1887
Volume 19, Issue 1-2
  • ISSN 1572-0373
  • E-ISSN: 1572-0381
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Language is a cognitively demanding human trait, but it is also a fundamentally cooperative enterprise that rests on the motivation to share information. Great apes possess many of the cognitive prerequisites for language, but largely lack the motivation to share information. Callitrichids (including marmosets and tamarins) are highly vocal monkeys that are more distantly related to humans than great apes are, but like humans, they are cooperative breeders and all group members help raising offspring. Among primates, this rearing system is correlated with proactive prosociality, which can be expressed as motivation to share information. We therefore propose that the unique coincidence of these two components in humans set the stage for language evolution: The cognitive component inherited from our great ape-like ancestors, and the motivational one added convergently as a result of cooperative breeding. We evaluate this scenario based on a review of callitrichd vocal communication and show that furthermore, they possess many of the mechanistic elements emphasized by the mirror system hypothesis of language evolution. We end by highlighting how more systematic phylogenetic comparisons will enable us to further promote our understanding of the role of cooperative breeding during language evolution.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/is.17026.bur
2018-09-17
2025-04-28
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Agamaite, J. A., Chang, C. -J., Osmanski, M. S., & Wang, X.
    (2015) A quantitative acoustic analysis of the vocal repertoire of the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 138(5), 2906–2928.10.1121/1.4934268
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4934268 [Google Scholar]
  2. Arbib, M. A.
    (2012) How the brain got language: The mirror system hypothesis (Vol.16). OUPUSA.10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199896684.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:osobl/9780199896684.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  3. Bardi, M., Petto, A. J., & Lee‐Parritz, D. E.
    (2001) Parental failure in captive cotton‐top tamarins (Saguinus Oedipus). American Journal of Primatology, 54(3), 159–169.10.1002/ajp.1020
    https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1020 [Google Scholar]
  4. Bezerra, B. M., & Souto, A.
    (2008) Structure and usage of the vocal repertoire of Callithrix jacchus. International Journal of Primatolgoy (29), 671–701.10.1007/s10764‑008‑9250‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-008-9250-0 [Google Scholar]
  5. Borjon, J. I., & Ghazanfar, A. A.
    (2014) Convergent evolution of vocal cooperation without convergent evolution of brain size. Brain, Behavior and Evolution, 84(2), 93–102.10.1159/000365346
    https://doi.org/10.1159/000365346 [Google Scholar]
  6. Brown, G. R., Almond, R. E. A., & van Bergen, Y.
    (2004) Begging, stealing and offering: food transfer in non-human primates. Advances in the Study of Behaviour, 34, 265–295.10.1016/S0065‑3454(04)34007‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(04)34007-6 [Google Scholar]
  7. Burkart, J., Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S., & van Schaik, C.
    (2012) Even simple forms of social learning rely on intention attribution in marmoset monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126(2), 129.10.1037/a0026025
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026025 [Google Scholar]
  8. Burkart, J. M., Allon, O., Amici, F., Fichtel, C., Finkenwirth, C., Heschl, A., … van Schaik, C. P.
    (2014) The evolutionary origin of human hyper-cooperation. Nature Communications, 5, 4747.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Burkart, J. M., Hrdy, S. B., & van Schaik, C. P.
    (2009) Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 18, 175–186.10.1002/evan.20222
    https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20222 [Google Scholar]
  10. Burkart, J. M., Schubiger, M. N., & van Schaik, C. P.
    (2017) The evolution of general intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1–65.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Burkart, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P.
    (2010) Cognitive consequences of cooperative breeding in primates. Animal Cognition, 13, 1–19.10.1007/s10071‑009‑0263‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-009-0263-7 [Google Scholar]
  12. (2016) The cooperative breeding perspective helps pinning down when uniquely human evolutionary processes are necessaryBehavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, e34.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Caine, N. G., Addington, R. L., & Windfelder, T. L.
