1887
Volume 18, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1572-0373
  • E-ISSN: 1572-0381
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Social and ecological research and theory are used to elaborate and enrich two important sets of accounts of language origins. One is the interdependence and shared intentionality hypothesis (e.g., Tomasello, 2014a ) of the ways in which humans became cooperative and conforming in ways that other apes did not, eventually leading to language. A second set of accounts addresses the emergence of bipedalism and its connections to language and to many other anatomical, cognitive, and social features that are distinctive in humans. Particular attention is given to the carrying and caretaking of infants. Research and theory challenging common assumptions about the role of conformity in cooperation and conversation are reviewed and integrated into these accounts. Together these varying perspectives point toward a more dialogical, dynamic, and distributed understanding of social interactions and the values that motivate and constrain humans’ social and linguistic skills.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/is.18.1.02hod
2017-07-28
2024-12-10
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Adornetti, I
    (2015) Competition and cooperation in language evolution: A comparison between communication of apes and humans. In F. E’Errico , I. Poggi , J. Vinciarelli , & L. Vincze (Eds.), Conflict and multimodal communication: Social research and machine intelligence (pp. 91–101). New York: Springer.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Allen, J. M. , Worman, C. O. , Light, J. E. , & Reed, D. L
    (2013) Parasitic lice help to fill in the gaps of early hominid history. In J. F. Brinkworth & K. Pechenkina (Eds.), Primates, pathogens and evolution (pp. 161–186). New York: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978‑1‑4614‑7181‑3_6
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-7181-3_6 [Google Scholar]
  3. Amaral, L. Q
    (2008) Mechanical analysis of infant carrying in hominoids. Naturwissenschaften, 95, 281–292. doi: 10.1007/s00114‑007‑0325‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-007-0325-0 [Google Scholar]
  4. Asch, S. E
    (1956) Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs, 70, no. 9 (Whole No. 416). doi: 10.1037/h0093718
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093718 [Google Scholar]
  5. Berger, J. , & Heath, C
    (2008) Who drives divergence? Identity signaling, outgroup similarity, and the abandonment of cultural tastes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95, 593–607. doi: 10.1037/0022‑3514.95.3.593
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.95.3.593 [Google Scholar]
  6. Boesch, C
    (2005) Joint cooperative hunting among wild chimpanzees in the Taï National Park. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 692–693. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X05230121
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X05230121 [Google Scholar]
  7. Bolhuis, J. J. , Tattersall, I. , Chomsky, N. , & Berwick, R. C
    (2014) How could language have evolved?PLOS Biology, 12, 8, e1001934. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001934
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001934 [Google Scholar]
  8. Boyd, R. , Richerson, P. J. , & Henrich, J
    (2011) The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences , 108, 10918–10925.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Bråten, S
    (2000) Essays on dialogue in infant and adult. Bergen, Norway: Sigma.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. (2004) Hominin infant decentration hypothesis: Mirror neurons system adapted to subserve mother-centered participation. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 508–509. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X0427011X
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0427011X [Google Scholar]
  11. (2007) Participant perception of the speaker by conversation partners and infant learners: On the origins of (m)othercentred participation. Paper presented at theSymposium of the Distributed Language Group, Language dynamics and the phenomenology of individual experience, Grimstad, Norway.
