1887
Volume 19, Issue 1-2
  • ISSN 1572-0373
  • E-ISSN: 1572-0381

Abstract

Abstract

We review major developmental evidence on the continuity from action to gesture to word and sign in human children, highlighting the important role of caregivers in the development of multimodal communication. In particular, the basic issues considered here and contributing to the current debate on the origins and development of the language-ready brain are: (1) links between early actions, gestures and words and similarities in representational strategies; (2) importance of multimodal communication and the interplay between gestures and spoken words; (3) interconnections between early actions, gestures and signs. The innovation of this report is in connecting these themes together to relevant findings from studies on children between 6 and 36 months of age and highlighting interesting parallels in studies on ape communicative behavior.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/is.17027.vol
2018-09-17
2025-02-07
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Antinoro Pizzuto, E., Rossini, P., Sallandre, M. A. & Wilkinson, E.
    (2006) Deixis, anaphora and highly iconic structures: Cross-linguistic evidence on American (ASL), French (LSF) and Italian (LIS) signed languages. TISLR9 (pp.475–495). Editora Arara Azul, Brazil.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Arbib, M. A., Ganesh, V. & Gasser, B.
    (2014) Dyadic Brain Modeling, Ontogenetic Ritualization of Gesture in Apes, and the Contributions of Primate Mirror Neuron Systems. Phil Trans Roy Soc B, 369 (1644), 20130414.10.1098/rstb.2013.0414
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0414 [Google Scholar]
  3. Arbib, M. A.
    (2016) Towards a Computational Comparative Neuroprimatology: Framing the language-ready brain. Physics of Life Reviews, 16, 1–54. 10.1016/j.plrev.2015.09.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2015.09.003 [Google Scholar]
  4. Arbib, M. A. & Rizzolatti, G.
    (1997) Neural expectations: A possible evolutionary path from manual skills to language. Communication and Cognition, 29, 393–424.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bates, E., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V.
    (1975) The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merril Palmer Quarterly, 21, 205–226.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Bello, A., Giannantoni, P., Pettenati, P., Stefanini, S. & Caselli, M. C.
    (2012) Assessing lexicon: validation and developmental data of the Picture Naming Game (PiNG), a new picture naming task for toddlers. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 47, 589–602. 10.1111/j.1460‑6984.2012.00168.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00168.x [Google Scholar]
  7. Boyatzis, C. J. & Watson, M. W.
    (1993) Preschool children’s symbolic representation of objects through gestures. Child Development, 64, 729–735. doi: 10.2307/1131214
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1131214 [Google Scholar]
  8. Boyes Braem, P.
    (1981) Features of the handshape in American Sign Language. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, San Diego.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Brentari, D., Di Renzo, A., Keane, J. & Volterra, V.
    (2014) Cognitive, Cultural, and Linguistic Sources of a Handshape Distinction Expressing Agentivity. Topics in Cognitive Science1–29. 10.1111/tops.12123
    https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12123 [Google Scholar]
  10. Capirci, O., Contaldo, A., Caselli, M. C. & Volterra, V.
    (2005) From Action to language through gesture: a longitudinal perspective, Gesture, 5, 155–177.10.1075/gest.5.1‑2.12cap
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.5.1-2.12cap [Google Scholar]
  11. Capirci, O., Cristilli, C., De Angelis, V. & Graziano, M.
    (2011) Learning to use gesture in narratives: developmental trends in formal and semantic gesture competence. InG. Stam & M. Ishino (Eds.) Integrating Gestures (pp.189–200), Amsterdam/Philadelphia, John Benjamins.10.1075/gs.4.16cap
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gs.4.16cap [Google Scholar]
  12. Capirci, O., Iverson, J. M., Pizzuto, E. & Volterra, V.
    (1996) Gestures and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of child language, 23, 645–673.10.1017/S0305000900008989
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008989 [Google Scholar]
  13. Capobianco, E., Antinoro Pizzuto, E. & Devescovi, A.
    (2017) Gesture–speech combinations and early verbal abilities. New longitudinal data during the second year of age. Interaction Studies18(1), 55–76. doi: 10.1075/is.18.1.03cap.
    https://doi.org/10.1075/is.18.1.03cap [Google Scholar]
  14. Caselli, M. C. & Volterra, V.
    (1994) From communication to language in hearing and deaf children. InV. Volterra & C. J. Erting (Eds.) From Gesture to Language in Hearing and Deaf Children (pp.263–277). 2nd ed., Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Caselli, M. C.
