–29. 10.2167/beb392.0
    https://doi.org/10.2167/beb392.0 [Google Scholar]
  15. Bedore, L. M., Peña, E. D., García, M., Cortez, C.
    (2005) Conceptual versus monolingual scoring: When does it make a difference?Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 36(3), 188–200. 10.1044/0161‑1461(2005/020)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2005/020) [Google Scholar]
  16. Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K.
    (2006) Seeing it differently: visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(6), 258–264. 10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.001 [Google Scholar]
  17. Berwick, R. C., & Chomsky, N.
    (2016) Why Only us. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 10.7551/mitpress/9780262034241.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262034241.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  18. Bialystok, E.
    (1999) Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70(3), 636–644. 10.1111/1467‑8624.00046
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00046 [Google Scholar]
  19. (2011) Reshaping the mind: the benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 229–235. 10.1037/a0025406
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025406 [Google Scholar]
  20. (2015) Bilingualism and the development of executive function: The role of attention. Child development perspectives, 9(2), 117–121. 10.1111/cdep.12116
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12116 [Google Scholar]
  21. Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G.
    (2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 4, 859–873.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Bialystok, E., Poarch, G., Luo, L., & Craik, F. I. M.
    (2014) Effects of bilingualism and aging on executive function and working memory. Psychology and aging, 29(3), 696–705. 10.1037/a0037254
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037254 [Google Scholar]
  23. Bialystok, E., & Senman, L.
    (2004) Executive Processes in Appearance-Reality Tasks: The Role of Inhibition of Attention and Symbolic Representation. Child Development, 75(2), 562–579. 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2004.00693.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00693.x [Google Scholar]
  24. Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M.
    (2009) Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112(3), 494–500. 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014 [Google Scholar]
  25. Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F.
    (2005) Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology: I: Generativity. Autism, 9(1), 7–27. 10.1177/1362361305049027
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361305049027 [Google Scholar]
  26. Bishop, D. V. M., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., Greenhalgh, T., & the catalise consortium
    (2017) CATALISE: a multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development. Phase 2. Terminology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58, 1068–1080. 10.1111/jcpp.12721
    https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12721 [Google Scholar]
  27. Blom, E., & Boerma, T.
    (2016) Why do children with language impairment have difficulties with narrative macrostructure?Research in Developmental Disabilities, 55, 301–11. 10.1016/j.ridd.2016.05.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2016.05.001 [Google Scholar]
  28. Bloom, P.
    (2000) How children learn the meaning of words. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. 10.7551/mitpress/3577.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/3577.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  29. Bloom, L., & Lahey, M.
    (1978) Language development and language disorders. New York: Wiley.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Boerma, T., & Blom, E.
    (2020) Effects of developmental language disorder and bilingualism on children’s executive functioning: A longitudinal study. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 107, 103782. 10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103782
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103782 [Google Scholar]
  31. Boerma, E., Leseman, P., Timmermeister, M., Wijnen, F. & Blom, E.
    (2016) Narrative abilities of monolingual and bilingual children with and without language impairment: implications for clinical practice. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 51(6), 626–638. 10.1111/1460‑6984.12234
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12234 [Google Scholar]
  32. Bohnacker, U., & Gagarina, N.
    (2019) Background on MAIN –Revised, how to use it and adapt it to other languages. ZAS Papers in Linguistics, 63, iv–xii.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Bolhuis, J. J., Tattersall, I., Chomsky, N., & Berwick, R. C.
    (2014) How Could Language Have Evolved?PLoS Biology, 12(8), e1001934. 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001934
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001934 [Google Scholar]
  34. Botting, N., & Conti-Ramsden, G.
    (2001) Non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 36(4), 421–432. 10.1080/13682820110074971
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820110074971 [Google Scholar]
  35. (2003) Autism, primary pragmatic difficulties, and specific language impairment: can we distinguish them using psycholinguistic markers?Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45(8), 515–524. 10.1111/j.1469‑8749.2003.tb00951.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2003.tb00951.x [Google Scholar]
  36. Bouvet, L., Rousset, S., Valdois, S., & Donnadieu, S.
    (2011) Global precedence effect in audition and vision: evidence for similar cognitive styles across modalities. Acta Psychologica, 138(2), 329–335. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.08.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.08.004 [Google Scholar]
  37. Brock, J., Norbury, C., Einav, S. & Nation, K.
    (2008) Do individuals with autism process words in context?Cognition, 108, 896–904. 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.007 [Google Scholar]
