1887
Volume 20, Issue 3
  • ISSN 1568-1475
  • E-ISSN: 1569-9773
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Gesture and speech are closely associated channels involved in conveying meaning. Previous studies have reported the relationship between gesture use and speech performance for highly proficient speakers. This study extends the investigation of this relationship by correlating the representational gesture rate and complexity, accuracy, and fluency measures for L2 speech produced by lower-intermediate-level EFL learners with Chinese as their L1. Three speech tasks were adopted with two activating concrete concepts and one abstract concepts. The results showed that the representational gesture rate was correlated with meaning-associated but not form-associated speech measures for L2 learners, and that the task type did not influence the relationship between gesture and speech. This study suggests that representational gesture use has a close relationship with meaning-related speech measures for lower proficiency L2 speakers when they talk about both concrete and abstract concepts.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/gest.19017.ma
2022-08-30
2025-04-20
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Alibali, Martha W.
    (2005) Gesture in spatial cognition: Expressing, communicating, and thinking about spatial information. Spatial Cognition & Computation, 5 (4), 307–331. 10.1207/s15427633scc0504_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15427633scc0504_2 [Google Scholar]
  2. Alibali, Martha W., Kita, Sotaro, & Young, Amanda J.
    (2000) Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language & Cognitive Processes, 15 (6), 593–613. 10.1080/016909600750040571
    https://doi.org/10.1080/016909600750040571 [Google Scholar]
  3. Aziz, Jasmine R. & Nicoladis, Elena
    (2019) “My French is rusty”: Proficiency and bilingual gesture use in a majority English community. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 22 (4), 826–835. 10.1017/S1366728918000639
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000639 [Google Scholar]
  4. Boroditsky, Lera
    (2000) Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75(1), 1–28. 10.1016/S0010‑0277(99)00073‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00073-6 [Google Scholar]
  5. Burns, Patrick, Mccormack, Teresa, Jaroslawska, Agnieszka J., O’Connor, Patrick A., & Caruso, Eugene M.
    (2019) Time points: A gestural study of the development of space-time mappings. Cognitive Science, 43 (12). 10.1111/cogs.12801
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12801 [Google Scholar]
  6. Cartmill, Erica A. & Goldin-Meadow, Susan
    (2016) Gesture. InDavid Matsumoto, Hyisung C. Hwang, & Mark G. Frank (Eds.), APA handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 307–333). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. 10.1037/14669‑012
    https://doi.org/10.1037/14669-012 [Google Scholar]
  7. Chu, Mingyuan, Meyer, Antje, Foulkes, Lucy, & Kita, Sotaro
    (2014) Individual differences in frequency and saliency of speech-accompanying gestures: The role of cognitive abilities and empathy. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 143 (2), 694–709. 10.1037/a0033861
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033861 [Google Scholar]
  8. Chui, Kawai
    (2011) Conceptual metaphors in gesture. Cognitive Linguistics, 22 (3), 437–458. 10.1515/cogl.2011.017
    https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2011.017 [Google Scholar]
  9. Cienki, Alan & Müller, Cornelia
    (Eds.) (2008) Metaphor and gesture. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 10.1075/gs.3
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gs.3 [Google Scholar]
  10. Cook, Susan Wagner, Yip, Terina Kuang Yi, & Goldin-Meadow, Susan
    (2012) Gestures, but not meaningless movements, lighten working memory load when explaining math. Language & Cognitive Processes, 27 (4), 594–610. 10.1080/01690965.2011.567074
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.567074 [Google Scholar]
