1887
Volume 20, Issue 2
  • ISSN 1572-0373
  • E-ISSN: 1572-0381
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

What determines if the first interaction between strangers will be a pleasant experience? We conducted an experiment to investigate the extent to which the perceived quality of an interaction is influenced by conversation content and context, and we document the physiological changes that are likely to play a role in establishing rapport. Females who did not know each other met in pairs and conducted a gossip- or creativity task, either face-to-face or online. The conversation content had no effect on the quality of online interactions. However in the face-to-face condition gossip was associated with better interaction quality. Tonic electrodermal activity steadily declined throughout the interaction, while phasic electrodermal activity first peaked and then returned to baseline. Neither were related to perceived interaction quality. Heart rate variability (HRV) dropped at first but then remained stable. A smaller drop in HRV drop corresponded to higher ratings of rapport and liking. Together these results suggest that gossip can improve the quality of a face-to-face interaction between strangers, and support the conjecture that parasympathetic activity is a marker of human openness to social engagement.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/is.18015.rud
2019-10-07
2024-10-15
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Acharya, U. R., Joseph, K. P., Kannathal, N., Lim, C. M., & Suri, J. S.
    (2006) Heart rate variability: a review. Medical and biological engineering and computing, 44(12), 1031–1051. 10.1007/s11517‑006‑0119‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11517-006-0119-0 [Google Scholar]
  2. Agnew, C. R., Loving, T. J., Le, B., & Goodfriend, W.
    (2004) Thinking close: Measuring relational closeness as perceived self-other inclusion. InD. J. Mashek, A. Aron, Handbook of closeness and intimacy, 103–115. Mahwah New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Amico, J. A., Ervin, M. G., Leake, R. D., Fisher, D. A., Finn, F. M., & Robinson, A. G.
    (1985) A novel oxytocin-like and vasotocin-like peptide in human plasma after administration of estrogen. The Journal of Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 60(1), 5–12. 10.1210/jcem‑60‑1‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1210/jcem-60-1-5 [Google Scholar]
  4. Andreassi, J. L.
    (2013) Psychophysiology: Human behavior & physiological response. Psychology Press. 10.4324/9781410602817
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410602817 [Google Scholar]
  5. Aron, A., Aron, E. N., & Smollan, D.
    (1992) Inclusion of Other in the Self Scale and the structure of interpersonal closeness. Journal of personality and social psychology, 63(4), 596. 10.1037/0022‑3514.63.4.596
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.4.596 [Google Scholar]
  6. Baltes, B. B., Dickson, M. W., Sherman, M. P., Bauer, C. C., & LaGanke, J. S.
    (2002) Computer-mediated communication and group decision making: A meta-analysis. Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 87(1), 156–179. 10.1006/obhd.2001.2961
    https://doi.org/10.1006/obhd.2001.2961 [Google Scholar]
  7. Bee, P. E., Bower, P., Lovell, K., Gilbody, S., Richards, D., Gask, L., & Roach, P.
    (2008) Psychotherapy mediated by remote communication technologies: a meta-analytic review. BMC psychiatry, 8(1), 60. 10.1186/1471‑244X‑8‑60
    https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244X-8-60 [Google Scholar]
  8. Benedek, M., & Kaernbach, C.
    (2010a) A continuous measure of phasic electrodermal activity. Journal of neuroscience methods, 190(1), 80–91. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.04.028
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.04.028 [Google Scholar]
  9. (2010b) Decomposition of skin conductance data by means of nonnegative deconvolution. Psychophysiology, 47(4), 647–658.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Bernieri, F. J., Davis, J. M., Rosenthal, R., & Knee, C. R.
    (1994) Interactional synchrony and rapport: Measuring synchrony in displays devoid of sound and facial affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(3), 303–311. 10.1177/0146167294203008
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294203008 [Google Scholar]
  11. Bernieri, F. J.
    (2005) The expression of rapport. InV. Manusov (Ed.) The sourcebook of nonverbal measures. Mahwah New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Bicchieri, C., and Lev-On, A.
    (2007) Computer-mediated communication and cooperation in social dilemmas: an experimental analysis. Politics, Philosophy and Economics, 6(2), 139–168. 10.1177/1470594X07077267
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1470594X07077267 [Google Scholar]
  13. Boero, R., Bravo, G., Castellani, M., and Squazzoni, F.
    (2009) Reputational cues in repeated trust games. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 38(6), 871–877. 10.1016/j.socec.2009.05.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2009.05.004 [Google Scholar]
  14. Bos, N., Olson, J., Gergle, D., Olson, G., and Wright, Z.
    (2002) Effects of four computer-mediated communications channels on trust development. Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on human factors in computing systems (pp.135–140). ACM.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Boucsein, W.
