1887
Volume 11, Issue 1
  • ISSN 2213-8706
  • E-ISSN: 2213-8714
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

This study explores the role of transitional probability (TP) in sentence processing in Chinese, a writing system that presents unique challenges due to its character-based structure and lack of word boundaries. The research investigates how the statistical regularities of character meaning, as captured by TP, aid in word segmentation and impact reading comprehension. Utilizing a moving window task, the study examines the processing speed of characters in high versus low TP conditions. Findings reveal that characters in high TP bigram conditions (indicating a consistent semantic association within a bigram) are processed more quickly, underscoring the importance of this statistical property of characters in Chinese sentence reading. These findings challenge conventional notions in Chinese linguistics concerning the relationship between characters, morphemes, and semantics, and suggests an alternative perspective on (and the need for reevaluation of) character-level semantics. The study also highlights the influence of prosodic context on reading speed, indicating that anticipatory linguistic patterns shape reader processing.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ijchl.00020.wan
2024-06-17
2024-12-13
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Baayen, R. H., & Milin, P.
    (2010) Analyzing reaction times. International journal of psychological research, 3(2), 12–28. 10.21500/20112084.807
    https://doi.org/10.21500/20112084.807 [Google Scholar]
  2. Bai, X. J., Tian, J., Yan, G. L., & Wang, T. L.
    (2009) The effect of word segmentation on Chinese text reading: Evidence from American students’ eye movements. Nankai Linguistics, 11, 140–185.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Bai, X., Yan, G., Liversedge, S. P., Zang, C., & Rayner, K.
    (2008) Reading spaced and unspaced Chinese text: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34(5), 1277–1287. 10.1037/0096‑1523.34.5.1277
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.34.5.1277 [Google Scholar]
  4. Bai, X., Zhang, T., Tian, L., Liang, F., & Wang, T.
    (2010) Effect of word segmentation on American students reading Chinese: Evidence from eye movements. Psychological Research, 51, 25–30.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bloomfield, L.
    (1935) Language. George Allen & Unwin, London. Linguistic Inquiry Monographs.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Booij, G.
    (2007) Construction morphology and the lexicon. Selected proceedings of the 5th Décembrettes: Morphology in Toulouse, 34–44.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. (2012) The grammar of words: An introduction to linguistic morphology. Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Brown, M., Salverda, A. P., Dilley, L. C., & Tanenhaus, M. K.
    (2011) Expectations from preceding prosody influence segmentation in online sentence processing. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(6), 1189–1196. 10.3758/s13423‑011‑0167‑9
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0167-9 [Google Scholar]
  9. Chen, K., Gu, L. & Bai, Q.
    (2023) Processing Chinese formulaic sequences in sentence context: a comparative study of native and non-native speakers. Humanit Soc Sci Commun101, 622. 10.1057/s41599‑023‑02137‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-023-02137-4 [Google Scholar]
  10. Conway, C. M., Bauernschmidt, A., Huang, S. S., & Pisoni, D. B.
    (2010) Implicit statistical learning in language processing: Word predictability is the key. Cognition, 114(3), 356–371. 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.10.009 [Google Scholar]
  11. Conway, C. M., & Christiansen, M. H.
    (2005) Modality-constrained statistical learning of tactile, visual, and auditory sequences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(1), 24.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Conway, C. M., & Pisoni, D. B.
    (2008) Neurocognitive basis of implicit learning of sequential structure and its relation to language processing. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1145(1), 113–131. 10.1196/annals.1416.009
    https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1416.009 [Google Scholar]
  13. Da, J.
    (2004) Chinese text computing. Online version at: lingua.mtsu.edu/chinesecomputing/old-version1/
    [Google Scholar]
  14. (2004) A corpus-based study of character and bigram frequencies in Chinese e-texts and its implications for Chinese language instruction. InProceedings of the fourth international conference on new technologies in teaching and learning Chinese (pp.501–11). Beijing: Tsinghua University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. DeFrancis, J.
    (1984) The Chinese language: Fact and fantasy. University of Hawaii Press. 10.1515/9780824840303
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9780824840303 [Google Scholar]
  16. Fine, A. B., & Jaeger, T. F.
    (2016) The role of verb repetition in cumulative structural priming in comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(9), 1362.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Fine, A. B., Jaeger, T. F., Farmer, T. A., & Qian, T.
    (2013) Rapid expectation adaptation during syntactic comprehension. PloS One, 8(10), e77661. 10.1371/journal.pone.0077661
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0077661 [Google Scholar]
  18. Frank, S. L., Fernandez Monsalve, I., Thompson, R. L., & Vigliocco, G.
    (2013) Reading time data for evaluating broad-coverage models of English sentence processing. Behavior research methods, 451, 1182–1190. 10.3758/s13428‑012‑0313‑y
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0313-y [Google Scholar]
  19. Gao, X. Y., Li, M. F., Chou, T. L., & Wu, J. T.
    (2016) Comparing the Frequency Effect Between the Lexical Decision and Naming Tasks in Chinese. JoVE (Journal of Visualized Experiments), (110), e53815–e53815.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Graf Estes, K., Evans, J. L., Alibali, M. W., & Saffran, J. R.