  12. (2009) The intersubjective mirror in infant learning and evolution of speech. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/aicr.76
    https://doi.org/10.1075/aicr.76 [Google Scholar]
  13. Carvalho, S. , Biro, D. , Cunha, E. , Hockings, K. , McGrew, W. C. , Richmond, B. G. , & Matsuzawa, T
    (2010) Chimpanzee carrying behaviour and the origins of human bipedality. Current Biology, 22(6), R180–R181. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.052
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.052 [Google Scholar]
  14. Cialdini, R. B. , & Goldstein, N. J
    (2004) Social influence: Compliance and conformity. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 591–621. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142015 [Google Scholar]
  15. Cialdini, R. , & Trost, M
    (1998) Social influence: Social norms, conformity, and compliance. In D. Gilbert , S. Fiske , & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2, 4th edition) (pp. 151–192). New York: McGraw-Hill.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Claidière, N. , & Whiten, A
    (2012) Integrating the study of conformity and culture in human and nonhuman animals. Psychological Bulletin, 138, 126–145. doi: 10.1037/a0025868
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025868 [Google Scholar]
  17. Corriveau, K. H. , Fusaro, M. , & Harris, P. L
    (2009) Going with the flow: Preschoolers prefer nondissenters as informants. Psychological Science, 20, 372–377. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.2009.02291.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02291.x [Google Scholar]
  18. Corriveau, K. H. , & Harris, P. L
    (2010) Preschoolers (sometimes) defer to the majority in making simple perceptual judgments. Developmental Psychology, 46, 437–445. doi: 10.1037/a0017553
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017553 [Google Scholar]
  19. Corriveau, K. H. , Kinzler, K. D. , & Harris, P. L
    (2013) Accuracy trumps accent in children’s endorsement of object labels. Developmental Psychology, 49, 470–479. doi: 10.1037/a0030604
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030604 [Google Scholar]
  20. Clark, H. H
    (1996) Using language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511620539
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620539 [Google Scholar]
  21. Cowley, S
    (2004) Early hominins, utterance-activity, and niche construction. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 509–510. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04290112
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04290112 [Google Scholar]
  22. Crockford, C. , Wittig, R. M. , Mundry, R. , & Zuberbühler, K
    (2012) Wild chimpanzees inform ignorant group members of danger. Current Biology, 22, 1–5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.053
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.053 [Google Scholar]
  23. Crockford, C. , Wittig, R. M. , Langergraber, K. , Ziegler, T. E. , Zuberbühler, K. , & Deschner, T
    (2013) Urinary oxytocin and social bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 280, 20122765. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2012.2765
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2012.2765 [Google Scholar]
  24. Duguid, S. , Wyman, E. , Bullinger, A. F. , Herfurth-Majstorovic, K. , & Tomasello, M
    (2014), Coordination strategies of chimpanzees and human children in a Stag Hunt game. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 281, 20141973. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.1973
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.1973 [Google Scholar]
  25. Efferson, C. , Lalive, R. , Richerson, P. J. , McElreath, R. , & Lubell, M
    (2008) Conformists and mavericks: the empirics of frequency-dependent cultural transmission. Evolution of Human Behavior, 29, 56–64. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.08.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.08.003 [Google Scholar]
  26. Efferson, C. , Richerson, P. J. , McElreath, R. , Lubell, M. , Edsten, E. , Waring, T. M. , Paciotti, B. , & Baum, W
    (2007) Learning, productivity, and noise: an experimental study of cultural transmission on the Bolivian Altiplano. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 11–17. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.05.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.05.005 [Google Scholar]
  27. Enquist, M. , Eriksson, K. , & Ghirlanda, S
    (2007) Critical social learning: A solution to Roger’s paradox of nonadaptive culture. American Anthropologist, 109, 727–734. doi: 10.1525/aa.2007.109.4.727
    https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.2007.109.4.727 [Google Scholar]
  28. Eriksson, K. , & Coultas, J. C
    (2009) Are people really conformist-biased? An empirical test and a new mathematical model. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 5–21. doi: 10.1556/JEP.7.2009.1.3
    https://doi.org/10.1556/JEP.7.2009.1.3 [Google Scholar]
  29. Eriksson, K. , Enquist, M. , & Ghirlanda, S