    (1990) Communicative gestures and first words. InV. Volterra, & C. J. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to Language in Hearing and Deaf Children (pp.56–67). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.10.1007/978‑3‑642‑74859‑2_6
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-74859-2_6 [Google Scholar]
  16. Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., Stefanini, S. & Volterra, V.
    (2012) Early Action and Gesture ‘‘Vocabulary’’ and Its Relation With Word Comprehension and Production. Child Development, 83, 526–542. 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2011.01727.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01727.x [Google Scholar]
  17. Clark, H. H.
    (2016) Depicting as a method of communication. Psychological review, 123(3), 324–347. 10.1037/rev0000026
    https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000026 [Google Scholar]
  18. Corballis, M. C.
    (2002) From hand to mouth. The origins of language. Princeton, Princeton University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Cuxac, C.
    (2000) La langue des signes française (LSF): les voies de l’iconocité (No. 15–16). Ophrys.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Enfield, N. J.
    (2001) ‘Lip-pointing’: A discussion of form and function with reference to data from Laos. Gesture, 1(2), 185–211.10.1075/gest.1.2.06enf
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.1.2.06enf [Google Scholar]
  21. Goldin-Meadow, S. & Butcher, C.
    (2003) Pointing toward two-word speech in children. InS. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: where language, culture and cognition meet (pp.85–107). Mahwah, N.J.: Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Gretscher, H., Tempelmann, S., Haun, D. B. M., Liebal, K. & Kaminski, J.
    (2017) Prelinguistic human infants and great apes show different communicative strategies in a triadic request situation. PLoS ONE12, 4, e0175227. 10.1371/journal.pone.0175227
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175227 [Google Scholar]
  23. Halina, M., Rossano, F. & Tomasello, M.
    (2013) The ontogenetic ritualization of bonobo gestures. Animal cognition, 16(4), 653–666.10.1007/s10071‑013‑0601‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-013-0601-7 [Google Scholar]
  24. Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O. & Caselli, M. C.
    (1994) From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9, 23–43.10.1016/0885‑2014(94)90018‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(94)90018-3 [Google Scholar]
  25. Iverson, J. M. & Thelen, E.
    (1999) Hand, mouth, and brain: The dynamic emergence of speech and gestures. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6, 19–40.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Kendon, A.
    (2014) Semiotic diversity in utterance production and the concept of ‘language’. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 369, 20130293.10.1098/rstb.2013.0293
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0293 [Google Scholar]
  27. Kita, S.
    (Ed.) (2003) Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Leavens, D. A.
    (2004) Manual deixis in apes and humans. Interaction Studies, 5(3), 387–408.10.1075/is.5.3.05lea
    https://doi.org/10.1075/is.5.3.05lea [Google Scholar]
  29. Liddell, S. K. & Metzger, M.
    (1998) Gesture in sign language discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 30, 657–697.10.1016/S0378‑2166(98)00061‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(98)00061-7 [Google Scholar]
  30. Liebal, K.
    (2016) The ontogeny of great ape gesture – not a simple story: comment on “Towards a computational comparative neuroprimatology: framing the language-ready brain” by M.A. Arbib. Physics of Life Reviews, 16, 85–87.10.1016/j.plrev.2016.01.021
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2016.01.021 [Google Scholar]
  31. Marentette, P. & Nicoladis, E.
    (2012) Does ontogenetic ritualization explain early communicative gestures in human infants?InS. Pika & K. Liebal (Eds.). Developments in Primate Gesture Research (pp.33–53). John Benjamins Publishing Company10.1075/gs.6.03mar
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gs.6.03mar [Google Scholar]
  32. Marentette, P., Pettenati, P., Bello, A. & Volterra, V.
    (2016) Gesture and symbolic representation in Italian and English-speaking Canadian two-year-olds. Child Development, 87(3), 944–961. 10.1111/cdev.12523
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12523 [Google Scholar]
  33. McNeill, D.
    (1992) Hand and mind. What the hands reveal about thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Newman, A. J., Supalla, T., Fernandez, N., Newport, E. L. & Bavelier, D.