  38. Brown-Schmidt, S.
    (2009) The role of executive function in perspective taking during online language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 893–900. 10.3758/PBR.16.5.893
    https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.16.5.893 [Google Scholar]
  39. Bruyer, R., & Scailquin, J. C.
    (2000) The fate of global precedence with age. Experimental Aging Research, 26(4), 285–314. 10.1080/036107300750015705
    https://doi.org/10.1080/036107300750015705 [Google Scholar]
  40. Carlson, S. M., & Meltzoff, A. N.
    (2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282–298. 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2008.00675.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x [Google Scholar]
  41. Charman, T., Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Cox, A., Baird, G., & Drew, A.
    (1997) Infants with autism: an investigation of empathy, pretend play, joint attention, and imitation. Developmental Psychology, 33(5), 781–789. 10.1037/0012‑1649.33.5.781
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.5.781 [Google Scholar]
  42. Cheney, D. L., & Seyfarth, R. M.
    (2005) Constraints and preadaptations in the earliest stages of language evolution. The Linguistic Review, 22(2–4), 135–159. 10.1515/tlir.2005.22.2‑4.135
    https://doi.org/10.1515/tlir.2005.22.2-4.135 [Google Scholar]
  43. Chondrogianni, V., Andreou, M., Nerantzini, M., Varlokosta, S., & Tsimpli, I. M.
    (2013) The Greek Sentence Repetition Task. COST Action IS0804.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Conti-Ramsden, G., Simkin, Z., & Botting, N.
    (2006) The prevalence of autistic spectrum disorders in adolescents with a history of specific language impairment (SLI). The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(6), 621–628. 10.1111/j.1469‑7610.2005.01584.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01584.x [Google Scholar]
  45. Cormack, A., & Smith, N.
    (2002) Compositionality, copy theory, and control. University College London Working Papers in Linguistics, 14, 355–373.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J.
    (1997) The Modular Nature of Human intelligence. InA. Scheibel & J. Schopf (Eds.), The Origin and Evolution of Intelligence (pp.71–101). Jones and Bartlett publishers, Sudbury, MA.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Dahlgren, S., & Sandberg, A. D.
    (2008) Referential communication in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 12(4), 335–348. 10.1177/1362361308091648
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361308091648 [Google Scholar]
  48. Davies, C., Andrés-Roqueta, C., & Norbury, C. F.
    (2016) Referring expressions and structural language abilities in children with specific language impairment: A pragmatic tolerance account. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 144, 98–113. 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.011
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.011 [Google Scholar]
  49. Dawson, G., & Fernald, M.
    (1987) Perspective-taking ability and its relationship to the social behavior of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 17, 487–498. 10.1007/BF01486965
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01486965 [Google Scholar]
  50. De Cat, C.
    (2013) Egocentric definiteness errors and perspective evaluation in preschool children. Journal of Pragmatics, 56, 58–69. 10.1016/j.pragma.2012.08.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2012.08.002 [Google Scholar]
  51. De Houwer, A., Bornstein, M. H., & Putnick, D. L.
    (2014) A bilingual-monolingual comparison of young children’s vocabulary size: Evidence from comprehension and production. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(6), 1189–1211. 10.1017/S0142716412000744
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000744 [Google Scholar]
  52. Demetriou, E. A., Lampit, A., Quintana, D. S., Naismith, S. L., Song, Y., Pye, J. E., Hickie, I., & Guastella, A. J.
    (2018) Autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of executive function. Molecular psychiatry, 23(5), 1198–1204. 10.1038/mp.2017.75
    https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.75 [Google Scholar]
  53. Díaz, V.
    (2021) Minds in action: Evidence that linguistic diversity helps children build a theory of mind. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1–11.
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Diaz, V., & Farrar, M. J.
    (2017) The missing explanation of the false-belief advantage in bilingual children: a longitudinal study. Developmental Science, 21(4), e12594. 10.1111/desc.12594
    https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12594 [Google Scholar]
  55. Dukette, D., & Stiles, J.
    (1996) Children’s analysis of hierarchical patterns: Evidence from a similarity judgment task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 63, 103–140. 10.1006/jecp.1996.0044
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.0044 [Google Scholar]
  56. Durrleman, S., Baldimtsi, E., Peristeri, E., & Tsimpli, I. M.
    (to appear). Bilingualism matters: a study of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder and Developmental Language Disorder. InK. Grohmann Ed. Multifaceted Multilingualism: Language, Cognition, and Communication (pp.xx–xx). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Engel de Abreu, P. M., Cruz-Santos, A., & Puglisi, M. L.
