1887
Volume 17, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1568-1475
  • E-ISSN: 1569-9773
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Using a repetition paradigm, in which speakers describe the same event to a sequence of listeners, we analyze the degree of reduction in representational gestures. We find that when listener feedback, both verbal and non-verbal, is minimal and unvarying, speakers steadily reduce their motoric commitment in repeated gestures across tellings without regard to the novelty of the information to the listener. Within this specific condition, we interpret the result to coincide with the view that gestures primarily serve as a part of speech production rather than a communicative act. Importantly, we propose that gestural sensitivity to the listener derives from an interaction between interlocutors, rather than simple modeling of the listener’s state of knowledge in the mind of the speaker alone.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/gest.00011.vaj
2018-10-19
2024-10-05
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Alibali, Martha W. , Lucia M. Flevares , & Susan Goldin-Meadow
    (1997) Assessing knowledge conveyed in gesture: Do teachers have the upper hand?Journal of Educational Psychology, 89 (1), 183–193.10.1037/0022‑0663.89.1.183
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.183 [Google Scholar]
  2. Alibali, Martha W. , Dana C. Heath , & Heather J. Myers
    (2001) Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44 (2), 169–188.10.1006/jmla.2000.2752
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2752 [Google Scholar]
  3. Alibali, Martha W. , Sotaro Kita , & Amanda J. Young
    (2000) Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language and cognitive processes, 15 (6), 593–613.10.1080/016909600750040571
    https://doi.org/10.1080/016909600750040571 [Google Scholar]
  4. Allwood, Jens & Loredana Cerrato
    (2003) A study of gestural feedback expressions. InProceedings of the First Nordic Symposium on Multimodal Communication (pp.7–22). Copenhagen: CST, Center for Sprogteknologi.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Anderson, Anne H. & Barbara Howard
    (2002) Referential form and word duration in video-mediated and face-to-face dialogues. In Johan Bos , Mary Ellen Foster , & Colin Matheson (Eds.), Proceedings of EDILOG 2002. Edinburgh: Cognitive Science Centre, University of Edinburgh.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Ariel, Mira
    (1990) Accessing noun phrase antecedents. London: Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. (1988) Referring and accessibility, Journal of Linguistics, 24, 65–87.10.1017/S0022226700011567
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226700011567 [Google Scholar]
  8. Aylett, Matthew & Alice Turk
    (2004) The smooth signal redundancy hypothesis: A functional explanation for relationships between redundancy, prosodic prominence, and duration in spontaneous speech. Language and Speech, 47, 31–56.10.1177/00238309040470010201
    https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309040470010201 [Google Scholar]
  9. (2006) Language redundancy predicts syllabic duration and the spectral characteristics of vocalic syllable nuclei. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119 (5), 3048–3058.10.1121/1.2188331
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2188331 [Google Scholar]
  10. Baker, Rachel E. & Ann R. Bradlow
    (2009) Variability in word duration as a function of probability, speech style, and prosody. Language and Speech, 52, 391–413.10.1177/0023830909336575
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830909336575 [Google Scholar]
  11. Bard, Ellen G. , Anne H. Anderson , Catherine Sotillo , Matthew Aylett , Gwyneth Doherty-Sneddon , & Alison Newlands
    (2000) Controlling the intelligibility of referring expressions in dialogue. Journal of Memory and Language, 42, 1–22.10.1006/jmla.1999.2667
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2667 [Google Scholar]
  12. Bard, Ellen G. & Matthew Aylett
    (2005) Referential form, duration, and modelling the listener in spoken dialogue. In John C. Trueswell & Michael K. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Approaches to studying world-situated language use: Bridging the language-as-product and language-as-action traditions (pp.173–191). Cambridge: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Bates, Douglas
    (2013) Linear mixed model implementation in lme4. University of Wisconsin–Madison.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Bavelas, Janet & Nicole Chovil
    (2000) Visible acts of meaning an integrated message model of language in face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Language and social Psychology, 19 (2), 163–194.10.1177/0261927X00019002001