    (1995) Factors affecting the rates of food calls given by red-bellied tamarins. Animal Behaviour, 50(1), 53–60.10.1006/anbe.1995.0220
    https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1995.0220 [Google Scholar]
  14. Campbell, M., & Snowdon, C. T.
    (2007) Vocal response of captive-reared Saguinus oedipus during mobbing. International Journal of Primatology, 28(2), 257–270.10.1007/s10764‑007‑9123‑y
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-007-9123-y [Google Scholar]
  15. Caro, T. M., & Hauser, M. D.
    (1992) Is there teaching in nonhuman animals?Quarterly Review of Biology, 67, 151–172.10.1086/417553
    https://doi.org/10.1086/417553 [Google Scholar]
  16. Cäsar, C., & Zuberbühler, K.
    (2012) Referential alarm calling behaviour in New World primates. Current Zoology, 58(5), 680–697.10.1093/czoolo/58.5.680
    https://doi.org/10.1093/czoolo/58.5.680 [Google Scholar]
  17. Chisholm, J. S.
    (2003) Uncertainty, contingency, and attachment: A life history theory of Theory of Mind. InK. Sterelny & J. Fitness (Eds.), From mating to mentality. Evaluating Evolutionary Psychology. (pp.125–154). New York: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Chow, C. P., Mitchell, J. F., & Miller, C. T.
    (2015) Vocal turn-taking in a non-human primate is learned during ontogeny. Proc. R. Soc. B, 282(1807), 20150069.10.1098/rspb.2015.0069
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.0069 [Google Scholar]
  19. Clay, Z., Smith, C. L., & Blumstein, D. T.
    (2012) Food-associated vocalizations in mammals and birds: what do these calls really mean?Animal Behaviour, 83(2), 323–330.10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.12.008 [Google Scholar]
  20. Cleveland, J., & Snowdon, C. T.
    (1982) The complex vocal repertoire of the adult cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus oedipus). Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 58, 231–270.10.1111/j.1439‑0310.1982.tb00320.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1982.tb00320.x [Google Scholar]
  21. de la Torre, S., & Snowdon, C. T.
    (2009) Dialects in pygmy marmosets? Population variation in call structure. American Journal of Primatology, 71(4), 333–342.10.1002/ajp.20657
    https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20657 [Google Scholar]
  22. Dell’Mour, V., Range, F., & Huber, L.
    (2009) Social learning and mother’s behavior in manipulative tasks in infant marmosets. American Journal of Primatology, 71(6), 503–509.10.1002/ajp.20682
    https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20682 [Google Scholar]
  23. Digby, L. J., Ferrari, S. F., & Saltzman, W.
    (2007) Callitrichines: The role of competition in cooperatively breeding species. InC. J. Campbell, A. Fuentes, K. C. MacKinnon, M. A. Panger, & S. K. Bearder (Eds.), Primates in perspective (pp.85–105). New York: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Eliades, S. J., & Miller, C. T.
    (2016) Marmoset vocal communication: Behavior and neurobiology. Developmental neurobiology.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Elowson, A. M., Snowdon, C. T., & Lazaro-Perea, C.
    (1998a) ‘Babbling’ and social context in infant monkeys: parallels to human infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 35–43.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. (1998b) Infant ‘babbling’ in a nonhuman primate: complex seqeunces of vocal behavior. Behaviour, 135, 643–664.10.1163/156853998792897905
    https://doi.org/10.1163/156853998792897905 [Google Scholar]
  27. Fitch, W. T.
    (2005) The evolution of language: a comparative review. Biology and Philosopy, 20, 193–230.10.1007/s10539‑005‑5597‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-005-5597-1 [Google Scholar]
  28. Garber, P. A.
    (1997) One for all and breeding for one: cooperation and competition as a tamarin reproductive strategy. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 5(6), 187–199.10.1002/(SICI)1520‑6505(1997)5:6<187::AID‑EVAN1>3.0.CO;2‑A
    https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1520-6505(1997)5:6<187::AID-EVAN1>3.0.CO;2-A [Google Scholar]
  29. Grice, H. P.
    (1975) Logic and conversation. InP. Cole (Ed.), Syntax and Semantics, Vol3, (pp.41–58). New York: Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Gros-Louis, J.