    (2007) Critical points in current theory of conformist social learning. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 5, 67–87. doi: 10.1556/JEP.2007.1009
    https://doi.org/10.1556/JEP.2007.1009 [Google Scholar]
  30. Eriksson, K. , & Strimling, P
    (2009) Biases for acquiring information individually rather than socially. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 7, 309–329. doi: 10.1556/JEP.7.2009.4.4
    https://doi.org/10.1556/JEP.7.2009.4.4 [Google Scholar]
  31. Falk, D
    (2004) Prelinguistic evolution in early hominins: Whence motherese?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 491–541. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04000111
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04000111 [Google Scholar]
  32. (2009) Finding our tongues: Mothers, infants, and the origins of language. New York: Basic Books.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Francis, W. N
    (1983) Dialectology. London: Longmans.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Fusaroli, R. , Bahrami, B. , Olsen, K. , Roepstorff, A. , Rees, G. , Frith, C. , & Tylén, K
    (2012) Coming to terms: quantifying the benefits of linguistic coordination. Psychological Science, 23931–939. doi: 10.1177/0956797612436816
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612436816 [Google Scholar]
  35. Haun, D.B. M. , Rekers, Y. , & Tomasello, M
    (2014) Children conform to the behavior of peers; Other great apes stick with what they know. Psychological Science, 25, 2160–2167. doi: 10.1177/0956797614553235
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614553235 [Google Scholar]
  36. Haun, D.B. M. , & Tomasello, M
    (2011) Conformity to peer pressure in preschool children. Child Development, 82, 1759–1767. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2011.01666.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01666.x [Google Scholar]
  37. Healey, P.G. T. , Purver, M. , & Howes, C
    (2014) Divergence in dialogue. PLOS One, 9, 6, e98598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0098598
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0098598 [Google Scholar]
  38. Heft, H
    (1983) Wayfinding as the perception of information over time. Population & Environment, 6, 133–150. doi: 10.1007/BF01258956
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01258956 [Google Scholar]
  39. Heyes, C. M
    (2011) Automatic imitation. Psychological Bulletin, 137, 463–483. doi: 10.1037/a0022288
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022288 [Google Scholar]
  40. Hodges, B. H
    (2007a) Good prospects: Ecological and social perspectives on conforming, creating, and caring in conversation. Language Sciences, 29, 584–604. doi: 10.1016/j.langsci.2007.01.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2007.01.003 [Google Scholar]
  41. (2007b) Values define fields: The intentional dynamics of driving, carrying, leading, negotiating, and conversing. Ecological Psychology, 19, 153–178.
    [Google Scholar]
  42. (2009) Ecological pragmatics: Values, dialogical arrays, complexity, and caring. Pragmatics & Cognition, 17, 628–652. doi: 10.1075/pc.17.3.08hod
    https://doi.org/10.1075/pc.17.3.08hod [Google Scholar]
  43. (2014) Rethinking conformity and imitation: Divergence, convergence, and social understanding. Frontiers in Psychology: Cognitive Science, 5, 726.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. (2017) Conformity and divergence in interactions, groups, and culture. In S. Harkins , K. Williams , & J. Burger (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social influence (pp.87–105). New York: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199859870.013.3
    https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199859870.013.3 [Google Scholar]
  45. Hodges, B. H. , & Baron, R. M
    (1992) Values as constraints on affordances: Perceiving and acting properly. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 22, 263–294. doi: 10.1111/j.1468‑5914.1992.tb00220.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-5914.1992.tb00220.x [Google Scholar]
  46. Hodges, B. H. , & Geyer, A
    (2006) A nonconformist account of the Asch experiments: Values, pragmatics, and moral dilemmas. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 2–19. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_1
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327957pspr1001_1 [Google Scholar]
  47. Hodges, B. H. , & Lindhiem, O
    (2006) Carrying babies and groceries: The effect of moral and social weight on caring. Ecological Psychology, 18, 93–111. doi: 10.1207/s15326969eco1802_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326969eco1802_2 [Google Scholar]
  48. Hodges, B. H. , Meagher, B. R. , Norton, D. J. , McBain, R. , & Sroubek, A
    (2014) Speaking from ignorance: Not agreeing with others we believe are correct. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 218–234. doi: 10.1037/a0034662
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0034662 [Google Scholar]
  49. Hofman, D. , Bos, P. A. , Schutter, J.L. G. , & van Honk, J
    (2012) Fairness modulates non-conscious facial mimicry in women. Proceedings of the Royal Society B , 279, 3535–3539.