    (2007) Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Statistical segmentation and word learning. Psychological science, 18(3), 254–260. 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.2007.01885.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01885.x [Google Scholar]
  21. Kuo, W. J., Yeh, T. C., Lee, C. Y., Wu, Y. T., Chou, C. C., Ho, L. T., … & Hsieh, J. C.
    (2003) Frequency effects of Chinese character processing in the brain: an event-related fMRI study. Neuroimage, 18(3), 720–730. 10.1016/S1053‑8119(03)00015‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00015-6 [Google Scholar]
  22. Leong, C. K.
    (1997) Paradigmatic analysis of Chinese word reading: Research findings and classroom practices. InCross-language studies of learning to read and spell (pp.379–417). Springer Netherlands. 10.1007/978‑94‑017‑1197‑5_20
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-1197-5_20 [Google Scholar]
  23. McBride-Chang, C., Bialystok, E., Chong, K. K., & Li, Y.
    (2004) Levels of phonological awareness in three cultures. Journal of experimental child psychology, 89(2), 93–111. 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.05.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2004.05.001 [Google Scholar]
  24. Meng, X., Shu, H., & Zhou, X.
    (2000) The relationship between tasks in Chinese reading developmental research. Psychological Science (China). 23(6), 677–681
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Pacton, S., Perruchet, P., Fayol, M., & Cleeremans, A.
    (2001) Implicit learning out of the lab: the case of orthographic regularities. Journal of experimental psychology: General, 130(3), 401. 10.1037/0096‑3445.130.3.401
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.401 [Google Scholar]
  26. Patterson, K., Plaut, D. C., Mcclelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., Behrmann, M., & Hodges, J. R.
    (1996) Connections and disconnections: A connectionist account of surface dyslexia. Neural Modeling of Brain and Cognitive Disorders (pp.177–199). 10.1142/9789812819819_0008
    https://doi.org/10.1142/9789812819819_0008 [Google Scholar]
  27. Perfetti, C. A., & Zhang, S.
    (1991) Phonological processes in reading Chinese characters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17(4), 633–643.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Redington, M., & Chater, N.
    (1997) Probabilistic and distributional approaches to language acquisition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(7), 273–281. 10.1016/S1364‑6613(97)01081‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01081-4 [Google Scholar]
  29. Rohde, D.
    (2005) Linger experiment presentation software. tedlab.mit.edu/~dr/Linger/
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Rueckl, J. G., Paz-Alonso, P. M., Molfese, P. J., Kuo, W. J., Bick, A., Frost, S. J., … & Lee, J. R.
    (2015) Universal brain signature of proficient reading: Evidence from four contrasting languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(50), 15510–15515. 10.1073/pnas.1509321112
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1509321112 [Google Scholar]
  31. Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L.
    (1996) Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 2741, 5294. 10.1126/science.274.5294.1926
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.274.5294.1926 [Google Scholar]
  32. Schielzeth, H., Dingemanse, N. J., Nakagawa, S., Westneat, D. F., Allegue, H., Teplitsky, C., … & Araya-Ajoy, Y. G.
    (2020) Robustness of linear mixed-effects models to violations of distributional assumptions. Methods in ecology and evolution, 11(9), 1141–1152. 10.1111/2041‑210X.13434
    https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.13434 [Google Scholar]
  33. Shu, H.
    (2003) Chinese writing system and learning to read. International Journal of Psychology, 38(5), 274–285. 10.1080/00207590344000060
    https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590344000060 [Google Scholar]
  34. Tan, L. H., Liu, H. L., Perfetti, C. a, Spinks, J. a, Fox, P. T., & Gao, J. H.
    (2001) The neural system underlying Chinese logograph reading. NeuroImage, 13(5), 836–46. 10.1006/nimg.2001.0749
    https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0749 [Google Scholar]
  35. Tan, L. H., Spinks, J. a, Gao, J. H., Liu, H. L., Perfetti, C. a, Xiong, J., … Fox, P. T.
    (2000) Brain activation in the processing of Chinese characters and words: a functional MRI study. Human brain mapping, 10(1), 16–27. 10.1002/(SICI)1097‑0193(200005)10:1<16::AID‑HBM30>3.0.CO;2‑M
    https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(200005)10:1<16::AID-HBM30>3.0.CO;2-M [Google Scholar]
  36. Wang, T., Cooper Borkenhagen, M., Barker, M., & Seidenberg, M. S.
    (2022) Meanings within meanings: skilled readers activate irrelevant meanings of radicals in Chinese. Reading and Writing, 35(6), 1381–1399. 10.1007/s11145‑022‑10260‑y
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-022-10260-y [Google Scholar]
  37. Yang, J., Cai, Q., & Tian, X.
    (2020) How do we segment text? Two-stage chunking operation in reading. eNeuro, 111, 0425. 10.1523/ENEURO.0425‑19.2020
    https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0425-19.2020 [Google Scholar]
  38. Zhou, X., Shu, H., Bi, Y., & Shi, D.
    (1999) Is there phonologically mediated access to lexical semantics in reading Chinese. Reading Chinese Script: A Cognitive Analysis, 135–171.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ijchl.00020.wan
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/ijchl.00020.wan
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): character-level semantics; reading in Chinese; transitional probability
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error