1887
Volume 21, Issue 3
  • ISSN 1572-0373
  • E-ISSN: 1572-0381
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Evolutionists studying human cooperation disagree about how to best explain it. One view is that humans are predisposed to engage in costly cooperation and punishment of free-riders as a result of culture/gene coevolution via group selection. Alternatively, some researchers argue that context-specific cognitive mechanisms associated with traditional neo-Darwinian self- and kin-maximization models sufficiently explain all aspects of human cooperation and punishment. There has been a great deal of research testing predictions derived from both positions; still, researchers generally agree that more naturalistic data are needed to complement mathematical modeling and laboratory and field experiments. Most of these data have been obtained from small-scale forager and other societies, but modern intentional communities offer another productive source of information. This exploratory study describes context-specific patterns of punishment in 46 American intentional communities that cast doubt on the prediction that people are predisposed to punish free-riders in naturalistic interactions.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/is.19011.qir
2021-02-09
2024-10-04
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Amici, F.
    (2015) The evolution and development of human cooperation. Interaction Studies, 16, 383–418. doi:  10.1075/is.16.3.03ami
    https://doi.org/10.1075/is.16.3.03ami [Google Scholar]
  2. Baumard, N.
    (2010) Has punishment played a role in the evolution of cooperation? A critical review. Mind & Society, 9, 171–192. doi:  10.1007/s11299‑010‑0079‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299-010-0079-9 [Google Scholar]
  3. (2012) The restorative logic of punishment: Another argument in favor of weak selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 17–18. doi:  10.1017/S0140525X11001166
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11001166 [Google Scholar]
  4. Bernard, H. R.
    (2011) Research methods in anthropology: Qualitative and quantitative approaches. (5th ed). Lanham, MD: AltaMira.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Boehm, C.
    (2012) Costs and benefits in hunter-gatherer punishment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 19–20. doi:  10.1017/S0140525X11001403
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11001403 [Google Scholar]
  6. Bowles, S. , & Gintis, H.
    (2004) The evolution of strong reciprocity: Cooperation in heterogeneous populations. Theoretical Population Biology, 65, 17–28. doi:  10.1016/j.tpb.2003.07.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tpb.2003.07.001 [Google Scholar]
  7. (2011) A cooperative species: Human reciprocity and its evolution. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Boyd, R. , Gintis, H. , & Bowles, S.
    (2010) Coordinated punishment of defectors sustains cooperation and can proliferate when rare. Science, 328, 617–620. doi:  10.1126/science.1183665
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1183665 [Google Scholar]
  9. Boyd, R. , & Richerson, P. J.
    (2009) Culture and the evolution of human cooperation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, 364, 3281–3288. doi:  10.1098/rstb.2009.0134
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0134 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bressan, P. , & Kramer, P.
    (2015) Human kin detection. Cognitive Science, 6, 299–311. doi:  10.1002/wcs.1347
    https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1347 [Google Scholar]
  11. Brown, S. L.
    (2002) Introduction. In S. L. Brown (Ed.), Intentional community: An anthropological perspective (pp.1–16). Albany, NY: SUNY Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Burnham, T. , & Johnson, D. D. P.
    (2005) The biological and evolutionary logic of human cooperation. Analyse & Kritik, 27, 113–135. doi:  10.1515/auk‑2005‑0107
    https://doi.org/10.1515/auk-2005-0107 [Google Scholar]
  13. Christensen, K. , & Levinson, D.
    (2003) Encyclopedia of community: From the village to the virtual world. (Vol.1). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 10.4135/9781412952583
    https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412952583 [Google Scholar]
  14. Cronk, L. , & Leech, B. L.
    (2013) Meeting at Grand Central: Understanding the social and evolutionary roots of cooperation. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. 10.1515/9781400845484
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9781400845484 [Google Scholar]
  15. Dennett, D. C.
    (1999) The evolution of culture. Edge, February16. https://www.edge.org/conversation/the-evolution-of-cultureAccessed22 September 2018.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Dreber, A. , Rand, D. G. , Fudenberg, D. , & Nowak, M. A.
    (2008) Winners don’t punish. Nature, 452, 348–51. doi:  10.1038/nature06723
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06723 [Google Scholar]
  17. Engel, C.
    (2011) Dictator games: A meta study. Experimental Economics, 14, 583–610. doi:  10.1007/s10683‑011‑9283‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-011-9283-7 [Google Scholar]
  18. Fehr, E. , Fischbacher, U. , & Gächter, S.
    (2002) Strong reciprocity, human cooperation, and the enforcement of social norms. Human Nature, 13, 1–25. 10.1007/s12110‑002‑1012‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-002-1012-7 [Google Scholar]
  19. Fehr, E. , & Gächter, S.
    (2002) Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature. 415, 137–40. doi:  10.1038/415137a
    https://doi.org/10.1038/415137a [Google Scholar]
  20. Fehr, E. , & Henrich, J.
    (2003) Is strong reciprocity a maladaptation? On the evolutionary foundations of human altruism. In: P. Hammerstein (Ed.), The genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation (pp.55–82). Cambridge, MA: MIT.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Fehr, E. , & Leibbrandt, A.