    (2012) Electrodermal activity. Springer Science & Business Media. 10.1007/978‑1‑4614‑1126‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-1126-0 [Google Scholar]
  16. Brondino, N., Fusar-Poli, L., & Politi, P.
    (2017) Something to talk about: Gossip increases oxytocin levels in a near real-life situation. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 77, 218–224. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.12.014
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.12.014 [Google Scholar]
  17. Buton, F., Fontayne, P., Heuzé, J. P., Bosselut, G., & Raimbault, N.
    (2007) The QAG-A: an analog version of the questionnaire sur l’ambiance du groupe for measuring the dynamic nature of group cohesion. Small Group Research, 38(2), 235–264. 10.1177/1046496407300476
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1046496407300476 [Google Scholar]
  18. Camm, A. J., Malik, M., Bigger, J. T., Breithardt, G., Cerutti, S., Cohen, R. J., … & Lombardi, F.
    (1996) Heart rate variability: standards of measurement, physiological interpretation and clinical use. Task Force of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Circulation, 93(5), 1043–1065. 10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043
    https://doi.org/10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043 [Google Scholar]
  19. Carter, C. S.
    (2007) Sex differences in oxytocin and vasopressin: implications for autism spectrum disorders?Behavioural brain research, 176(1), 170–186. 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.08.025
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2006.08.025 [Google Scholar]
  20. Chaminade, T.
    (2017) An experimental approach to study the physiology of natural social interactions. Interaction Studies, 18(2), 254–275. 10.1075/is.18.2.06gry
    https://doi.org/10.1075/is.18.2.06gry [Google Scholar]
  21. Churchland, P. S., & Winkielman, P.
    (2012) Modulating social behavior with oxytocin: how does it work? What does it mean?. Horm. Behav. 61(3), 392–399. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2011.12.003 [Google Scholar]
  22. Conty, L., Russo, M., Loehr, V., Hugueville, L., Barbu, S., Huguet, P., … & George, N.
    (2010) The mere perception of eye contact increases arousal during a word-spelling task. Social neuroscience, 5(2), 171–186. 10.1080/17470910903227507
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910903227507 [Google Scholar]
  23. Cordonier, L., Breton, A., Trouche, E., & Van der Henst, J. B.
    (2017) Does prestige affect us physiologically?. A study in interactional context. Interaction Studies, 18(2), 214–233. 10.1075/is.18.2.04cor
    https://doi.org/10.1075/is.18.2.04cor [Google Scholar]
  24. Critchley, H. D.
    (2002) Electrodermal responses: what happens in the brain. The Neuroscientist, 8(2), 132–142. 10.1177/107385840200800209
    https://doi.org/10.1177/107385840200800209 [Google Scholar]
  25. Critchley, H. D., & Harrison, N. A.
    2013 Visceral influences on brain and behavior. Neuron77(4), 624–638. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.02.008 [Google Scholar]
  26. Cummings, J. N., Butler, B., & Kraut, R.
    (2002) The quality of online social relationships. Communications of the ACM, 45(7), 103–108. 10.1145/514236.514242
    https://doi.org/10.1145/514236.514242 [Google Scholar]
  27. Darlington, R. B., & Hayes, A. F.
    (2017) Regression analysis and linear models: Concepts, application, and implementation.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Filion, D. L.
    (2007) The electrodermal system. InI. T. Cacioppo & L. G. Tassinary (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology (pp.159–181). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511546396.007
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511546396.007 [Google Scholar]
  29. De Backer, C. J., Nelissen, M., Vyncke, P., Braeckman, J., and McAndrew, F. T.
    (2007) Celebrities: From teachers to friends. Human Nature, 18(4), 334–354. 10.1007/s12110‑007‑9023‑z
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-007-9023-z [Google Scholar]
  30. De Backer, C., and Fisher, M. L.
    (2012) Tabloids as windows into our interpersonal relationships: A content analysis of mass media gossip from an evolutionary perspective. Journal of Social, Evolutionary, and Cultural Psychology, 6(3), 404–424. 10.1037/h0099244
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0099244 [Google Scholar]