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X00019002001 [Google Scholar]
  15. Bavelas, Janet , Jennifer Gerwing , Chantelle Sutton , & Danielle Prevost
    (2008) Gesturing on the telephone: Independent effects of dialogue and visibility. Journal of Memory and Language, 58 (2), 495–520.10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.004 [Google Scholar]
  16. Bavelas, Janet & Sara Healing
    (2013) Reconciling the effects of mutual visibility on gesturing: A review. Gesture, 13 (1), 63–92.10.1075/gest.13.1.03bav
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.13.1.03bav [Google Scholar]
  17. Beattie, Geoffrey & Rima Aboudan
    (1994) Gestures, pauses and speech: An experimental investigation of the effects of changing social context on their precise temporal relationships. Semiotica, 99 (3–4), 239–272.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Beattie, Geoffrey & Jane Coughlan
    (1999) An experimental investigation of the role of iconic gestures in lexical access using the tip‐of‐the‐tongue phenomenon. British Journal of Psychology, 90 (1), 35–56.10.1348/000712699161251
    https://doi.org/10.1348/000712699161251 [Google Scholar]
  19. Beattie, Geoffrey & Heather Shovelton
    (2005) Why the spontaneous images created by the hands during talk can help make TV advertisements more effective. British Journal of Psychology, 96 (1), 21–37.10.1348/000712605X103500
    https://doi.org/10.1348/000712605X103500 [Google Scholar]
  20. Bell, Alan , Jason M. Brenier , Michelle Gregory , Cynthia Girand , & Dan Jurafsky
    (2009) Predictability effects on durations of content and function words in conversational English. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 92–111.10.1016/j.jml.2008.06.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.06.003 [Google Scholar]
  21. Bybee, Joan
    (2001) Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511612886
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511612886 [Google Scholar]
  22. (2002) Word frequency and context of use in the lexical diffusion of phonetically conditioned sound change. Language Variation and Change, 14, 261–290.10.1017/S0954394502143018
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394502143018 [Google Scholar]
  23. Chafe, Wallace
    (1976) Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics and point of view. In Charles N. Li (Ed.), Subject and topic (pp.25–55). New York: Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Chawla, Purnima & Robert M. Krauss
    (1994) Gesture and speech in spontaneous and rehearsed narratives. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30, 580–601.10.1006/jesp.1994.1027
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jesp.1994.1027 [Google Scholar]
  25. Chovil, Nicole
    (1991) Discourse-oriented facial displays in conversation. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 25, 163–194.10.1080/08351819109389361
    https://doi.org/10.1080/08351819109389361 [Google Scholar]
  26. Clark, Herbert H. & Thomas B. Carlson
    (1982) Hearers and speech acts. Language, 58 (2), 332–373.10.1353/lan.1982.0042
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1982.0042 [Google Scholar]
  27. Clark, Herbert H. & Catherine R. Marshall
    (1981) Definite reference and mutual knowledge. In Aravind K. Joshi , Bonnie Webber , & Ivan A. Sag (Eds.), Elements of discourse understanding (pp.10–63). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Clark, Herbert H. & Edward F. Schaefer
    (1989) Contributing to discourse. Cognitive Science, 13 (2), 259–294.10.1207/s15516709cog1302_7
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1302_7 [Google Scholar]
  29. Clark, Herbert H. & Deanna Wilkes-Gibbs
    (1986) Referring as a collaborative process. Cognition, 22 (1), 1–39.10.1016/0010‑0277(86)90010‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(86)90010-7 [Google Scholar]
  30. Cohen, Akiba A. & Randall P. Harrison
    (1973) Intentionality in the use of hand illustrators in face-to-face communication situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28 (2), 276–279.10.1037/h0035792
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035792 [Google Scholar]
  31. Du Bois, John W.
    (1980) Beyond definiteness: The trace of identity in discourse. In Wallace Chafe (Ed.), The pear stories: Cognitive, cultural, and linguistic aspects of narrative production (pp.203–274). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Fisher, Cynthia & Hisayo Tokura
    (1995) The given-new contract in speech to infants. Journal of Memory and Language, 34, 287–310.10.1006/jmla.1995.1013
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1013 [Google Scholar]