    (2015) Neural systems supporting linguistic structure, linguistic experience, and symbolic communication in sign language and gesture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America112(37), 11684–89.10.1073/pnas.1510527112
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1510527112 [Google Scholar]
  35. Occhino, C. & Wilcox, S.
    (2017) Gesture or sign? A categorization problem. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e66. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X15003015
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15003015 [Google Scholar]
  36. Padden, C. A., Meir, I., Hwang, S. O., Lepic, R., Seegers, S. & Sampson, T.
    (2013) Patterned iconicity in sign language lexicons. Gesture, 13(3), 287–308.10.1075/gest.13.3.03pad
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.13.3.03pad [Google Scholar]
  37. Pettenati, P., Stefanini, S. & Volterra, V.
    (2010) Motoric characteristics of representational gestures produced by young children in a naming task. Journal of Child Language, 37(4), 887–911.10.1017/S0305000909990092
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000909990092 [Google Scholar]
  38. Piaget, J.
    (1945) La formation du symbole chez l’enfant: Imitation, jeu et rêve, image et representation. Neuchatel; Paris: Delachaux et Niestlé.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Reddy, V.
    (2003) On being the object of attention: implications for self–other consciousness. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 7(9), 397–402.10.1016/S1364‑6613(03)00191‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00191-8 [Google Scholar]
  40. Russon, A. E. & Andrews, K. A.
    (2011) Pantomime in great apes: evidence and implications. Communicative and Integrative Biology, 4(3), 315–317.10.4161/cib.4.3.14809
    https://doi.org/10.4161/cib.4.3.14809 [Google Scholar]
  41. Pettenati, P., Sekine, K., Congestrì, E. & Volterra, V.
    (2012) A comparative study on representational gestures in Italian and Japanese children. Journal of Non Verbal Behaviour, 36, 149–164. doi: 10.1007/s10919‑011‑0127‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-011-0127-0 [Google Scholar]
  42. Slobin, D.
    (2008) Breaking the Molds: Signed language and the nature of human language. Sign Language Studies, 8(2), 114–130.10.1353/sls.2008.0004
    https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.2008.0004 [Google Scholar]
  43. Sparaci, L. & Volterra, V.
    (2017) Hands shaping communication: From gestures to signs. In: M. Bertolaso & N. Di Stefano (Eds.), The Hand. Studies in Applied Philosophy, Epistemology and Rational Ethics, vol38, (pp.29–54). Springer, Cham.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Stefanini, S., Bello, A., Caselli, M. C., Iverson, J. M. & Volterra, V.
    (2009) Co-Speech gestures in a naming task: Developmental data. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(2), 168–189.10.1080/01690960802187755
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960802187755 [Google Scholar]
  45. Tomasello, M.
    (2006) Why don’t apes point?InN. J. Nicholadis & S. C. Levinson (Eds.), Roots of human sociality: Culture, Cognition, and Interaction (pp.506–524). Oxford: Berg.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. (2008) The origin of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Caselli, M. C., Rinaldi, P., & Sparaci, L.
    (2017) Developmental evidence for continuity from action to gesture to sign/word. Language, Interaction, Acquisition, 8 (1), 13–41. doi: doi:10.1075/lia.8.1.02vol
    https://doi.org/doi:10.1075/lia.8.1.02vol [Google Scholar]
  48. Zukow-Goldring, P.
    (2012) Assisted imitation: First steps in the seed model of language development. Language Sciences, 34, 569–582.10.1016/j.langsci.2012.03.012
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2012.03.012 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/is.17027.vol
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/is.17027.vol
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): actions; child development; communication; gestures; multimodality; signs; words
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error