  31. Diamond, L. M., & Cribbet, M. R.
    (2012) Links between adolescent sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system functioning and interpersonal behavior over time. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 88(3), 339–348. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.008 [Google Scholar]
  32. Diamond, L. M., Hicks, A. M., & Otter-Henderson, K. D.
    (2011) Individual differences in vagal regulation moderate associations between daily affect and daily couple interactions. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 37, 731–744. 10.1177/0146167211400620
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211400620 [Google Scholar]
  33. Dieleman, G. C., Huizink, A. C., Tulen, J. H., Utens, E. M., Creemers, H. E., van der Ende, J., & Verhulst, F. C.
    (2015) Alterations in HPA-axis and autonomic nervous system functioning in childhood anxiety disorders point to a chronic stress hypothesis. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 51, 135–150. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.09.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.09.002 [Google Scholar]
  34. Dieleman, G. C., van der Ende, J., Verhulst, F. C., & Huizink, A. C.
    (2010) Perceived and physiological arousal during a stress task: can they differentiate between anxiety and depression?. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 35(8), 1223–1234. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.02.012
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.02.012 [Google Scholar]
  35. Duarte, J., Siegel, S., and Young, L.
    (2012) Trust and credit: the role of appearance in peer-to-peer lending. The Review of Financial Studies, 25(8), 2455–2484. 10.1093/rfs/hhs071
    https://doi.org/10.1093/rfs/hhs071 [Google Scholar]
  36. Dunbar, R. I.
    (2004) Gossip in evolutionary perspective. Review of general psychology, 8(2), 100. 10.1037/1089‑2680.8.2.100
    https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.8.2.100 [Google Scholar]
  37. Emery, N. J.
    (2000) The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 24(6), 581–604. 10.1016/S0149‑7634(00)00025‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00025-7 [Google Scholar]
  38. Evans, B. E., Greaves-Lord, K., Euser, A. S., Tulen, J. H., Franken, I. H., & Huizink, A. C.
    (2013) Determinants of physiological and perceived physiological stress reactivity in children and adolescents. PloS one, 8(4), e61724. 10.1371/journal.pone.0061724
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0061724 [Google Scholar]
  39. Fehr, E., & Schneider, F.
    (2009) Eyes are on us, but nobody cares: are eye cues relevant for strong reciprocity?Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1686), 1315–1323. 10.1098/rspb.2009.1900
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1900 [Google Scholar]
  40. Feinberg, M., Willer, R., Stellar, J., and Keltner, D.
    (2012) The virtues of gossip: Reputational information sharing as prosocial behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(5), 1015–1030. 10.1037/a0026650
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026650 [Google Scholar]
  41. Feldman, R., Gordon, I., & Zagoory-Sharon, O.
    (2010) The cross-generation transmission of oxytocin in humans. Horm. Behav. 58(4), 669–676. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.06.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2010.06.005 [Google Scholar]
  42. Foster, E. K.
    (2004) Research on gossip: Taxonomy, methods, and future directions. Review of General Psychology, 8(2), 78. 10.1037/1089‑2680.8.2.78
    https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.8.2.78 [Google Scholar]
  43. Geisler, F. C., Vennewald, N., Kubiak, T., & Weber, H.
    (2010) The impact of heart rate variability on subjective well-being is mediated by emotion regulation. Pers. Indiv. Differ. 49(7), 723–728. 10.1016/j.paid.2010.06.015
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.06.015 [Google Scholar]
  44. Gillis, J. S., & Bernieri, F.
    (2001) The perception and judgment of rapport. InK. R. Hammond, T. R. Steward (Eds.) The Essential Brunswik (pp.380–384). Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Gorin, V., and Dubied, A.
    (2011) Desirable people: Identifying social values through celebrity news. Media, culture and society, 33(4), 599–618. 10.1177/0163443711398695
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0163443711398695 [Google Scholar]
  46. Grewen, K. M., & Light, K. C.
    (2011) Plasma oxytocin is related to lower cardiovascular and sympathetic reactivity to stress. Biol. Psychol. 87(3), 340–349. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.04.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.04.003 [Google Scholar]
  47. Grewen, K. M., Girdler, S. S., Amico, J., & Light, K. C.
    (2005) Effects of partner support on resting oxytocin, cortisol, norepinephrine, and blood pressure before and after warm partner contact. Psychosom. Med. 67(4), 531–538. 10.1097/01.psy.0000170341.88395.47
    https://doi.org/10.1097/01.psy.0000170341.88395.47 [Google Scholar]
  48. Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W.
