1887
Volume 17, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1572-0373
  • E-ISSN: 1572-0381
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

When individuals learn from what others tell them, the information is subject to transmission error that does not arise in learning from direct experience. Yet evidence shows that humans consistently prefer this apparently more unreliable source of information. We examine the effect this preference has in cases where the information concerns a judgment on others’ behaviour and is used to establish cooperation in a society. We present a spatial model confirming that cooperation can be sustained by gossip containing a high degree of uncertainty. Accuracy alone does not predict the value of information in evolutionary terms; the impact of information on behavioural outcomes, must also be considered. We then show that once relevance is incorporated as a criterion, second-hand information can no longer be discounted on the basis of its poor fidelity alone. Finally we show that the relative importance of accuracy and relevance depends on factors of life history and demography.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/is.17.1.01mit
2016-09-26
2024-10-11
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Abbink, K. , Brandts, J. , Herrmann, B. , and Orzen, H.
    (2012) Parochial altruism in inter-group conflicts. Economics Letters, 117(1):45–48.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Alexander, R. D.
    (1987) The Biology of Moral Systems. Foundations of Human Behavior. Aldine de Gruyter, New York.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Alizon, S. and Taylor, P.
    (2008) Empty sites can promote altruistic behaviour. Evolution, 62(6):1335–1344.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Asch, S. E.
    (1956) Studies of independence and conformity: I. a minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 70(9):1–70.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Axelrod, R. and Hamilton, W. D.
    (1981) The evolution of cooperation. Science, 211:1390–1396.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Dawkins, R.
    (1976) Hierarchical organisation: A candidate principle for ethology. In Bateson, P. P. G. and Hinde, R. A. , editors, Growing Points in Ethology, pages7–54. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Dessalles, J.-L.
    (2007) Why we talk: the evolutionary origins of language. Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Enquist, M. and Leimar, O.
    (1993) The evolution of cooperation in mobile organisms. Animal Behaviour, 45:747–757.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Fahle, M. and Poggio, T.
    (1981) Visual hyperacuity: Spatiotemporal interpolation in human vision. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 213(1193):451–477.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Gergely, G. , Bekkering, H. , and Király, I.
    (2002) Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415:755.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Gergely, G. and Csibra, G.
    (2006) Sylvia’s recipe: the role of imitation and pedagogy in the transmission of cultural knowledge, pages229–255. Roots of Human Sociality: Culture, Cognition and Interaction. Berg, New York.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Goldberg, D. and Deb, K.
    (1991) A comparative analysis of selection schemes used in genetic algorithms. Urbana, 51:61801–2996.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Gruber, T. , Poisot, T. , Zuberbühler, K. , Hoppitt, W. , and Hobaiter, C.
    (2015) The spread of a novel behavior in wild chimpanzees: New insights into the ape cultural mind. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 8(2):e1017164.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Hardin, G.
    (1968) The tragedy of the commons. Science, 162:1243–1248.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Henrich, J. and McElreath, R.
    (2003) The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 12(3):123–135.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Hess, N. and Hagen, E.
    (2006) Psychological adaptations for assessing gossip veracity. Human Nature, 17(3):337–354.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Jacobs, R. A. , Jordan, M. I. , Nowlan, S. J. , and Hinton, G. E.
    (1991) Adaptive mixtures of local experts. Neural computation, 3(1):79–87.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Kao, A. B. and Couzin, I. D.
    (2014) Decision accuracy in complex environments is often maximized by small group sizes. Proceedings ofthe Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1784).
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Kokko, H.
    (1997) Evolutionarily stable strategies of age-dependent sexual advertisement. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 41(2):99–107.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. McElreath, R. and Boyd, R.
    (2007) Mathematical models of social evolution: A guide for the perplexed. University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Meltzoff, A. N.
    (1988) Infant imitation after a 1-week delay: Long-term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli. Developmental psychology, 24(4):470.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Milinski, M. , Semmann, D. , and Krambeck, H.-J.
    (2006) Reputation helps solve the tragedy of the commons. Nature, 415(6870).
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Moravec, H. P.
    (1988) Sensor fusion in certainty grids for mobile robots. AI magazine, 9(2):61.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Nagell, K. , Olguin, R. S. , and Tomasello, M.
    (1993) Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (pan troglodytes) and human children (homo sapiens). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 107(2):174–186.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Nakamura, M. and Masuda, N.
    (2011) Indirect reciprocity under incomplete observation. PLoS Computational Biology, 7(7):e1002113.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Nowak, M. A. and Sigmund, K.
    (1998) Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature, 393:573–577.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. (2005) Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437:1291–1298.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Ohtsuki, H. and Iwasa, Y.
    (2006) The leading eight: Social norms that can maintain cooperation by indirect reciprocity. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 239(4):435–444.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Ohtsuki, H. , Iwasa, Y. , and Nowak, M. A.
    (2009) Indirect reciprocity provides only a narrow margin of efficiency for costly punishment. Nature, 457(7225):79–82.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Rand, D. G. , Nowak, M. A. , Fowler, J. H. , and Christakis, N. A.
    (2014) Static network structure can stabilize human cooperation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(48):17093–17098.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Rauwolf, P. , Mitchell, D. , and Bryson, J. J.
    (2015) Value homophily benefits cooperation but motivates employing incorrect social information. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 367(0):246–261.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Richerson, P. J. and Boyd, R.
    (2005) Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago University Press, Chicago.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Roberts, G.
    (2008) Evolution of direct and indirect reciprocity. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275(1631):173–179.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Schelling, T.
    (1960) The strategy of conflict. Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Scott-Phillips, T. C.
    (2008) Defining biological communication. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 21(2):387–395.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Sommerfeld, R. D. , Krambeck, H. J. , Semmann, D. , and Milinski, M.
    (2007) Gossip as an alternative for direct observation in games of indirect reciprocity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(44):17435–17440.
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Sperber, D. and Wilson, D.
    (1995) Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Wiley.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Taylor, D. J.
    (2014) Evolution of the Social Contract. PhD thesis, Department of Computer Science, University of Bath.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Tomasello, M.
    (1999) The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Trivers, R. L.
    (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46(1):35–57.
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Čače, I. and Bryson, J. J.
    (2007) Agent based modelling of communication costs: Why information can be free. In Lyon, C. , Nehaniv, C. L. , and Cangelosi, A. , editors, Emergence and Evolution of Linguistic Communication, pages305–322. Springer, London.
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Whiten, A. , McGuigan, N. , Marshall-Pescini, S. , and Hopper, L. M.
    (2009) Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528):2417–2428.
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Williams, G. C.
    (1966) Adaptation and Natural Selection. Princeton University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Yoo, J. H.
    (2009) The power of sharing negative information in a dyadic context. Communication Reports, 22(1):29–40.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/is.17.1.01mit
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/is.17.1.01mit
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): cooperation; gossip; reputation; social norm
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error