    (2011) A field study on cooperativeness and impatience in the Tragedy of the Commons. Journal of Public Economics, 95, 1144–1155. doi:  10.1016/j.jpubeco.2011.05.013
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2011.05.013 [Google Scholar]
  22. Ferguson, E. , & Corr, P.
    (2012) Blood, sex, personality, power, and altruism: Factors influencing the validity of strong reciprocity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 25–26. doi:  10.1017/S0140525X11001245
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11001245 [Google Scholar]
  23. Gächter, S.
    (2012) In the lab and the field: Punishment is rare in equilibrium. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35, 26–28. 10.1017/S0140525X11001415
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11001415 [Google Scholar]
  24. Gintis, H.
    (2011) Gene–culture coevolution and the nature of human sociality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366, 878–888. doi:  10.1098/rstb.2010.0310
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0310 [Google Scholar]
  25. Gintis, H. , Smith, E. A. , & Bowles, S.
    (2001) Costly signaling and cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 213, 103–119. doi:  10.1006/jtbi.2001.2406
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jtbi.2001.2406 [Google Scholar]
  26. Glowacki, L. , & von Rueden, C.
    (2015) Leadership solves collective action problems in small-scale societies. Philosophical Transactions of The Royal Society B, 370, 20150010. doi:  10.1098/rstb.2015.0010
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0010 [Google Scholar]
  27. Glowacki, L. , & Wrangham, R. W.
    (2013) The role of rewards in motivating participation in simple warfare. Human Nature, 24, 444–460. doi:  10.1007/s12110‑013‑9178‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-013-9178-8 [Google Scholar]
  28. Guala, F.
    (2012) Reciprocity: weak or strong? What punishment experiments do (and do not) demonstrate. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 35, 1–15. doi:  10.1017/S0140525X11000069
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X11000069 [Google Scholar]
  29. Hamilton, W. D.
    (1964) The genetical evolution of social behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–16. doi:  10.1016/0022‑5193(64)90038‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-5193(64)90038-4 [Google Scholar]
  30. Henrich, J. , & Boyd, R.
    (1998) The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 215–241. doi:  10.1016/S1090‑5138(98)00018‑X
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(98)00018-X [Google Scholar]
  31. Henrich, J. , Ensminger, J. , McElreath, R. , Barr, A. , Barrett, C. , Bolyanatz, A. , Cardenas, J. C. ,
    (2010) Market, religion, community size and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 327, 1480–1484. doi:  10.1126/science.1182238
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1182238 [Google Scholar]
  32. Henrich, J. , & Henrich, N.
    (2006) Culture, evolution and the puzzle of human cooperation. Cognitive Systems Research, 7, 220–245. doi:  10.1016/j.cogsys.2005.11.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2005.11.010 [Google Scholar]
  33. Henrich, J. , McElreath, R. , Ensminger, J. , Barr, A. , Barrett, C. , Bolyanatz, A. , Cardenas, J. C. ,
    (2006) Costly punishment across human societies. Science, 312, 1767–1770. doi:  10.1126/science.1127333
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1127333 [Google Scholar]
  34. Hill, K. , Walker, R. S. , Bozicević, M. , Eder, J. , Headland, T. , Hewlett, B. , Hurtado, A. M. , Marlowe, F. , Wiessner, P. , & Wood, B.
    (2011) Co-residence patterns in hunter-gatherer societies show unique human social structure. Science, 331, 1286–1289. doi:  10.1126/science.1199071
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1199071 [Google Scholar]
  35. Johnson, N. , & Mislin, A.
    (2011) Trust games: A meta-analysis. Journal of Economic Psychology, 32, 865–889. doi:  10.1016/j.joep.2011.05.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2011.05.007 [Google Scholar]
  36. Jordan, J. J. , Hoffman, M. , Bloom, P. , & Rand, D. G.
    (2016) Third-party punishment as a costly signal of trustworthiness. Nature, 530, 473–476. doi:  10.1038/nature16981
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nature16981 [Google Scholar]
  37. Kaplan, H. S. , Schniter, E. , Smith, V. L. , & Wilson, B. J.
    (2012) Risk and the evolution of human exchange. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 279, 2930–2935. doi:  10.1098/rspb.2011.2614
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2011.2614 [Google Scholar]
  38. Kiper, J. , & Sosis, R.
    (2017) The logic and location of strong reciprocity: Anthropological and philosophical considerations. In: M. Li , & D. Tracer (Eds.), Interdisciplinary perspectives on fairness, equity, and justice (pp.107–128). Cham, Switzerland: Springer. doi:  10.1007/978‑3‑319‑58993‑0_7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-58993-0_7 [Google Scholar]
  39. Kozeny, G.
    (1995) Intentional communities: Lifestyles based on ideals. Community Directory, Fellowship for Intentional Communities. https://www.ic.org/wiki/intentional-communities-lifestyles-based-ideals/Accessed22 September 2018.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Kraft-Todd, G. T. , Yoeli, E. , Bhanot, S. , & Rand, D. G.