    (1993) Emotional suppression: physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of personality and social psychology, 64(6), 970. 10.1037/0022‑3514.64.6.970
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.64.6.970 [Google Scholar]
  49. Hastings, P. D., Nuselovici, J. N., Utendale, W. T., Coutya, J., McShane, K. E., & Sullivan, C.
    (2008) Applying the polyvagal theory to children’s emotion regulation: Social context, socialization, and adjustment. Biological psychology, 79(3), 299–306. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.07.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.07.005 [Google Scholar]
  50. Horvat-Gordon, M., Granger, D. A., Schwartz, E. B., Nelson, V. J., & Kivlighan, K. T.
    (2005) Oxytocin is not a valid biomarker when measured in saliva by immunoassay. Physiology & behavior, 84(3), 445–448. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.01.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.01.007 [Google Scholar]
  51. Huber, P. J.
    (1967) The behavior of maximum likelihood estimates under nonstandard conditions. Proceedings of the fifth Berkeley symposium on mathematical statistics and probability1(1), 221–233.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Insel, T. R., & Fernald, R. D.
    (2004) How the brain processes social information: searching for the social brain. Annu. Rev. Neurosci, 27, 697–722. 10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144148
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144148 [Google Scholar]
  53. Itier, R. J., & Batty, M.
    (2009) Neural bases of eye and gaze processing: the core of social cognition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(6), 843–863. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.02.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.02.004 [Google Scholar]
  54. Kagerbauer, S. M., Martin, J., Schuster, T., Blobner, M., Kochs, E. F., & Landgraf, R.
    (2013) Plasma oxytocin and vasopressin do not predict neuropeptide concentrations in human cerebrospinal fluid. J. Neuroendocrinol. 25(7), 668–673. 10.1111/jne.12038
    https://doi.org/10.1111/jne.12038 [Google Scholar]
  55. Karkow, F. J., Spiandorello, W. P., Godoy, R. F., Pezzi, P., Karkow, A. G., & Faintuch, J.
    (2004) Subjective versus objective stress in noncritically ill hospitalized and outpatient adult men. Revista do Hospital das Clínicas, 59(4), 161–167. 10.1590/S0041‑87812004000400002
    https://doi.org/10.1590/S0041-87812004000400002 [Google Scholar]
  56. Kemp, A. H., Quintana, D. S., Kuhnert, R. L., Griffiths, K., Hickie, I. B., & Guastella, A. J.
    (2012) Oxytocin increases heart rate variability in humans at rest: implications for social approach-related motivation and capacity for social engagement. PLoS One, 7(8), e44014. 10.1371/journal.pone.0044014
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0044014 [Google Scholar]
  57. Kéri, S., & Kiss, I.
    (2011) Oxytocin response in a trust game and habituation of arousal. Physiol. Behav. 102(2), 221–224. 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.11.011
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.11.011 [Google Scholar]
  58. Kleinke, C. L.
    (1986) Gaze and eye contact: a research review. Psychological bulletin, 100(1), 78. 10.1037/0033‑2909.100.1.78
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.100.1.78 [Google Scholar]
  59. Kok, B. E., & Fredrickson, B. L.
    (2010) Upward spirals of the heart: Autonomic flexibility, as indexed by vagal tone, reciprocally and prospectively predicts positive emotions and social connectedness. Biological psychology, 85(3), 432–436. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.005 [Google Scholar]
  60. Lambert, B., Declerck, C. H., Boone, C., & Parizel, P. M.
    (2017) A functional MRI study on how oxytocin affects decision making in social dilemmas: Cooperate as long as it pays off, aggress only when you think you can win. Horm. Behav., 94, 145–152. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.06.011
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.06.011 [Google Scholar]
  61. Lange, C. G., & James, W.
    (1922) The emotions (Vol. 1). Williams & Wilkins. 10.1037/10735‑000
    https://doi.org/10.1037/10735-000 [Google Scholar]
  62. Levin, J., & Arluke, A.
    (1985) An exploratory analysis of sex differences in gossip. Sex roles, 12(3–4), 281–286. 10.1007/BF00287594
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00287594 [Google Scholar]
  63. Lewis, M. D., Lamm, C., Segalowitz, S. J., Stieben, J., & Zelazo, P. D.
    (2006) Neurophysiological correlates of emotion regulation in children and adolescents. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 18(3), 430–443. 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.3.430
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.3.430 [Google Scholar]
  64. Lopatina, O., Inzhutova, A., Salmina, A. B., & Higashida, H.
    2013 The roles of oxytocin and CD38 in social or parental behaviors. Front. Neurosci, 6, 182. 10.3389/fnins.2012.00182
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2012.00182 [Google Scholar]
  65. MacDonald, K., & Feifel, D.
    (2014) Oxytocin׳ s role in anxiety: A critical appraisal. Brain Res 1580, 22–56. 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.01.025
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.01.025 [Google Scholar]
  66. Marshall, J. R.
    (1995) "Telephone Phobia.” InJ. R. Marshall, S. Lipsett. Social Phobia: From Shyness to Stage Fright. New York: BasicBooks.