    (2014) Specific language impairment in language-minority children from low-income families. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49(6), 736–747. 10.1111/1460‑6984.12107
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12107 [Google Scholar]
  58. Fan, S. P., Liberman, Z., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D.
    (2015) The exposure advantage: Early exposure to a multilingual environment promotes effective communication. Psychological Science, 26, 1090–1097. 10.1177/0956797615574699
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615574699 [Google Scholar]
  59. Farhadian, M., Abdullah, R., Mansor, M., Redzuan, M., Gazanizadand, N., & Kumar, V.
    (2010) Theory of mind in bilingual and monolingual preschool children. The Journal of Psychology, 1, 39–46. 10.1080/09764224.2010.11885444
    https://doi.org/10.1080/09764224.2010.11885444 [Google Scholar]
  60. Fisher, N., Happé, F., & Dunn, J.
    (2005) The relationship between vocabulary, grammar, and false belief task performance in children with autistic spectrum disorders and children with moderate learning difficulties. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(4), 409–419. 10.1111/j.1469‑7610.2004.00371.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00371.x [Google Scholar]
  61. Fleckstein, A., Prévost, P., Tuller, L., Sizaret, E., & Zebib, R.
    (2018) How to identify SLI in bilingual children: a study on sentence repetition in French. Language Acquisition, 25, 85–101. 10.1080/10489223.2016.1192635
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2016.1192635 [Google Scholar]
  62. Fodor, J. A.
    (2008) LOT 2: The Language of Thought Revisited: The Language of Thought Revisited. Oxford, Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199548774.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199548774.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  63. Frith, U.
    (1989) Autism: explaining the enigma. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  64. (2012) Why we need cognitive explanations of autism. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 65(11), 2073–2092. 10.1080/17470218.2012.697178
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2012.697178 [Google Scholar]
  65. Frith, C., Frith, U.
    (2012) Mechanisms of social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 287–313. 10.1146/annurev‑psych‑120710‑100449
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100449 [Google Scholar]
  66. Frith, U., & Happé, F.
    (1994a) Autism: beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition, 50(1–3), 115–132. 10.1016/0010‑0277(94)90024‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90024-8 [Google Scholar]
  67. Frith, U., Happé, F.
    (1994b) Language and communication in autistic disorders. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1994 Oct29;346(1315):97–104. 10.1098/rstb.1994.0133
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1994.0133 [Google Scholar]
  68. Gagarina, N., Klop, D., Kunnari, S., Tantele, K., Välimaa, T., Balčiūnienė, I., Bohnacker, U., & Walters, J.
    (2012) MAIN: Multilingual assessment instrument for narratives. ZAS Papers in Linguistics, 56, 1–140.
    [Google Scholar]
  69. (2015) Assessment of narrative abilities in bilingual children. InArmon-Lotem, S., Jong, J. D. & Meir, N. (Eds.), Assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment (pp.243–276). Bristol: Multilingual Matters. 10.21832/9781783093137‑011
    https://doi.org/10.21832/9781783093137-011 [Google Scholar]
  70. Gagarina, N., Klop, D., Kunnari, S., Tantele, K., Välimaa, T., Bohnacker, U., & Walters, J.
    (2019) MAIN: Multilingual Assessment Instrument for Narratives. Revised version. ZAS Papers in Linguistics, 63. 10.21248/zaspil.63.2019.516
    https://doi.org/10.21248/zaspil.63.2019.516 [Google Scholar]
  71. Garfield, J. L., Peterson, C. C., & Perry, T.
    (2001) Social cognition, language acquisition and the development of the theory of mind. Mind & Language, 16(5), 494–541. 10.1111/1468‑0017.00180
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1468-0017.00180 [Google Scholar]
  72. Georgas, J., Paraskevopoulos, I. N., Besevegis, E., Giannitsas, N., & Mylonas, K.
    (2003) Ελληνικό WISC-III: Wechsler κλίμακες νοημοσύνης για παιδιά [Greek WISC-III: Wechsler intelligence scale for children]. Athens: Ellinika Grammata.
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Goetz, P.