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Hrdy, S. B
    (1999) Mother nature: A history of mothers, infants and natural selection. New York: Pantheon.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. (2009) Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Hunt, K. D
    (1994) The evolution of human bipedality: Ecology and functional morphology. Journal of Human Evolution, 26, 183–202. doi: 10.1006/jhev.1994.1011
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1994.1011 [Google Scholar]
  53. Isbell, L. A. , & Young, T. P
    (1996) The evolution of bipedalism in hominids and reduced group size in chimpanzees: Alternative responses to decreasing resource availability. Journal of Human Evolution, 30, 289–297. doi: 10.1006/jhev.1996.0034
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1996.0034 [Google Scholar]
  54. Jetten, J. , & Hornsey, M. J
    (2014) Deviance and dissent in groups. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 461–485. doi: 10.1146/annurev‑psych‑010213‑115151
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115151 [Google Scholar]
  55. Johansson, S. , Zlatev, J. , & Gardenfors, P
    (2006) Why don’t chimps talk and humans sing like canaries?Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 287–288. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X06319061
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X06319061 [Google Scholar]
  56. Karremans, J. C. , & Verwijmeren, T
    (2008) Mimicking attractive opposite-sex others: The role of romantic relationship status. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 939–950. doi: 10.1177/0146167208316693
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167208316693 [Google Scholar]
  57. Laks, B
    (2013) Why is there variation rather than nothing?Language Sciences, 39, 31–53. doi: 10.1016/j.langsci.2013.02.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2013.02.009 [Google Scholar]
  58. Lewis, D
    (1969) Convention. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Locke, J. L. , & Bogin, B
    (2006) Language and life history: A new perspective on the development and evolution of human language. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 259–325.
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Lovejoy, C. O
    (1981) The origin of man. Science, 211, 341–350. doi: 10.1126/science.211.4480.341
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.211.4480.341 [Google Scholar]
  61. (2009) Reexamining human origins in light of Ardipithecus ramidus . Science, 326, 74e1-74e8. doi: 10.1126/science.1175834
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1175834 [Google Scholar]
  62. Lovejoy, C. O. , Suwa, G. , Spurlock, L. , Asfaw, B. , & White, T. D
    (2009) The pelvis and femur of Ardipithecus ramidus: The emergence of upright walking. Science, 326, 71e1-71e6. doi: 10.1126/science.1175831
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1175831 [Google Scholar]
  63. Lumsden, J. , Miles, L. K. , Richardson, M. J. , Smith, C. A. , & Macrae, C. N
    (2012) Who syncs? Social motives and interpersonal coordination. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48, 746–751. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2011.12.007 [Google Scholar]
  64. Marsh, K. L. , Richardson, M. J. , & Schmidt, R. C
    (2009) Social connection through joint action and interpersonal coordination. Topics in Cognitive Science, 1, 320–339. doi: 10.1111/j.1756‑8765.2009.01022.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1756-8765.2009.01022.x [Google Scholar]
  65. McBride, G
    (2014) Storytelling, behavior planning, and language evolution in context. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1131. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01131
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01131 [Google Scholar]
  66. McElreath, R. , Lubell, M. , Richerson, P. J. , Waring, T. M. , Baum, W. , Edsten, E. , Efferson, C. , & Paciotti, B
    (2005) Applying evolutionary models to the laboratory study of social learning. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26, 483–508. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.04.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.04.003 [Google Scholar]
  67. Mercier, H. , & Sperber, D
    (2011) Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 57–111. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X10000968
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000968 [Google Scholar]
  68. Mesoudi, A
    (2011) An experimental comparison of human social learning strategies: Payoff-biased social learning is adaptive but underused. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32, 334–342. doi: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.12.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.12.001 [Google Scholar]
  69. Millikan, R. G
    (2005) Language: A biological model. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/0199284768.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/0199284768.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  70. Mills, G. J
    (2014) Dialogue in joint activity: Complementarity, convergence and conventionalization. New Ideas in Psychology, 32, 158–173. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2013.03.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2013.03.006 [Google Scholar]
  71. Moll, H. , & Tomasello, M
    (2007) Co-operation and human cognition: The Vygotskian intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, 362, 639–648. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2006.2000
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.2000 [Google Scholar]
  72. (2012) Three-year-olds understand appearance and reality - Just not about the same object at the same time. Developmental Psychology, 48, 1124–1132. doi: 10.1037/a0025915
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025915 [Google Scholar]
  73. Moore, R
    (2013) Imitation and conventional communication. Biology & Philosophy, 28, 481–500. doi: 10.1007/s10539‑012‑9349‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-012-9349-8 [Google Scholar]
  74. Nielsen, M
    (2006) Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 555–565. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.42.3.555
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.3.555 [Google Scholar]
  75. Nielsen, M. , Mushin, I. , Tomaselli, K. , & Whiten, A
    (2014) Where culture takes hold: “Overimitation” and its flexible deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen children. Child Development, 85, 2169–2184.