  11. Dewaele, Jean-Marc & Furnham, Adrian
    (2000) Personality and speech production: A pilot study of second language learners. Personality & Individual Differences, 28 (2), 355–365. 10.1016/S0191‑8869(99)00106‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00106-3 [Google Scholar]
  12. Ellis, Rod & Barkhuizen, Gary
    (2005) Analysing learner language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Feyereisen, Pierre & Havard, Isabelle
    (1999) Mental imagery and production of hand gestures while speaking in younger and older adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23 (2), 153–171. 10.1023/A:1021487510204
    https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021487510204 [Google Scholar]
  14. Forceville, Charles
    (2009) Non-verbal and multimodal metaphor in a cognitivist framework: Agendas for research. InCharles J. Forceville & Eduardo Urios-Aparisi (Eds.), Multimodal metaphor (pp. 19–42). Berlin &  New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 10.1515/9783110215366.1.19
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110215366.1.19 [Google Scholar]
  15. Foster, Pauline, Tonkyn, Alan, & Wigglesworth, Gillian
    (2000) Measuring spoken language: A unit for all reasons. Applied Linguistics, 21 (3), 354–375. 10.1093/applin/21.3.354
    https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/21.3.354 [Google Scholar]
  16. Frick-Horbury, Donna
    (2002) The use of hand gestures as self-generated cues for recall of verbally associated targets. The American Journal of Psychology, 115 (1), 1–20. 10.2307/1423671
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1423671 [Google Scholar]
  17. Goldin-Meadow, Susan
    (2003) Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Cambridge, MA.: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Goldin-Meadow, Susan, Nusbaum, Howard, Kelly, Spencer, & Wagner, Susan
    (2001) Explaining math: Gesturing lightens the load. Psychological Science, 12 (6), 516–522. 10.1111/1467‑9280.00395
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00395 [Google Scholar]
  19. Gullberg, Marianne
    (1998) Gesture as a communication strategy in second language discourse: A study of learners of French and Swedish. Doctoral dissertation, Lund University, Lund.
  20. Hostetter, Autumn B. & Alibali, Martha W.
    (2004) On the tip of the mind: Gesture as a key to conceptualization. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 261, 589–594. Retrieved fromhttps://escholarship.org/uc/cognitivesciencesociety
    [Google Scholar]
  21. (2007) Raise your hand if you’re spatial: Relations between verbal and spatial skills and gesture production. Gesture, 7 (1), 73–95. 10.1075/gest.7.1.05hos
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.7.1.05hos [Google Scholar]
  22. Hostetter, Autumn B., Alibali, Martha W., & Kita, Sotaro
    (2007) I see it in my hands’ eye: Representational gestures reflect conceptual demands. Language & Cognitive Processes, 22 (3), 313–336. 10.1080/01690960600632812
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960600632812 [Google Scholar]
  23. Hostetter, Autumn B. & Potthoff, Andrea L.
    (2012) Effects of personality and social situation on representational gesture production. Gesture, 12 (1), 63–84. 10.1075/gest.12.1.04hos
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.12.1.04hos [Google Scholar]
  24. Johnson, Mark
    (1987) The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 10.7208/chicago/9780226177847.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226177847.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  25. Kendon, Adam
    (2004) Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511807572
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807572 [Google Scholar]
  26. Kita, Sotaro, Alibali, Martha W., & Chu, Mingyuan
    (2017) How do gestures influence thinking and speaking? The gesture-for-conceptualization hypothesis. Psychological Review, 124 (3), 245–266. 10.1037/rev0000059
    https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000059 [Google Scholar]