    (2003) The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6, 1–15. 10.1017/S1366728903001007
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728903001007 [Google Scholar]
  74. Gonzalez-Barrero, A. M., & Nadig, A. S.
    (2017) Can Bilingualism Mitigate Set-Shifting Difficulties in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders?Child Development, 90(4), 1043–1060. 10.1111/cdev.12979
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12979 [Google Scholar]
  75. Grandgeorge, M., Hausberger, M., Tordjman, S., Deleau, M., Lazartigues, A., & Lemonnier, E.
    (2009) Environmental Factors Influence Language Development in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. PLoS ONE, 4(4), e4683. 10.1371/journal.pone.0004683
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004683 [Google Scholar]
  76. Gray, S.
    (2006) The Relationship Between Phonological Memory, Receptive Vocabulary, and Fast Mapping in Young Children With Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(5), 955–969. 10.1044/1092‑4388(2006/069)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/069) [Google Scholar]
  77. Greenberg, A., Bellana, B., & Bialystok, E.
    (2013) Perspective-taking ability in bilingual children: Extending advantages in executive control to spatial reasoning. Cognitive development, 28(1), 41–50. 10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.10.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.10.002 [Google Scholar]
  78. Gross, M., Buac, M., & Kaushanskaya, M.
    (2014) Conceptual scoring of receptive and expressive vocabulary measures in simultaneous and sequential bilingual children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 23(4), 574–586. 10.1044/2014_AJSLP‑13‑0026
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_AJSLP-13-0026 [Google Scholar]
  79. Guy, M. W., Reynolds, G. D., & Zhang, D.
    (2013) Visual attention to global and local stimulus properties in six-month-old infants: Individual differences and event-related potentials. Child Development, 84, 1392–1406. 10.1111/cdev.12053
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12053 [Google Scholar]
  80. Han, S., & Lee, K.
    (2013) Cognitive and affective perspective-taking ability of young bilinguals in South Korea. Child Studies in Asia-Pacific Contexts, 3(1), 69–80. 10.5723/csdc.2013.3.1.069
    https://doi.org/10.5723/csdc.2013.3.1.069 [Google Scholar]
  81. Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T.
    (2002) The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?Science, 298(5598), 1569–1579. 10.1126/science.298.5598.1569
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.298.5598.1569 [Google Scholar]
  82. Hudry, K., Leadbitter, K., Temple, K., Slonims, V., McConachie, H., Aldred, C., Howlin, P., Charman, T., & the PACT Consortium
    (2010) Preschoolers with autism show greater impairment in receptive compared with expressive language abilities. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 45(6), 681–690. 10.3109/13682820903461493
    https://doi.org/10.3109/13682820903461493 [Google Scholar]
  83. Ikizer, E., & Ramirez-Esparza, N.
    (2018) Bilinguals’ social flexibility. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(5), 957–969. 10.1017/S1366728917000414
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000414 [Google Scholar]
  84. Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V.
    (2007) Integrating simulation and theory of mind: From self to social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(5), 194–196. 10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.02.002 [Google Scholar]
  85. Kimchi, R., Hadad, B., Behrmann, M., & Palmer, S. E.
    (2005) Microgenesis and ontogenesis of perceptual organization: evidence from global and local processing of hierarchical patterns. Psychological Science, 16(4), 282–290. 10.1111/j.0956‑7976.2005.01529.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01529.x [Google Scholar]
  86. Kimura, D.
    (1993) Neuromotor mechanisms in human communication. Oxford University Press, Oxford. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195054927.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195054927.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  87. Kissine, M., Luffin, X., Aiad, F., Bourourou, R., Deliens, G., & Gaddour, N.
    (2019) Noncolloquial Arabic in Tunisian Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Possible Instance of Language Acquisition in a Noninteractive Context. Language Learning, 69(1), 44–70. 10.1111/lang.12312
    https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12312 [Google Scholar]
  88. Kovács, A. M.
    (2009) Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false-belief reasoning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 48–54. 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2008.00742.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00742.x [Google Scholar]
  89. Kover, S. T., McDuffie, A. S., Hagerman, R. J., & Abbeduto, L.
    (2013) Receptive vocabulary in boys with autism spectrum disorder: cross-sectional developmental trajectories. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(11), 2696–709. 10.1007/s10803‑013‑1823‑x
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1823-x [Google Scholar]
  90. Koyama, T., Tachimori, H., Osada, H., Takeda, T., & Kurita, H.
    (2007) Cognitive and symptom profiles in Asperger’s syndrome and high-functioning autism. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 61, 99–104. 10.1111/j.1440‑1819.2007.01617.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01617.x [Google Scholar]
  91. Krizman, J., Marian, V., Shook, A., Skoe, E., & Kraus, N.
    (2012) Subcortical encoding of sound is enhanced in bilinguals and relates to executive function advantages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109 (20) 7877–7881. 10.1073/pnas.1201575109
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1201575109 [Google Scholar]
  92. Leonard, L. B.
    (2014) Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 10.7551/mitpress/9152.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9152.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  93. Leonard, L. B., Eyer, J. A., Bedore, L. M., & Grela, B. G.