  33. Fowler, Carol A.
    (1988) Differential shortening of repeated context words produced in various communicative contexts. Language and Speech, 31, 307–319.10.1177/002383098803100401
    https://doi.org/10.1177/002383098803100401 [Google Scholar]
  34. Fowler, Carol A. & Jonathan Housum
    (1987) Talkers’ signaling of “new” and “old” words in speech and listeners’ perception and use of the distinction. Journal of Memory and Language, 26 (5), 489–504.10.1016/0749‑596X(87)90136‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(87)90136-7 [Google Scholar]
  35. Fowler, Carol A. , Elena T. Levy , & Julie M. Brown
    (1997) Reductions of spoken words in certain discourse contexts. Journal of Memory and Language, 37 (1), 24–40.10.1006/jmla.1996.2504
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1996.2504 [Google Scholar]
  36. Frank, Austin F. & T. Florian Jaeger
    (2008) Speaking rationally: Uniform information density as an optimal strategy for language production. In Bradley C. Love , Kenneth M. McRae , & Vladimir M. Sloutsky (Eds.), Proceedings of the 30th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.939–944). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Freeman, Valerie
    (2014) Hyperarticulation as a signal of stance. Journal of Phonetics, 45, 1–11.10.1016/j.wocn.2014.03.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2014.03.002 [Google Scholar]
  38. Frick-Horbury, Donna & Robert E. Guttentag
    (1998) The effects of restricting hand gesture production on lexical retrieval and free recall. The American Journal of Psychology, 111 (1), 43–62.10.2307/1423536
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1423536 [Google Scholar]
  39. Galati, Alexia & Susan E. Brennan
    (2010) Attenuating repeated information: For the speaker, or for the addressee?Journal of Memory and Language, 62, 35–51.10.1016/j.jml.2009.09.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2009.09.002 [Google Scholar]
  40. (2013) Speakers adapt gestures to addressees’ knowledge: implications for models of co-speech gesture. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29 (4), 435–451.10.1080/01690965.2013.796397
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2013.796397 [Google Scholar]
  41. Gerwing, Jennifer & Janet Bavelas
    (2004) Linguistic influences on gesture’s form. Gesture, 4 (2), 157–195.10.1075/gest.4.2.04ger
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.4.2.04ger [Google Scholar]
  42. (2013) The social interactive nature of gestures: Theory, assumptions, methods, and findings. In Cornelia Müller , Alan Cienki , Ellen Fricke , Silva S. Ladewig , David McNeill , & Sedinha Teßendorf (Eds). Body – language – communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (Vol.1, pp.821–836). Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Goodwin, Charles
    (1981) Conversational organization: Interaction between speakers and hearers. New York: Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Hilliard, Caitlin & Susan W. Cook
    (2015, June 29) Bridging gaps in common ground: Speakers design their gestures for their listeners. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1037/xlm000015
    https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm000015 [Google Scholar]
  45. Hoetjes, Marieke , Ruud Koolen , Martjin Goudbeek , Emiel Krahmer , & Marc Swerts
    (2011) GREEBLES Greeble greeb. On reduction in speech and gesture in repeated references. InProceedings of the 33rd annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci), July 2011, Boston, Massachusetts (pp.3250–3255). Austin, TX: Cognitive. Science Society.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. (2015) Reduction in gesture during the production of repeated references. Journal of Memory and Language, 79–80, 1–17.10.1016/j.jml.2014.10.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2014.10.004 [Google Scholar]
  47. Hoetjes, Marieke , Emiel Krahmer , & Marc Swerts
    (2014) On what happens in speech and gesture when communication is unsuccessful. Proceedings of the 36th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci), July 2014, Québec City, Canada (pp.2369–2374). Austin, TX: Cognitive. Science Society.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Holler, Judith & Rachel Stevens
    (2007) The effect of common ground on how speakers use gesture and speech to represent size information. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 26(1), 4–27.10.1177/0261927X06296428
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X06296428 [Google Scholar]
  49. Holler, Judith & Katie Wilkin
    (2009) Communicating common ground: how mutually shared knowledge influences the representation of semantic information in speech and gesture in a narrative task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24, 267–289.10.1080/01690960802095545
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960802095545 [Google Scholar]