  27. Kita, Sotaro & Davies, Thomas S.
    (2009) Competing conceptual representations trigger co-speech representational gestures. Language & Cognitive Processes, 24 (5), 761–775. 10.1080/01690960802327971
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960802327971 [Google Scholar]
  28. Kormos, Judit
    (2006) Speech production and second language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Kövecses, Zoltán
    (2000) Metaphor and emotion: Language, culture, and body in human feeling. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Krauss, Robert M. & Hadar, Uri
    (1999) The role of speech-related arm/hand gestures in word retrieval. InLynn S. Messing & Ruth Campbell (Eds.), Gesture, speech, and sign (pp. 93–116). Oxford: Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524519.003.0006
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198524519.003.0006 [Google Scholar]
  31. Lakoff, George
    (1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. 10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  32. (1993) The contemporary theory of metaphor. InAndrew Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (2nd ed.) (pp. 202–251). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9781139173865.013
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139173865.013 [Google Scholar]
  33. Lakoff, George & Johnson, Mark
    (1980) Metaphors we live by. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Laurent, Angélique & Nicoladis, Elena
    (2015) Gesture restriction affects French-English bilinguals’ speech only in French. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 18 (2), 340–349. 10.1017/S1366728914000042
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000042 [Google Scholar]
  35. Laurent, Angélique, Nicoladis, Elena, & Marentette, Paula
    (2015) The development of storytelling in two languages with words and gestures. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19 (1), 56–74. 10.1177/1367006913495618
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006913495618 [Google Scholar]
  36. Lausberg, Hedda & Sloetjes, Han
    (2009) Coding gestural behavior with the NEUROGES-ELAN system. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 41 (3), 841–849. 10.3758/BRM.41.3.841
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.3.841 [Google Scholar]
  37. Lavergne, Joanne & Kimura, Doreen
    (1987) Hand movement asymmetry during speech: No effect of speaking topic. Neuropsychologia, 25 (4), 689–693. 10.1016/0028‑3932(87)90060‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(87)90060-1 [Google Scholar]
  38. Levelt, Willem J. M.
    (1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Li, Shaofeng & Fu, Mengxia
    (2018) Strategic and unpressured within-task planning and their associations with working memory. Language Teaching Research, 22 (2), 230–253. 10.1177/1362168816684367
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168816684367 [Google Scholar]
  40. Lin, Yen-Liang
    (2017) Co-occurrence of speech and gestures: A multimodal corpus linguistic approach to intercultural interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 1171, 155–167. 10.1016/j.pragma.2017.06.014
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2017.06.014 [Google Scholar]
  41. Lindstromberg, Seth
    (2010) English prepositions explained (Revised edition). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins. 10.1075/z.157
    https://doi.org/10.1075/z.157 [Google Scholar]
  42. McCarthy, Philip M. & Jarvis, Scott
    (2010) MTLD, vocd-D, and HD-D: A validation study of sophisticated approaches to lexical diversity assessment. Behavior Research Methods, 42 (2), 381–392. 10.3758/BRM.42.2.381
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.2.381 [Google Scholar]
  43. McNeill, David
    (1992) Hand and mind: What the hands reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Melinger, Alissa & Kita, Sotaro
    (2007) Conceptualization load triggers gesture production. Language & Cognitive Processes, 22 (4), 473–500. 10.1080/01690960600696916
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960600696916 [Google Scholar]
  45. Mori, Souma
    (2019) A cognitive analysis of the preposition OVER: Image-schema transformations and metaphorical extensions. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 64 (3), 444–474. 10.1017/cnj.2018.43
    https://doi.org/10.1017/cnj.2018.43 [Google Scholar]
  46. Morsella, Ezequiel & Krauss, Robert M.
    (2004) The role of gestures in spatial working memory and speech. The American Journal of Psychology, 117 (3), 411–424. 10.2307/4149008
    https://doi.org/10.2307/4149008 [Google Scholar]
  47. Mota, Mailce B.
    (2005) Working memory capacity and fluency, accuracy, complexity, and lexical density in L2 speech production. Fragmentos, 241, 69–104. Retrieved fromhttps://dialnet.unirioja.es/servlet/revista?codigo=14944
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Nagpal, Jaya, Nicoladis, Elena, & Marentette, Paula
    (2011) Predicting individual differences in L2 speakers’ gestures. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15 (2), 205–214. 10.1177/1367006910381195
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006910381195 [Google Scholar]
  49. Nicoladis, Elena, Marentette, Paula, & Navarro, Samuel
    (2016) Gesture frequency linked primarily to story length in 4-10-year old children’s stories. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 45 (2), 189–204. 10.1007/s10936‑014‑9342‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-014-9342-2 [Google Scholar]
  50. Nicoladis, Elena, Mayberry, Rachel I., & Genesee, Fred
    (1999) Gesture and early bilingual development. Developmental Psychology, 35 (2), 514–526. 10.1037/0012‑1649.35.2.514
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.514 [Google Scholar]
  51. Nicoladis, Elena, Pika, Simone, Yin, Hui, & Marentette, Paula
    (2007) Gesture use in story recall by Chinese-English bilinguals. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28 (4), 721–735. 10.1017/S0142716407070385
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070385 [Google Scholar]
  52. Nicoladis, Elena, Pika, Simone, & Marentette, Paula
    (2009) Do French-English bilingual children gesture more than monolingual children?Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 38 (6), 573–585. 10.1007/s10936‑009‑9121‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-009-9121-7 [Google Scholar]
  53. Oya, Taeko, Manalo, Emmanuel, & Greenwood, Jackie
    (2004) The influence of personality and anxiety on the oral performance of Japanese speakers of English. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 18 (7), 841–855. 10.1002/acp.1063
    https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1063 [Google Scholar]