    (1997) Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme difficulties of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40 (4), 741–753. 10.1044/jslhr.4004.741
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4004.741 [Google Scholar]
  94. Liberman, Z., Woodward, A. L., Keysar, B., & Kinzler, K. D.
    (2017) Exposure to multiple languages enhances communication skills in infancy. Developmental science, 20(1), 10.1111/desc.12420
    https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12420 [Google Scholar]
  95. Losh, M., & Capps, L.
    (2003) Narrative ability in high-functioning children with autism or Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(3), 239–251. 10.1023/A:1024446215446
    https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1024446215446 [Google Scholar]
  96. Loukusa, S., Mäkinen, L., Kuusikko-Gauffin, S., Ebeling, H., & Moilanen, I.
    (2014) Theory of mind and emotion recognition skills in children with specific language impairment, autism spectrum disorder and typical development: Group differences and connection to knowledge of grammatical morphology, word-finding abilities and verbal working memory. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49, 498–507. 10.1111/1460‑6984.12091
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12091 [Google Scholar]
  97. Manson, J., Bryant, G., Gervais, M., & Kline, M.
    (2013) Convergence of speech rate in conversation predicts cooperation. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(6), 419–426. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.08.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.08.001 [Google Scholar]
  98. Marinis, T., & Sharon Armon-Lotem, S.
    (2015) Sentence repetition. InS. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Assessing Multilingual Children: Disentangling Bilingualism from Language Impairment (pp.95–124). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Martin, I., & McDonald, S.
    (2003) Weak coherence, no theory of mind, or executive dysfunction? Solving the puzzle of pragmatic disorders. Brain and Language, 85, 451–466. 10.1016/S0093‑934X(03)00070‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00070-1 [Google Scholar]
  100. Mattheoudakis, M., Chatzidaki, A., Maligkoudi, C., & Agathopoulou, A.
    (2016) Family and School Language Input: Their role in bilingual children’s vocabulary development. Journal of Applied Linguistics, 31, 49–69.
    [Google Scholar]
  101. Matthews, D., Biney, H., & Abbot-Smith, K.
    (2018) Individual Differences in Children’s Pragmatic Ability: A Review of Associations with Formal Language, Social Cognition, and Executive Functions. Language Learning and Development, 14(3), 186–223. 10.1080/15475441.2018.1455584
    https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2018.1455584 [Google Scholar]
  102. Maynard-Smith, J., & Szathmáry, E.
    (1995) The Major Transitions in Evolution. New York: Oxford University Press. 10.1093/oso/9780198502944.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198502944.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  103. McEvoy, R. E., Rogers, S. J., & Pennington, B. F.
    (1993) Executive function and social communication deficits in young autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 563–578. 10.1111/j.1469‑7610.1993.tb01036.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb01036.x [Google Scholar]
  104. Meir, N., & Novogrodsky, R.
    (2019) Prerequisites of Third-Person Pronoun Use in Monolingual and Bilingual Children With Autism and Typical Language Development. Frontiers in Psychology. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02289
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02289 [Google Scholar]
  105. (2020) Syntactic abilities and verbal memory in monolingual and bilingual children with High Functioning Autism (HFA). First Language, 40(4), 341–366. 10.1177/0142723719849981
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723719849981 [Google Scholar]
  106. Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E.
    (2013) Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114, 187–202. 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.002 [Google Scholar]
  107. Nadig, A., Vivanti, G., & Ozonoff, S.
    (2009) Adaptation of object descriptions to a partner under increasing communicative demands: a comparison of children with and without autism. Autism Research, 2, 334–347. 10.1002/aur.102
    https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.102 [Google Scholar]
  108. Navon, D.
    (1981) The forest revisited: More on global precedence. Psychological Research, 43, 1–32. 10.1007/BF00309635
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00309635 [Google Scholar]
  109. Norbury, C. F., & Bishop, D.
    (2002) Inferential processing and story recall in children with communication problems: A comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 37(3), 227–251. 10.1080/13682820210136269
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820210136269 [Google Scholar]
  110. Norrick, N. R.
    (2015) Narrative illocutionary acts direct and indirect. Journal of Pragmatics, 86, 94–99. 10.1016/j.pragma.2015.05.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2015.05.008 [Google Scholar]
  111. Nowell, K. P., Schanding, G. T. S., Kanne, S. M., & Goin-Kochel, R. P.
    (2015) Cognitive profiles in youth with autism spectrum disorder: an investigation of base rate discrepancies using the Differential Ability Scales – Second Edition. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 1978–1988. 10.1007/s10803‑014‑2356‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2356-7 [Google Scholar]
  112. O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S.
    (2001) Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 719–730.