  50. Holler, Judith & Katie Wilkin
    (2011) An experimental investigation of how addressee feedback affects co-speech gestures accompanying speakers’ responses. Journal of Pragmatics, 43 (14), 3522–3536.10.1016/j.pragma.2011.08.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2011.08.002 [Google Scholar]
  51. Horton, William S. & Boaz Keysar
    (1996) When do speakers take into account common ground?Cognition, 59 (1), 91–117.10.1016/0010‑0277(96)81418‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(96)81418-1 [Google Scholar]
  52. Hostetter, Autumn B. & Martha W. Alibali
    (2007) Raise your hand if you’re spatial: Relations between verbal and spatial skills and gesture production. Gesture, 7 (1), 73–95.10.1075/gest.7.1.05hos
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.7.1.05hos [Google Scholar]
  53. (2008) Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 15 (3), 495–514.10.3758/PBR.15.3.495
    https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.15.3.495 [Google Scholar]
  54. (2010) Language, gesture, action! A test of the Gesture as Simulated Action framework. Journal of Memory and Language, 63 (2), 245–257.10.1016/j.jml.2010.04.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.04.003 [Google Scholar]
  55. Isaacs, Ellen A. & Herbert H. Clark
    (1987) References in conversation between experts and novices. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 116 (1), 26–37.10.1037/0096‑3445.116.1.26
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.116.1.26 [Google Scholar]
  56. Iverson, Jana M. & Susan Goldin‐Meadow
    (1997) What’s communication got to do with it? Gesture in children blind from birth. Developmental Psychology, 33 (3), 453–467.10.1037/0012‑1649.33.3.453
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.3.453 [Google Scholar]
  57. (2001) The resilience of gesture in talk: Gesture in blind speakers and listeners. Developmental Science, 4 (4), 416–422.10.1111/1467‑7687.00183
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-7687.00183 [Google Scholar]
  58. Jacobs, Naomi & Alan Garnham
    (2007) The role of conversational hand gestures in a narrative task. Journal of Memory and Language, 56 (2), 291–303.10.1016/j.jml.2006.07.011
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.07.011 [Google Scholar]
  59. Jaeger, Florian T.
    (2010) Redundancy and reduction: Speakers manage syntactic information density. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 23–62.10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.02.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.02.002 [Google Scholar]
  60. Jelec, Anna & Dorota Jaworska
    (2014) Thoughts on the table: Gesture as a tool for thinking in blind and visually impaired children. Yearbook of the Poznan Linguistic Meeting, 1 (1), 73–88.10.1515/yplm‑2015‑0004
    https://doi.org/10.1515/yplm-2015-0004 [Google Scholar]
  61. Kapatsinski, Vsevolod
    (2010) Frequency of use leads to automaticity of production: Evidence from repair in conversation. Language & Speech, 53 (1), 71–105.10.1177/0023830909351220
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830909351220 [Google Scholar]
  62. Kendall, Tyler
    (2013) Speech rate, pause and sociolinguistic variation: studies in corpus sociophonetics. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.10.1057/9781137291448
    https://doi.org/10.1057/9781137291448 [Google Scholar]
  63. Kendon, Adam
    (1994) Do gestures communicate? A review. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 27 (3), 175–200.10.1207/s15327973rlsi2703_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327973rlsi2703_2 [Google Scholar]
  64. (2004) Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511807572
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807572 [Google Scholar]
  65. Kimbara, Irene
    (2006) On gestural mimicry. Gesture, 6 (1), 39–61.10.1075/gest.6.1.03kim
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.6.1.03kim [Google Scholar]
  66. (2008) Gesture form convergence in joint description. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 32 (2), 123–131.10.1007/s10919‑007‑0044‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-007-0044-4 [Google Scholar]
  67. Kita, Sotaro
    (1993) Language and thought interface: A study of spontaneous gestures and Japanese mimetics. Unpublished PhD Dissertation. University of Chicago.
  68. (2000) How representational gestures help speaking. In David McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture: Window into thought and action (pp.162–185). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511620850.011
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620850.011 [Google Scholar]
  69. Kita, Sotaro & Aslı Özyürek
    (2003) What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48 (1), 16–32.10.1016/S0749‑596X(02)00505‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00505-3 [Google Scholar]