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Mitchell, D., Bryson, J. J., Rauwolf, P., & Ingram, G. P.
    (2016) On the reliability of unreliable information. Gossip as Cultural Memory. Interaction Studies, 17(1), 1–18. 10.1075/is.17.1.01mit
    https://doi.org/10.1075/is.17.1.01mit [Google Scholar]
  68. Nikolić, M., Vente, W., Colonnesi, C., & Bögels, S. M.
    (2016) Autonomic arousal in children of parents with and without social anxiety disorder: a high-risk study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 57(9), 1047–1055. 10.1111/jcpp.12563
    https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12563 [Google Scholar]
  69. Nummenmaa, L., Glerean, E., Hari, R., & Hietanen, J. K.
    (2014) Bodily maps of emotions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(2), 646–651. 10.1073/pnas.1321664111
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1321664111 [Google Scholar]
  70. Oldehinkel, A. J., Ormel, J., Bosch, N. M., Bouma, E., Van Roon, A. M., Rosmalen, J. G., & Riese, H.
    (2011) Stressed out? Associations between perceived and physiological stress responses in adolescents: The TRAILS study. Psychophysiology, 48(4), 441–452. 10.1111/j.1469‑8986.2010.01118.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01118.x [Google Scholar]
  71. Petrovický, P., Němcová, V., Hans, J., Overeem, S., Vos, P.
    (2011) The reticular formation and some related nuclei. InJ. Hans (Ed.) Clinical neuroanatomy (pp.211–247). Springer Berlin Heidelberg. 10.1007/978‑3‑642‑19134‑3_5
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-19134-3_5 [Google Scholar]
  72. Piazza, J., and Bering, J. M.
    (2008) Concerns about reputation via gossip promote generous allocations in an economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(3), 172–178. 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  73. Porges, S. W.
    (2001) The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic substrates of a social nervous system. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 42(2), 123–146. 10.1016/S0167‑8760(01)00162‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(01)00162-3 [Google Scholar]
  74. (2003) The polyvagal theory: Phylogenetic contributions to social behavior. Physiol. Behav. 79(3), 503–513. 10.1016/S0031‑9384(03)00156‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00156-2 [Google Scholar]
  75. 2007 The polyvagal perspective. Biol. Psychol. 74(2), 116–143. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2006.06.009 [Google Scholar]
  76. Quintana, D. S., Guastella, A. J., Outhred, T., Hickie, I. B., & Kemp, A. H.
    (2012) Heart rate variability is associated with emotion recognition: Direct evidence for a relationship between the autonomic nervous system and social cognition. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 86(2), 168–172. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.012
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.012 [Google Scholar]
  77. Quintana, D. S., Kemp, A. H., Alvares, G. A., & Guastella, A. J.
    (2013) A role for autonomic cardiac control in the effects of oxytocin on social behavior and psychiatric illness. Front. Neurosci. 7, 1–9. 10.3389/fnins.2013.00048
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2013.00048 [Google Scholar]
  78. Ragot, M., Martin, N., Em, S., Pallamin, N., & Diverrez, J. M.
    (2017) Emotion recognition using physiological signals: laboratory vs. wearable sensors. International Conference on Applied Human Factors and Ergonomics (pp.15–22). Springer.
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Ross, H. E., & Young, L. J.