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Papadopoulos, D.
    (2021) Mothers’ Experiences and Challenges Raising a Child with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Qualitative Study. Brain Sciences, 11, 309. 10.3390/brainsci11030309
    https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11030309 [Google Scholar]
  114. Pappas, M. A., Papoutsi, C., & Drigas, A. S.
    (2018) Policies, Practices, and Attitudes toward Inclusive Education: The Case of Greece. Social Sciences, 7(6), 90. 10.3390/socsci7060090
    https://doi.org/10.3390/socsci7060090 [Google Scholar]
  115. Pearson, B. Z., Fernández, S. C., & Oller, D. K.
    (1993) Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43, 93–120. 10.1111/j.1467‑1770.1993.tb00174.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1993.tb00174.x [Google Scholar]
  116. Peristeri, E., Andreou, M., & Tsimpli, I. M.
    (2017) Syntactic and Story Structure Complexity in the Narratives of High- and Low-Language Ability Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Frontiers in Psychology. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02027
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02027 [Google Scholar]
  117. Peristeri, E., Baldimtsi, E., Andreou, M., & Tsimpli, I. M.
    (2020) The Impact of Bilingualism on the Narrative Ability and the Executive Functions of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Communication Disorders, 85, 105999. 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2020.105999
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2020.105999 [Google Scholar]
  118. Peristeri, E., Baldimtsi, E., Tsimpli, I. M., & Durrleman, S.
    (2019) Bilingualism Effects in Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Metalinguistic Awareness, Executive Functions, and False-Belief Reasoning. InM. M. Brown, & B. Dailey (Eds.), BUCLD 43: Proceedings of the 43rd annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp.549–560). Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Peristeri, E., Baldimtsi, E., Vogelzang, M., Tsimpli, I. M., & Durrleman, S.
    (2021) The cognitive benefits of bilingualism in autism spectrum disorder: Is theory of mind boosted and by which underlying factors?Autism Research, 14(8), 1695–1709. 10.1002/aur.2542
    https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2542 [Google Scholar]
  120. Peristeri, E., Tsimpli, I. M., & Andreou, M.
    (2016) Object clitic production in monolingual and bilingual children with Specific Language Impairment: a comparison between elicited production and narratives. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 7(3/4), 394–430.
    [Google Scholar]
  121. Peristeri, E., Tsimpli, I. M., & Durrleman, S.
    (2021) Positive Effects of Bilingualism on Social Cognition in Autism Spectrum Disorder: a Study of Social Exclusion and Theory of Mind. InD. Dionne & L. A. Vidal Covas (Eds.), BUCLD 45: Proceedings of the 44th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp.626–638). Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  122. Pijnacker, J., Geurts, B., van Lambalgen, M., Buitelaar, J., Hagoort, P.
    (2010) Exceptions and anomalies: an ERP study on context sensitivity in autism. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2940–2951. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.06.003 [Google Scholar]
  123. Pijnacker, J., Hagoort, P., Buitelaar, J., Teunisse, J. P., & Geurts, B.
    (2009) Pragmatic Inferences in High-Functioning Adults with Autism and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39, 607. 10.1007/s10803‑008‑0661‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-008-0661-8 [Google Scholar]
  124. Pinker, S., & Bloom, P.
    (1990) Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13(4), 707–727. 10.1017/S0140525X00081061
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00081061 [Google Scholar]
  125. Pinker, S., & Jackendoff, R.
    (2005) The faculty of language: what’s special about it?Cognition, 95(2), 201–236. 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.08.004 [Google Scholar]
  126. Prentza, A., Tafiadis, D., Chondrogianni, V., & Tsimpli, I. M.
    (2022) Validation of a Greek Sentence Repetition task with typically developing monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research. 10.1007/s10936‑022‑09853‑z
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-022-09853-z [Google Scholar]
  127. Reed, T., & Peterson, C.
    (1990) A Comparative Study of Autistic Subjects’ Performance at Two Levels of Visual and Cognitive Perspective Taking. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 555–567. 10.1007/BF02216060
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02216060 [Google Scholar]
  128. Renfrew, C.
    (1997) The renfrew language scales – Bus story test: A test of narrative speech/Word finding vocabulary test/Action picture test (4th ed.). Speechmark, Milton Keynes.