  70. Krauss, Robert M.
    (1998) Why do we gesture when we speak?Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 54–60.10.1111/1467‑8721.ep13175642
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep13175642 [Google Scholar]
  71. Krauss, Robert M. , William Apple , Nancy Morency , Charlotte Wenzel , & Ward Winton
    (1981) Verbal, vocal, and visible factors in judgments of another’s affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40 (2), 312–320.10.1037/0022‑3514.40.2.312
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.40.2.312 [Google Scholar]
  72. Krauss, Robert M. , Yihsiu Chen , & Purnima Chawla
    (1996) Nonverbal behavior and nonverbal communication: What do conversational hand gestures tell us?Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 28, 389–450.10.1016/S0065‑2601(08)60241‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60241-5 [Google Scholar]
  73. Krauss, Robert M. , Yihsiu Chen , & Rebecca F. Gottesman
    (2000) Lexical gestures and lexical access: a process model. In David McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture: Window into thought and action (pp.261–283). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511620850.017
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620850.017 [Google Scholar]
  74. Krauss, Robert M. , Robert A. Dushay , Yihsiu Chen , & Frances Rauscher
    (1995) The communicative value of conversational hand gesture. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31 (6), 533–552.10.1006/jesp.1995.1024
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jesp.1995.1024 [Google Scholar]
  75. Krauss, Robert M. , Palmer Morrel-Samuels , & Christina Colasante
    (1991) Do conversational hand gestures communicate?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 (5), 743–754.10.1037/0022‑3514.61.5.743
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.5.743 [Google Scholar]
  76. Kuhlen, Anna K. & Susan E. Brennan
    (2013) Language in dialogue: When confederates might be hazardous to your data. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20, 54–72.10.3758/s13423‑012‑0341‑8
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-012-0341-8 [Google Scholar]
  77. Kuhlen, Anna K. & Mandana Seyfeddinipur
    (2007) From speaker to speaker: Repeated gestures across speakers. Presentation held atBerlin Gesture Center Colloquium, Berlin.
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Lam, Tuan Q. & Duane G. Watson
    (2010) Repetition is easy: Why repeated referents have reduced prominence. Memory and Cognition, 38, 1137–1146.10.3758/MC.38.8.1137
    https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.38.8.1137 [Google Scholar]
  79. Levelt, Willem J.
    (1992) Accessing words in speech production: Stages, processes and representations. Cognition, 42 (1), 1–22.10.1016/0010‑0277(92)90038‑J
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90038-J [Google Scholar]
  80. Lewis, David
    (1969) Convention: A philosophical study. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Lindblom, Björn
    (1990) Explaining phonetic variation: A sketch of H&H theory. In William J. Hardcastle & Alain Marchal (Eds), Speech production and speech modeling (pp. 403–439). Dordrecht: Kluwer.10.1007/978‑94‑009‑2037‑8_16
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-2037-8_16 [Google Scholar]
  82. Masson-Carro, Ingrid , Martijn Goudbeek , & Emiel Krahmer
    (2013) The influence of cognitive load on repeated references in speech and gesture. Proceedings of the Tilburg Gesture Research Meeting (TiGeR 2013). June 2013, Tilburg, The Netherlands.
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Masson-Carro, Ingrid, Martijn Goudbeek, & Emiel Krahmer
    (2014) On the automaticity of reduction in dialogue: Cognitive load and repeated multimodal references. InPaul Bello et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 976–981). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
    [Google Scholar]
  84. McNeill, David
    (1992) Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  85. (2005) Gesture and thought. Chicago: Chicago University Press.10.7208/chicago/9780226514642.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226514642.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  86. Melinger, Alissa & Willem J. M. Levelt
    (2004) Gesture and the communicative intention of the speaker. Gesture, 4 (2), 119–141.10.1075/gest.4.2.02mel
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.4.2.02mel [Google Scholar]
  87. Mol, Lisette , Emiel Krahmer , Alfons Maes , & Marc Swerts
    (2009a) Communicative gestures and memory load. In Niels Taatgen & Hedderik van Rijn (Eds.), Proceedings of the 31st Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.1569–1574). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
    [Google Scholar]
  88. (2009b) The communicative import of gestures: Evidence from a comparative analysis of human–human and human–machine interactions. Gesture, 9 (1), 97–126.10.1075/gest.9.1.04mol
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.9.1.04mol [Google Scholar]
  89. (2012) Adaptation in gesture: Converging hands or converging minds?Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 249–264.10.1016/j.jml.2011.07.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.07.004 [Google Scholar]
  90. Morrel-Samuels, Palmer & Robert M. Krauss
    (1992) Word familiarity predicts temporal asynchrony of hand gestures and speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18 (3), 615–623.
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Özyürek, Aslı
    (2002) Do speakers design their cospeech gestures for their addressees? The effects of addressee location on representational gestures. Journal of Memory and Language, 46 (4), 688–704.10.1006/jmla.2001.2826
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2826 [Google Scholar]
  92. Parrill, Fey
    (2010) The hands are part of the package: Gesture, common ground, and information packaging. In Sally Rice & John Newman (Eds.), Empirical and experimental methods in cognitive/functional research (pp.285–302). Stanford: CSLI Publications.