    (2004) The function of food-associated calls in white-faced capuchin monkeys, Cebus capucinus, from the perspective of the signaller. Animal Behaviour, 67(3), 431–440.10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.04.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2003.04.009 [Google Scholar]
  31. Guerreiro Martins, E. M., & Burkart, J. M.
    (2013) Common marmosets preferentially share difficult to obtain food items. Folia Primatologica, 84(3–5), 281–282.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. (in prep.). Teaching in marmosets:contingent on age or skill levels of immatures?
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Guerreiro Martins, E. M., Moura, A. C., Finkenwirth, C., & Burkart, J. M.
    (subm). Food sharing in three species of callitrichid monkeys (Callithrix jacchus, Leontopithecus chrysomelas & Sanguinus midas): Individual differences and interspecific variation. Animal Behaviour.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Gultekin, Y. B., & Hage, S. R.
    (2017) Limiting parental feedback disrupts vocal development in marmoset monkeys. Nature Communications, 8.10.1038/ncomms14046
    https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms14046 [Google Scholar]
  35. Hawkes, K.
    (2014) Primate sociality to human cooperation. Human Nature, 25(1), 28–48.10.1007/s12110‑013‑9184‑x
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9184-x [Google Scholar]
  36. Horn, L., Scheer, C., Bugnyar, T., & Massen, J. J.
    (2016) Proactive prosociality in a cooperatively breeding corvid, the azure-winged magpie (Cyanopica cyana). Biology letters, 12(10), 20160649.10.1098/rsbl.2016.0649
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2016.0649 [Google Scholar]
  37. Hrdy, S.
    (2005a) Comes the child before the man: how cooperative breeding and prolonged postweaning dependence shaped human potentials. InB. Hewlett & M. Lamb (Eds.), Hunter gatherer childhood. (pp.65–91). Psicataway: Transactions.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. (2005b) Evolutionary context of human development: The cooperative breeding model. InC. S. Carter, L. Ahnert, K. E. Grossmann, S. B. Hrdy, M. E. Lamb, S. W. Porges, & N. Sachser (Eds.), Attachment and Bonding: A New Synthesis; From the 92nd Dahlem Workshop Report (pp.9–32): MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. (2009) Mothers & Others: The Evolutionary Origins of Mutual Understanding. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Hrdy, S. B.
    (2016) Variable postpartum responsiveness among humans and other primates with “cooperative breeding”: A comparative and evolutionary perspective. Hormones and Behavior, 77, 272–283.10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.10.016
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.10.016 [Google Scholar]
  41. Humle, T., & Snowdon, C. T.
    (2008) Socially biased learning in the acquisition of a complex foraging task in juvenile cottontop tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Animal Behaviour, 27(1), 267–277.10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.05.021
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.05.021 [Google Scholar]
  42. Jaeggi, A., Burkart, J. M., & van Schaik, C. P.
    (2010) On the psychology of cooperation in humans and other primates: The natural history of food sharing and experimental evidence of prosociality. PhilTransB12(365), 2723–2735.
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Kirchhof, J., & Hammerschmidt, K.