  54. Peng, Jian-E.
    (2014) Willingness to communicate in the Chinese EFL university classroom: An ecological perspective. Bristol: Multilingual Matters. 10.21832/9781783091560
    https://doi.org/10.21832/9781783091560 [Google Scholar]
  55. Rauscher, Frances H., Krauss, Robert M., & Chen, Yihsiu
    (1996) Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychological Science, 7 (4), 226–231. 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.1996.tb00364.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00364.x [Google Scholar]
  56. Skehan, Peter
    (1998) A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. (2009) Modelling second language performance: Integrating complexity, accuracy, fluency, and lexis. Applied Linguistics, 30 (4), 510–532. 10.1093/applin/amp047
    https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amp047 [Google Scholar]
  58. (Ed.) (2014) Processing perspectives on task performance. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 10.1075/tblt.5
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tblt.5 [Google Scholar]
  59. Skehan, Peter & Foster, Pauline
    (1999) The influence of task structure and processing conditions on narrative retellings. Language Learning, 49 (1), 93–120. 10.1111/1467‑9922.00071
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9922.00071 [Google Scholar]
  60. (2001) Cognition and tasks. InPeter Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 183–205). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9781139524780.009
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139524780.009 [Google Scholar]
  61. Smithson, Lisa, Nicoladis, Elena & Marentette, Paula
    (2011) Bilingual children’s gesture use. Gesture, 11 (3), 330–347. 10.1075/gest.11.3.04smi
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.11.3.04smi [Google Scholar]
  62. So, Wing C.
    (2010) Cross-cultural transfer in gesture frequency in Chinese-English bilinguals. Language & Cognitive Processes, 25 (10), 1335–1353. 10.1080/01690961003694268
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690961003694268 [Google Scholar]
  63. Stam, Gale
    (2016) Gesture as a window onto conceptualization in multiple tasks: Implications for second language teaching. Yearbook of the German Cognitive Linguistics Association, 4 (1), 289–314. 10.1515/gcla‑2016‑0017
    https://doi.org/10.1515/gcla-2016-0017 [Google Scholar]
  64. Wang, Shouren (王守仁)
    (2018) The application of China’s Standards of English Language Ability to college English teaching (中国英语能力等级量表在大学英语教学中的应用). Foreign Language Education (《外语教学》), 39 (4), 1–4. 10.16362/j.cnki.cn61‑1023/h.2018.04.001
    https://doi.org/10.16362/j.cnki.cn61-1023/h.2018.04.001 [Google Scholar]
  65. Willems, Roel M., Özyürek, Aslı, & Hagoort, Peter
    (2007) When language meets action: The neural integration of gesture and speech. Cerebral Cortex, 17 (10), 2322–2333. 10.1093/cercor/bhl141
    https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhl141 [Google Scholar]
  66. Yuan, Fangyuan & Ellis, Rod
    (2003) The effects of pre-task planning and on-line planning on fluency, complexity and accuracy in L2 monologic oral production. Applied Linguistics, 24 (1), 1–27. 10.1093/applin/24.1.1
    https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/24.1.1 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/gest.19017.ma
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/gest.19017.ma
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error