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Nielsen, M. , & Tomaselli, K
    (2010) Overimitation in Kalahari bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science, 21, 729–736. doi: 10.1177/0956797610368808
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610368808 [Google Scholar]
  77. Niemitz, C
    (2010) The evolution of upright posture and gait—a review and a new synthesis. Naturwissenschaften, 97, 241–263. doi: 10.1007/s00114‑009‑0637‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s00114-009-0637-3 [Google Scholar]
  78. Over, H. , & Carpenter, M
    (2012) Putting the social into social learning: Explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children’s copying behavior. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 126, 182–192. doi: 10.1037/a0024555
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024555 [Google Scholar]
  79. Pasquini, E. S. , Corriveau, K. H. , Koenig, M. , & Harris, P. L
    (2007) Preschoolers monitor the relative accuracy of informants. Developmental Psychology, 43, 1216–1226. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.43.5.1216
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1216 [Google Scholar]
  80. Pickering, M. J. , & Garrod, S
    (2004) Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169–190. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.43.5.1216
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1216 [Google Scholar]
  81. Pontzer, H. , Raichlen, D. A. , & Sockol, M. D
    (2009) The metabolic cost of walking in humans, chimpanzees, and early hominins. Journal of Human Evolution, 56, 43–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.09.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.09.001 [Google Scholar]
  82. Provine, R. H
    (2004) Walkie-talkie evolution: Bipedalism and vocal production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 520–521. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.09.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2008.09.001 [Google Scholar]
  83. Raubal, M
    (2008) Wayfinding: Affordances and agent simulation. Encyclopedia of GIS (pp. 1243–1246). New York: Springer. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X04410115
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04410115 [Google Scholar]
  84. Reddy, V
    (2008) How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Redhead, G. , & Dunbar, R.I. M
    (2013) The functions of language: An experimental study. Evolutionary Psychology, 11, 845–854. doi: 10.1177/147470491301100409
    https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491301100409 [Google Scholar]
  86. Reed, E. S
    (1996) Encountering the world: Toward an ecological psychology. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1177/147470491301100409
    https://doi.org/10.1177/147470491301100409 [Google Scholar]
  87. Rekers, Y. , Haun, D.B. M. , & Tomasello, M
    (2011) Children, but not chimpanzees, prefer to collaborate. Current Biology, 21, 1756–1758. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.066
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.066 [Google Scholar]
  88. Richerson, P. J. , & Boyd, R
    (2005) Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.066
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.066 [Google Scholar]
  89. Riley, M. A. , & Turvey, M. T
    (2002) Variability and determinism in motor behavior. Journal of Motor Behavior, 34, 99–125. doi: 10.1080/00222890209601934
    https://doi.org/10.1080/00222890209601934 [Google Scholar]
  90. Roche, J. M. , Dale, R. , & Caucci, G. M
    (2012) Double up on double meanings: Pragmatic alignment. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 1–24. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2010.509929
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.509929 [Google Scholar]
  91. Rost, G. C. , & McMurray, B
    (2010) Finding the signal by adding noise: The role of noncontrastive phonetic variability in early world learning. Infancy, 15, 608–635. doi: 10.1111/j.1532‑7078.2010.00033.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7078.2010.00033.x [Google Scholar]
  92. Schel, A. M. , Townsend, S. W. , Machanda, Z. , Zuberbühler, K. , & Slocombe, K. E
    (2013) Chimpanzee alarm call production meets key criteria for intentionality. PLOS One, 8, e76674. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076674
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0076674 [Google Scholar]
  93. Shockley, K. , Santana, M. V. , & Fowler, C. A
    (2003) Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 326–332. doi: 10.1037/0096‑1523.29.2.326
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.29.2.326 [Google Scholar]
  94. Sockol, M. D. , Raichlen, D. A. , & Pontzer, H
    (2007) Chimpanzee locomotor energetics and the origin of human bipedalism. PNAS, 104, 12265–12269. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703267104
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0703267104 [Google Scholar]