    (2006) Functionally referential alarm calls in tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis and Saguinus mystax)–evidence from playback experiments. Ethology, 112(4), 346–354.10.1111/j.1439‑0310.2006.01165.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01165.x [Google Scholar]
  44. Kitzmann, C. D., & Caine, N. G.
    (2009) Marmoset (Callithrix geoffroyi) Food‐Associated Calls are Functionally Referential. Ethology, 115(5), 439–448.10.1111/j.1439‑0310.2009.01622.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2009.01622.x [Google Scholar]
  45. Kline, M. A.
    (2014) How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1–70.10.1017/S0140525X14000090
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14000090 [Google Scholar]
  46. Kupferberg, A., Glasauer, S., & Burkart, J. M.
    (2013) Do robots have goals? How agent cues influence action understanding in non-human primates. Behavioural Brain Research.10.1016/j.bbr.2013.01.047
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2013.01.047 [Google Scholar]
  47. Leighton, G. M.
    (2017) Cooperative breeding influences the number and type of vocalizations in avian lineages. Proc. R. Soc. B, 284, 20171508.10.1098/rspb.2017.1508
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2017.1508 [Google Scholar]
  48. MacLean, E. L., Hare, B., Nunn, C. L., Addessi, E., Amici, F., Anderson, R. C., … Barnard, A. M.
    (2014) The evolution of self-control. PNAS, 111(20), E2140–E2148.10.1073/pnas.1323533111
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1323533111 [Google Scholar]
  49. MacLean, E. L., Matthews, L. J., Hare, B., Nunn, C. L., Anderson, R. C., Aureli, F., … Emery, N. J.
    (2012) How does cognition evolve? Phylogenetic comparative psychology. Animal Cognition, 15(2), 223–238.10.1007/s10071‑011‑0448‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-011-0448-8 [Google Scholar]
  50. Masataka, N.
    (1982) A field study on the vocalizations of Goeldi’s monkeys (Callimico goeldii). Primates, 23, 206–219.10.1007/BF02381161
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02381161 [Google Scholar]
  51. McComb, K., & Semple, S.
    (2005) Coevolution of vocal communication and sociality in primates. Biology Letters, 1(4), 381–385.10.1098/rsbl.2005.0366
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2005.0366 [Google Scholar]
  52. Melis, A. P., & Warneken, F.
    (2016) The psychology of cooperation: Insights from chimpanzees and children. Evolutionary Anthropology, 25(6), 297–305.10.1002/evan.21507
    https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21507 [Google Scholar]
  53. Milward, S. J., Kita, S., & Apperly, I. A.
    (2016) Individual differences in children’s co-representation of self and other in joint action. Child Development.
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Miss, F. M., & Burkart, J. M.
    (2018) Corepresentation During Joint Action in Marmoset Monkeys (Callithrix jacchus). Psychological Science, 10.1177/0956797618772046.
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797618772046 [Google Scholar]
  55. Moura, A. C., Nunes, H. G., & Langguth, A.
    (2010) Food Sharing in Lion Tamarins (Leontopithecus chrysomelas): Does Foraging Difficulty Affect Investment in Young by Breeders and Helpers?International Journal of Primatology, 31(5), 848–862.10.1007/s10764‑010‑9432‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-010-9432-4 [Google Scholar]
  56. Noble, J.
    (2000) Cooperation, competition and the evolution of prelinguistic communication. The Emergence of Language, 40–61.10.1017/CBO9780511606441.004
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511606441.004 [Google Scholar]
  57. Pistorio, A. L., Vintch, B., & Wang, X.
    (2006) Acoustic analysis of vocal development in a New World primate, the common marmoet (Callithrix jacchus). J Aocust Soc Am, 120(3), 1655–1670.10.1121/1.2225899
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2225899 [Google Scholar]
  58. Pollick, A. S., Gouzoules, H., & de Waal, F. B. M.
    (2005) Audience effects on food calls in captive brown capuchin monkeys, Cebus apella. Animal Behaviour, 70, 1273–1281.10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.03.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.03.007 [Google Scholar]
  59. Rapaport, L. G.
    (2011) Progressive parenting behavior in wild golden lion tamarins. Behavioral Ecology.10.1093/beheco/arr055
    https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arr055 [Google Scholar]
  60. Roupe, S. L., Pistorio, A., & Wang, X.
    (2003) Vocal plasticity induced by auditory deprivation in the common marmoset. Paper presented atthe Society for Neuroscience, New Orleans, Nov. 11.