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Pederson, Eric
    (2014) Listener head gestures and the co-construction of narrative timing. Talk given atthe meeting of the International Society for Gesture Studies (ISGS 6), San Diego.
    [Google Scholar]
  94. Pluymaekers, Mark , Mirjam Ernestus , & R. Harald Baayen
    (2005) Articulatory planning is continuous and sensitive to informational redundancy. Phonetica, 62, 146–159.10.1159/000090095
    https://doi.org/10.1159/000090095 [Google Scholar]
  95. R Development Core Team
    R Development Core Team (2014) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available from: www.R-project.org
  96. Rauscher, Frances H. , Robert M. Krauss , & Yihsui Chen
    (1996) Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychological Science, 7(4), 226–231.10.1111/j.1467‑9280.1996.tb00364.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00364.x [Google Scholar]
  97. Rimé, Bernard
    (1982) The elimination of visible behaviour from social interactions: Effects on verbal, nonverbal and interpersonal variables. European journal of Social Psychology, 12 (2), 113–129.10.1002/ejsp.2420120201
    https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420120201 [Google Scholar]
  98. de Ruiter, Jan
    (1995) Why do people gesture at the telephone?InProceedings of the CLS opening Academic Year 1995–1996 (pp.49–56). Nijmegen: Center for Language Studies.
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Schelling, Thomas C.
    (1960) The strategy of conflict. Cambridge; MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Schneider, Walter , Amy Eschman , & Anthony Zuccolotto
    (2012) E-Prime user’s guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.
  101. Singer, Melissa A. & Susan Goldin-Meadow
    (2005) Children learn when their teacher’s gestures and speech differ. Psychological Science, 16 (2), 85–89.10.1111/j.0956‑7976.2005.00786.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00786.x [Google Scholar]
  102. Sloetjes, Han & Peter Wittenburg
    (2008, May) Annotation by category: ELAN and ISO DCR. InProceedings of the 6th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC 2008).
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Stave, Matthew & Eric Pederson
    (2015) Is the listener really listening? Exploring the effect of verbal and gestural speaker cues on backchanneling. In David C. Noelle , Rick Dale , Anne Warlaumont , Jeff Yoshimi , Teenie Matlock , Carolyn Jennings , & Paul P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp.3088–3095). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
    [Google Scholar]
  104. Tabensky, Alexis
    (2001) Gesture and speech rephrasings in conversation. Gesture, 1 (2), 213–236.10.1075/gest.1.2.07tab
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.1.2.07tab [Google Scholar]
  105. Tannen, Deborah
    (2007) Talking voices: Repetition, dialogue, and imagery in conversational discourse. New York: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511618987
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511618987 [Google Scholar]
  106. Vajrabhaya, Prakaiwan & Vsevolod Kapatsinski
    (2011) There is more to the story: First-mention lengthening in Thai interactive discourse. InProceedings of the International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (Vol.17, pp.2050–2053).
    [Google Scholar]
  107. (2013) Modeling the listener? What resets acoustic durations of repeated English words. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 134 (5), 4203–4203.10.1121/1.4831426
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4831426 [Google Scholar]
  108. Vajrabhaya, Prakaiwan & Eric Pederson
    (2012) Have I told you this before? Reduction in magnitude over repetition in co-speech gestures. Poster presented atthe meeting of the International Society for Gesture Studies (ISGS 5), Lund, Sweden.
    [Google Scholar]
  109. (2013) Repetition vs. listener accommodation: A case study of co-speech gesture in retellings. Poster presented atthe meeting of New Ways of Analyzing Variation, Pittsburgh, PA.
    [Google Scholar]
  110. (2015) “The baking stick thing”: Automatization of co-speech gesture during lexical access. In David C. Noelle , Rick Dale , Anne Warlaumont , Jeff Yoshimi , Teenie Matlock , Carolyn Jennings , & Paul P. Maglio (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (p.3101–3107). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
    [Google Scholar]
  111. Wagner, Petra , Zofia Malisz , & Stefan Kopp
    (2014) Gesture and speech in interaction: an overview. Speech Communication, 57, 209–232.10.1016/j.specom.2013.09.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2013.09.008 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/gest.00011.vaj
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/gest.00011.vaj
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): automatization; co-speech gesture; interaction; reduction; repetition
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error