    (2009) Oxytocin and the neural mechanisms regulating social cognition and affiliative behavior. Frontiers in neuroendocrinology, 30(4), 534–547. 10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.05.004 [Google Scholar]
  80. Rubin, L. H., Carter, C. S., Bishop, J. R., Pournajafi-Nazarloo, H., Drogos, L. L., Hill, S. K., … & Pearlson, G. D.
    (2014) Reduced levels of vasopressin and reduced behavioral modulation of oxytocin in psychotic disorders. Schizophrenia bulletin, 40(6), 1374–1384. 10.1093/schbul/sbu027
    https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbu027 [Google Scholar]
  81. Rudnicki, K., De Backer, C. J., & Declerck, C.
    (2019) The effects of celebrity gossip on trust are moderated by prosociality of the gossipers. Personality and Individual Differences, 143, 42–46. 10.1016/j.paid.2019.02.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.02.010 [Google Scholar]
  82. Schultheiss, O. C., & Stanton, S. J.
    (2009) Assessment of salivary hormones. Methods in social neuroscience, 17–44.
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Shahrestani, S., Stewart, E. M., Quintana, D. S., Hickie, I. B., & Guastella, A. J.
    (2015) Heart rate variability during adolescent and adult social interactions: A meta-analysis. Biological psychology, 105, 43–50. 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.12.012
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.12.012 [Google Scholar]
  84. Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., & Abu-Akel, A.
    (2016) The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biol. Psychiat. 79(3), 194–202. 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.020
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.020 [Google Scholar]
  85. Sommerfeld, R. D., Krambeck, H. J., and Milinski, M.
    (2008) Multiple gossip statements and their effect on reputation and trustworthiness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 275(1650), 2529–2536. 10.1098/rspb.2008.0762
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.0762 [Google Scholar]
  86. Sommerfeld, R. D., Krambeck, H. J., Semmann, D., and Milinski, M.
    (2007) Gossip as an alternative for direct observation in games of indirect reciprocity. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences, 104(44), 17435–17440. 10.1073/pnas.0704598104
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0704598104 [Google Scholar]
  87. Strathearn, L., Fonagy, P., Amico, J., & Montague, P. R.
    (2009) Adult attachment predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacol. 34(13), 2655–2666. 10.1038/npp.2009.103
    https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2009.103 [Google Scholar]
  88. Tarvainen, M. P., Niskanen, J. P., Lipponen, J. A., Ranta-Aho, P. O., & Karjalainen, P. A.
    (2014) Kubios HRV–heart rate variability analysis software. Computer methods and programs in biomedicine, 113(1), 210–220. 10.1016/j.cmpb.2013.07.024
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cmpb.2013.07.024 [Google Scholar]
  89. Tarvainen, M. P., Ranta-Aho, P. O., & Karjalainen, P. A.
    (2002) An advanced detrending method with application to HRV analysis. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering, 49(2), 172–175. 10.1109/10.979357
    https://doi.org/10.1109/10.979357 [Google Scholar]
  90. Thayer, J. F., & Lane, R. D.
    (2000) A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. Journal of affective disorders, 61(3), 201–216. 10.1016/S0165‑0327(00)00338‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4 [Google Scholar]
  91. Tickle-Degnen, L., & Rosenthal, R.
    (1990) The nature of rapport and its nonverbal correlates. Psychological inquiry, 1(4), 285–293. 10.1207/s15327965pli0104_1
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327965pli0104_1 [Google Scholar]
  92. Tropp, L. R., & Wright, S. C.
    (2001) Ingroup identification as the inclusion of ingroup in the self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 27(5), 585–600. 10.1177/0146167201275007
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167201275007 [Google Scholar]
  93. Waddington, K., & Fletcher, C.
    (2005) Gossip and emotion in nursing and health-care organizations. Journal of health organization and management, 19(4/5), 378–394. 10.1108/14777260510615404
    https://doi.org/10.1108/14777260510615404 [Google Scholar]
  94. Warner, R. M.
    (1996) Coordinated cycles in behavior and physiology during face-to-face social interactions. InJ. H. Watt & C. A. VanLear (Eds.), Dynamic patterns in communication processes (pp.327–352). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Watson, D., Clark, L. A., & Tellegen, A.
    (1988) Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. Journal of personality and social psychology, 54(6), 1063. 10.1037/0022‑3514.54.6.1063
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.6.1063 [Google Scholar]
  96. White, H.
    (1982) Maximum likelihood estimation of misspecified models. Econometrica: Journal of the Econometric Society, 50, 1–25. 10.2307/1912526
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1912526 [Google Scholar]
  97. Wu, T., Luo, Y., Broster, L. S., Gu, R., & Luo, Y. J.
    (2013) The impact of anxiety on social decision-making: Behavioral and electrodermal findings. Social neuroscience, 8(1), 11–21. 10.1080/17470919.2012.694372
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2012.694372 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/is.18015.rud
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/is.18015.rud
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error