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Roy, S., Miller, C. T., Gottsch, D., & Wang, X.
    (2011) Vocal control by the common marmoset in the presence of interfering noise. The Journal of Experimental Biology, 214(21), 3619–3629.10.1242/jeb.056101
    https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.056101 [Google Scholar]
  62. Ruch, H., Zürcher, Y., & Burkart, J. M.
    (2018) The function of vocal accommodation in humans and other primates. Biological Reviews, 93(2), 996–1013.10.1111/brv.12382
    https://doi.org/10.1111/brv.12382 [Google Scholar]
  63. Rukstalis, M., Fite, J. E., & French, J. A.
    (2003) Social Change Affects Vocal Structure in a Callitrichid Primate (Callithrix kuhlii). Ethology, 109(4), 327–340.10.1046/j.1439‑0310.2003.00875.x
    https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1439-0310.2003.00875.x [Google Scholar]
  64. Rukstalis, M., & French, J. A.
    (2005) Vocal buffering of the stress response: exposure to conspecific vocalizations moderates urinary cortisol excretion in isolated marmosets. Hormones and Behavior, 47(1), 1–7.10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.09.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2004.09.004 [Google Scholar]
  65. Shelley, E. L. & Blumstein, D. T.
    (2004) The evolution of vocal alarm communication in rodents. Behavioral Ecology, 16(1), 169–77.10.1093/beheco/arh148
    https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arh148 [Google Scholar]
  66. Sebanz, N., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W.
    (2003) Representing others’ actions: just like one’s own?Cognition, 88(3), B11–B21.10.1016/S0010‑0277(03)00043‑X
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(03)00043-X [Google Scholar]
  67. Snowdon, C. T.
    (2013) Language parallels in New World primatesAnimal Models of Speech and Language Disorders (pp.241–261). New York: Springer.10.1007/978‑1‑4614‑8400‑4_9
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-8400-4_9 [Google Scholar]
  68. (2001) Social processes in communication and cognition in callitrichid monkeys: a review. Animal Cognition, 4, 247–257.10.1007/s100710100094
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s100710100094 [Google Scholar]
  69. (2017a) Cultural Phenomena in Cooperatively Breeding Primates. InJ. M. Causadias, E. H. Telzer, & N. A. Gonzales (Eds.), The Handbook of Culture and Biology: John Wiley & Sons.10.1002/9781119181361.ch6
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9781119181361.ch6 [Google Scholar]
  70. (2017b) Learning from monkey “talk”. Science, 355(6330), 1120–1122.10.1126/science.aam7443
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aam7443 [Google Scholar]
  71. (2017c) Vocal Communication in Family-Living and Pair-Bonded PrimatesPrimate Hearing and Communication (pp.141–174): Springer.10.1007/978‑3‑319‑59478‑1_6
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-59478-1_6 [Google Scholar]
  72. Snowdon, C. T., & Cleveland, J.
    (1984) “Conversations” among pygmy marmosets. American Journal of Primatology7, 15–20.10.1002/ajp.1350070104
    https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350070104 [Google Scholar]
  73. Snowdon, C. T., & Cronin, K. A.
    (2007) Cooperative breeders do cooperate. Behavioural Processes, 76(2), 138–141.10.1016/j.beproc.2007.01.016
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2007.01.016 [Google Scholar]
  74. Snowdon, C. T., & Elowson, A. M.
    (2001) ‘Babbling’ in pygmy marmosets: development after infancy. Behaviour, 138(10), 1235–1248.10.1163/15685390152822193
    https://doi.org/10.1163/15685390152822193 [Google Scholar]
  75. Snowdon, C. T., & Roskos, T. R.
    (2017) Stick-weaving: Innovative behavior in tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). Journal of Comparative Psychology, 131(2), 174.10.1037/com0000071
    https://doi.org/10.1037/com0000071 [Google Scholar]
  76. Solomon, N. G., & French, J. A.
    (1997) Cooperative Breeding in Mammals. New York: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  77. Suzuki, W., Banno, T., Miyakawa, N., Abe, H., Goda, N., & Ichinohe, N.