  95. Sterelny, K
    (2003) Thought in a hostile world. Malden, MA: Basil Blackwell. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0703267104
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0703267104 [Google Scholar]
  96. Strigul, N
    (2009) Can imitation explain dialect origins?Ecological Modeling, 220, 2624–2639. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2009.07.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2009.07.005 [Google Scholar]
  97. Suddendorf, T
    (2013) The gap: The science of what separates us from other animals. New York: Basic Books. doi: 10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2009.07.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecolmodel.2009.07.005 [Google Scholar]
  98. Sutou, S
    (2012) Hairless mutation: A driving force of humanization from a human-ape common ancestor by enforcing upright walking while holding a baby with both hands. Genes to Cells, 17, 264–272. doi: 10.1111/j.1365‑2443.2012.01592.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2443.2012.01592.x [Google Scholar]
  99. Tilsen, S
    (2015) Structured nonstationarity in articulatory timing. InThe Scottish Consortium for ICPhS 2015 (Ed.), Proceedings of the 18th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences. Glasgow: University of Glasgow. doi: 10.1111/j.1365‑2443.2012.01592.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2443.2012.01592.x [Google Scholar]
  100. Tomasello, M
    (1999) The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  101. (2008) Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  102. (2009) Why we cooperate. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  103. (2014a) A natural history of human thinking. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. doi: 10.4159/9780674726369
    https://doi.org/10.4159/9780674726369 [Google Scholar]
  104. (2014b) The ultra-social animal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 187–194. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2015
    https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2015 [Google Scholar]
  105. Tomasello, M. , & Haberl, K
    (2003) Understanding attention: 12- and 18-month olds know what is new for other persons. Developmental Psychology, 39, 906–912. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.39.5.906
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.5.906 [Google Scholar]
  106. Tomasello, M. , Melis, A. P. , Tennie, C. , Wyman, E. , & Herrmann, E
    (2012) Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: The interdependence hypothesis. Current Anthropology, 53, 673–692. doi: 10.1086/668207
    https://doi.org/10.1086/668207 [Google Scholar]
  107. Tomasello, M. , & Vaish, A
    (2013) Origins of human cooperation and morality. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 231–255. doi: 10.1146/annurev‑psych‑113011‑143812
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143812 [Google Scholar]
  108. Waters, D. P
    (2012) From extended phenotype to extended affordance: Distributed language at the intersection of Gibson and Dawkins. Language Sciences, 34, 507–512. doi: 10.1016/j.langsci.2012.03.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2012.03.007 [Google Scholar]
  109. Watson, J. , Payne, R. , Chamberlain, A. , Jones, R. K. , & Sellers, W. I
    (2008) The energetic costs of load-carrying and the evolution of bipedalism. Journal of Human Evolution, 54, 675–683. doi: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.10.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2007.10.004 [Google Scholar]
  110. (2009) The kinematics of load carrying in humans and great apes: Implications for the evolution of human bipedalism. Folia Primatology, 80, 309–328. doi: 10.1159/000258646
    https://doi.org/10.1159/000258646 [Google Scholar]
  111. Whalen, D
    (2014) Deliberate variability in speech for flexibly engaging other speakers. Poster at conference, Finding Common Ground: Social, Ecological, and Cognitive Perspectives on Language Use , University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT. doi: 10.1159/000258646
    https://doi.org/10.1159/000258646 [Google Scholar]
  112. Whiten, A. , McGuigan, N. , Marshall-Pescini, S. , & Hopper, L. M
    (2009) Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 2417–2428. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2009.0069
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0069 [Google Scholar]
  113. Wittig, R. M. , Crockford, C. , Deschner, T. , Langergraber, K. , Ziegler, T. E. , & Zuberbühler, K
    (2014) Food sharing is linked to urinary oxytocin levels and bonding in related and unrelated wild chimpanzees. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 281, 20133096. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2013.3096
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.3096 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/is.18.1.02hod
Loading
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error