    [Google Scholar]
  129. Riches, N. G.
    (2012) Sentence repetition in children with specific language impairment: an investigation of underlying mechanisms. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 47(5), 499–510. 10.1111/j.1460‑6984.2012.00158.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00158.x [Google Scholar]
  130. Roqueta, C. A., & Katsos, N.
    (2020) A distinction between linguistic and social pragmatics helps the precise characterization of pragmatic challenges in children with autism spectrum disorders and developmental language disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63, 1494–1508. 10.1044/2020_JSLHR‑19‑00263
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2020_JSLHR-19-00263 [Google Scholar]
  131. Roselló, B., Berenguer, C., Navío, P., Baixauli, I., & Miranda, A.
    (2017) Executive Functioning, Social Cognition, Pragmatics, and Social Interaction in Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Autism Spectrum Disorder. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 4, 72–77. 10.1007/s40474‑017‑0114‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-017-0114-1 [Google Scholar]
  132. Rutter, M., Le Couteur, A., & Lord, C.
    (2003) ADI-R: The Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.
    [Google Scholar]
  133. Sandgren, O. & Holmström, K.
    (2015) Executive functions in mono- and bilingual children with language impairment – issues for speech-language pathology. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1074. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01074
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01074 [Google Scholar]
  134. Schaeffer, J., Durrleman, S. & Eigsti, I. E.
    (2021) On links between language development and extralinguistic cognitive knowledge: What we can learn from autism, Language Acquisition, 28:1, 1–5, 10.1080/10489223.2020.1769631
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10489223.2020.1769631 [Google Scholar]
  135. Schneider, P., Dubé, R. V., & Hayward, D.
    (2005) The Edmonton Narrative Norms Instrument. Retrieved fromUniversity of Alberta Faculty of Rehabilitation Medicine website: www.rehabmed.ualberta.ca/spa/enni
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A.
    (2002) E-Prime User’s Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.
    [Google Scholar]
  137. Schroeder, R. C.
    (2018) Do Bilinguals Have an Advantage in Theory of Mind? A Meta Analysis. Frontiers in Communication. 10.3389/fcomm.2018.00036
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2018.00036 [Google Scholar]
  138. Senju, A.
    (2013) Atypical development of spontaneous social cognition in autism spectrum disorders. Brain & development, 35(2), 96–101. 10.1016/j.braindev.2012.08.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.braindev.2012.08.002 [Google Scholar]
  139. Sharaan, S., Fletcher-Watson, S., & MacPherson, S. E.
    (2021) The impact of bilingualism on the executive functions of autistic children: A study of English-Arabic children. Autism Research, 14(3), 533–544. 10.1002/aur.2439
    https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2439 [Google Scholar]
  140. Sheng, L., & McGregor, K.
    (2010) Lexical–Semantic Organization in Children With Specific Language Impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(1), 146–159. 10.1044/1092‑4388(2009/08‑0160)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0160) [Google Scholar]
  141. Sheng, L., Shi, H., Wang, D., Hao, Y., & Zheng, L.
    (2020) Narrative production in Mandarin-speaking children: Effects of language ability and elicitation method. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(3), 774–792. 10.1044/2019_JSLHR‑19‑00087
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-19-00087 [Google Scholar]
  142. Siegel, D. J., Minshew, N. J., & Goldstein, G.
    (1996) Wechsler IQ profiles in diagnosis of high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 389–406. 10.1007/BF02172825
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02172825 [Google Scholar]
  143. Silleresi, S., Tuller, L., Delage, H., Durrleman, S., Bonnet-Brilhault, F., Malvy, J., & Prevost, P.
    (2018) Sentence repetition and language impairment in French-speaking children with ASD. InA. Gavarró (Eds.), Language Acquisition and Language Disorders (pp.235–258). John Benjamins, Amsterdam.
    [Google Scholar]
  144. Smith, N., & Tsimpli, I.
    (1995) The mind of a savant: Language learning and modularity. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  145. Stenbäck, V., Hällgren, M., & Larsby, B.
    (2016) Executive functions and working memory capacity in speech communication under adverse conditions. Speech, Language and Hearing, 19(4), 218–226. 10.1080/2050571X.2016.1196034
    https://doi.org/10.1080/2050571X.2016.1196034 [Google Scholar]
  146. Stokes, S. F., Wong, A. M-Y., Fletcher, P., & Leonard, L. B.
    (2006) Nonword Repetition and Sentence Repetition as Clinical Markers of Specific Language Impairment: The Case of Cantonese. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 49(2), 219–236. 10.1044/1092‑4388(2006/019)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/019) [Google Scholar]
  147. Tager-Flusberg, H.
    (1999) A psychological approach to understanding the social and language impairments in autism. International Review of Psychiatry, 11, 325–334. 10.1080/09540269974203
    https://doi.org/10.1080/09540269974203 [Google Scholar]
  148. Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M.