    (2015) Mirror neurons in a new world monkey, common marmoset. Frontiers in neuroscience, 9.10.3389/fnins.2015.00459
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2015.00459 [Google Scholar]
  78. Takahashi, D. Y., Fenley, A. R., & Ghazanfar, A. A.
    (2016) Early development of turn-taking with parents shapes vocal acoustics in infant marmoset monkeys. Phil. Trans. B, 371(1693), 20150370.10.1098/rstb.2015.0370
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0370 [Google Scholar]
  79. Takahashi, D. Y., Fenley, A. R., Teramoto, Y., Narayanan, D. Z., Borjon, J. I., Holmes, P., & Ghazanfar, A. A.
    (2015) The developmental dynamics of marmoset monkey vocal production. Science, 349(6249), 734–738.10.1126/science.aab1058
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aab1058 [Google Scholar]
  80. Takahashi, D. Y., Liao, D. A., & Ghazanfar, A. A.
    (2017) Vocal Learning via Social Reinforcement by Infant Marmoset Monkeys. Current Biology.10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.05.004 [Google Scholar]
  81. Takahashi, D. Y., Narayanan, D. Z., & Ghazanfar, A. A.
    (2013) Coupled oscillator dynamics of vocal turn-taking in monkeys. Current Biology, 23(21), 2162–2168.10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.09.005 [Google Scholar]
  82. Tomasello, M.
    (2017) What did we learn from the ape language studies?. In: Hare, B. & Yamamoto, S. (eds.) Bonobos: Unique in Mind, Brain, and Behavior. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 95–105
    [Google Scholar]
  83. (2008) Origins of human communication. Cambridge MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Tomasello, M., & Gonzalez-Cabrera, I.
    (2017) The role of ontogeny in the evolution of human cooperation. Human Nature, 1–15.
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Vitale, A., Zanzoni, M., Queyras, A., & Chiarotti, F.
    (2003) Degree of social contact affects the emission of food calls in the common marmoset (Callithrix jacchus). American Journal of Primatology, 59, 21–28.10.1002/ajp.10060
    https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.10060 [Google Scholar]
  86. Voelkl, B., & Huber, L.
    (2007) Imitation as faithful copying of a novel techinque in marmoset monkeys. PLoS ONE, 2(7), e611.10.1371/journal.pone.0000611
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0000611 [Google Scholar]
  87. Weiss, D. J., Garibaldi, B. T., & Hauser, M. D.
    (2001) The production and perception of long calls by cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus): acoustic analyses and playback experiments. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 115(3), 258.10.1037/0735‑7036.115.3.258
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.115.3.258 [Google Scholar]
  88. Wen, T., & Hsieh, S.
    (2015) Neuroimaging of the joint Simon effect with believed biological and non-biological co-actors. Frontiers in human neuroscience, 9.10.3389/fnhum.2015.00483
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00483 [Google Scholar]
  89. Zuberbühler, K.
    (2011) Cooperative breeding and the evolution of vocal flexibility. InM. Tallerman & K. Gibson (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Evolution.
    [Google Scholar]
  90. Zürcher, Y., & Burkart, J. M.
    (2017) Evidence for dialects in three captive populations of common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). International Journal of Primatolgoy, 38(4), 780–793.10.1007/s10764‑017‑9979‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-017-9979-4 [Google Scholar]
  91. (in prep). Translocation experiments provide evidence for vocal accommodation learing in marmosets.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/is.17026.bur
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/is.17026.bur
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error