    (2004) Identifying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism. InU. Frith & E. Hill (Eds.), Autism: Mind and brain (pp.43–66). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  149. Terzi, A., Marinis, T. & Francis, K.
    (2016) The Interface of Syntax with Pragmatics and Prosody in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 2692–2706. 10.1007/s10803‑016‑2811‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2811-8 [Google Scholar]
  150. Theodorou, E., Kambanaros, M., & Grohmann, K.
    (2013) Specific language impairment in Cypriot Greek: Diagnostic issues. Linguistic Variation, 13(2), 213–236. 10.1075/lv.13.2.04the
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lv.13.2.04the [Google Scholar]
  151. Thomaidis, L., Mavroeidi, N., Richardson, C., Choleva, A., Damianos, G., Bolias, K., & Tsolia, M.
    (2020) Autism Spectrum Disorders in Greece: Nationwide Prevalence in 10-11-Year-Old Children and Regional Disparities. Journal of Clinical Medicine, 9(7), 2163. 10.3390/jcm9072163
    https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm9072163 [Google Scholar]
  152. Thordardottir, E.
    (2011) The relationship between bilingual exposure and vocabulary development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15(4), 426–445. 10.1177/1367006911403202
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006911403202 [Google Scholar]
  153. Tomasello, M.
    (1992) The social bases of language acquisition. Social Development, 1(1), 67–87. 10.1111/j.1467‑9507.1992.tb00135.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.1992.tb00135.x [Google Scholar]
  154. (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  155. (2003) Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  156. Tsimpli, I. M.
    (2013) Autistic savants. InP. Robinson (Eds.), The Routledge encyclopaedia of second language acquisition (pp.51–53). New York: Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  157. Tsimpli, I. M., Peristeri, E., & Andreou, M.
    (2016) Narrative production in monolingual and bilingual children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(1), 195–216. 10.1017/S0142716415000478
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716415000478 [Google Scholar]
  158. Vogindroukas, I., Protopapas, A., & Sideridis, G.
    (2009) Experiment on the Expressive Vocabulary (Greek Version of Renfrew Word Finding Vocabulary Test). Chania: Glafki.
    [Google Scholar]
  159. Vugs, B., Knoors, H., Cuperus, J., Hendriks, M., & Verhoeven, L.
    (2016) Executive function training in children with SLI: A pilot study. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 33(1), 1–20.
    [Google Scholar]
  160. Wardlow, L.
    (2013) Individual differences in speakers’ perspective taking: The roles of executive control and working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20, 766–772. 10.3758/s13423‑013‑0396‑1
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0396-1 [Google Scholar]
  161. Wechsler, D.
    (1991) The Wechsler intelligence scale for children – third edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
    [Google Scholar]
  162. Wermelinger, S., Gampe, A., & Daum, M. M.
    (2017) Bilingual toddlers have advanced abilities to repair communication failure. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 155, 84–94. 10.1016/j.jecp.2016.11.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.11.005 [Google Scholar]
  163. Williams, S., Moore, K., Crossman, A. M., & Talwar, V.
    (2016) The role of executive functions and theory of mind in children’s prosocial lie-telling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 256–266. 10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.001 [Google Scholar]
  164. Wing, L.
    (1997) The autistic spectrum. The Lancet, 350, 1761–1766. 10.1016/S0140‑6736(97)09218‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(97)09218-0 [Google Scholar]
  165. Wynn, C. J., Borrie, S. A., & Sellers, T. P.
    (2018) Speech Rate Entrainment in Children and Adults With and Without Autism Spectrum Disorder. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology: A Journal of Clinical Practice, 27(3), 965–974. 10.1044/2018_AJSLP‑17‑0134
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_AJSLP-17-0134 [Google Scholar]
  166. Yanjie, Y. X. S.
    (2001) Global precedence of hierarchical figure perception in children. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 33(2), 142–147.
    [Google Scholar]
  167. Yirmiya, N., Erel, O., Shaked, M. & Solomonica-Levi, D.
    (1998) Meta-analyses comparing theory of mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation, and normally developing individuals. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 283–307. 10.1037/0033‑2909.124.3.283
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.283 [Google Scholar]
  168. Zelazo, P. D., & Müller, U.
    (2002) Executive function in typical and atypical development. InU. Goswami (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development (pp.445–469). Blackwell Publishing. 10.1002/9780470996652.ch20
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470996652.ch20 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/elt.00037.dur
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/elt.00037.dur
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error