    (2015) Promoting cooperation in the field. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 96–101. doi:  10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.02.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.02.006 [Google Scholar]
  41. Krasnow, M. M. , Delton, A. W. , Cosmides, L. , & Tooby, J.
    (2016) Looking under the hood of third-party punishment reveals design for personal benefit. Psychological Science, 27, 405–418. doi:  10.1177/0956797615624469
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615624469 [Google Scholar]
  42. Kurzban, R. , DeScioli, P. , & O’Brien, Erin
    (2007) Audience effects on moralistic punishment. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 75–84. doi:  10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.06.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.06.001 [Google Scholar]
  43. Laland, K. N. , & Brown, G. R.
    (2011) Sense and nonsense: Evolutionary perspectives on human behavior. (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Marlowe, F. W.
    (2009) Hadza cooperation: Second-party punishment, yes; Third-party punishment, no. Human Nature, 20, 417–430. doi:  10.1007/s12110‑009‑9072‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-009-9072-6 [Google Scholar]
  45. Marlowe, F. W. , Berbesque, J. C. , Barr, A. ,
    (2008) More “altruistic” punishment in larger societies. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 275, 587–592. doi:  10.1098/rspb.2007.1517
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.1517 [Google Scholar]
  46. Mathew, S.
    (2015) Evolution of human cooperation. In: J. D. Wright (Ed.), International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed., Vol.11, pp.259–266). Oxford: Elsevier. 10.1016/B978‑0‑08‑097086‑8.81036‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.81036-3 [Google Scholar]
  47. (2017) How the second-order free rider problem is solved in a small-scale society. American Economic Review, 107, 578–81. doi:  10.1257/aer.p20171090
    https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.p20171090 [Google Scholar]
  48. Mathew, S. , & Boyd, R.
    (2011) Punishment sustains large-scale cooperation in prestate warfare. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108, 11375–11380. doi:  10.1073/pnas.1105604108
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1105604108 [Google Scholar]
  49. Melis, A. P. , & Semmann, D.
    (2010) How is human cooperation different?Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 365, 2663–2674. doi:  10.1098/rstb.2010.0157
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0157 [Google Scholar]
  50. Meltzer, G.
    (1999) Cohousing: Linking communitarianism and sustainability. Communal Societies, 19, 85–100.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Miller, T.
    (2013) The encyclopedic guide to American intentional communities. Clinton, NY: Richard W. Couter.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Nowak, M. A. , & Sigmund, K.
    (2005) Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437, 1291–1298. doi:  10.1038/nature04131
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04131 [Google Scholar]
  53. Ottone, S. , Ponzano, F. , & Zarri, L.
    (2008) Moral sentiments and material interests behind altruistic third-party punishment. Working Paper Series, Department of Economics, University of Verona, 48ISSN: 2036–2919 (paper), 2036–4679 (online). dse.univr.it/workingpapers/WP48.pdf
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Price, M. E.
    (2008) The resurrection of group selection as a theory of human cooperation. Social Justice Research, 21, 228–240. doi:  10.1007/s11211‑008‑0064‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-008-0064-1 [Google Scholar]
  55. Qirko, H. N.
    (2004) Altruistic celibacy, kin-cue manipulation, and the development of religious institutions. Zygon, 39, 681–706. doi:  10.1111/j.1467‑9744.2004.t01‑1‑00608.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9744.2004.t01-1-00608.x [Google Scholar]
  56. Richerson, P. J. , Boyd, R. T. , & Henrich, J.
    (2003) Cultural evolution of human cooperation. In: P. Hammerstein (Ed.), The genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation (pp.373–404). Cambridge, MA: MIT.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Roberts, G.
    (2005) Cooperation through interdependence. Animal Behavior, 70, 901–908. doi:  10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.02.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2005.02.006 [Google Scholar]
  58. (2015) Partner choice drives the evolution of cooperation via indirect reciprocity. PLOS ONE, 10, e0129442. doi:  10.1371/journal.pone.0129442
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0129442 [Google Scholar]
  59. Rustagi, D. , Engel, S. , & Kosfeld, M.
    (2010) Conditional cooperation and costly monitoring explain success in forest commons management. Science, 330, 961–965. doi:  10.1126/science.1193649
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1193649 [Google Scholar]
  60. Schaub, L.
    (2010) Good news in hard times. In L. Schaub (Ed.), Communities Directory (6th ed., pp.11–13). Rutledge, MO: Fellowship for Intentional Community.
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Schensul, S. L. , Schensul, J. J. , & LeCompte, M. D.
    (1999) Essential ethnographic methods. Lanham, MD: AltaMira.
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Shenker, B.
    (1986) Intentional communities: Ideology and alienation in communal societies. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Sosis, R. , & Bressler, E.
    (2003) Cooperation and commune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research, 37, 211–239. doi:  10.1177/1069397103037002003
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1069397103037002003 [Google Scholar]
  64. Sutton, R. P.
    (2003) Communal utopias and the American experience. Westport, CT: Praeger.
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Trivers, R. L.
    (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57. doi:  10.1086/406755
    https://doi.org/10.1086/406755 [Google Scholar]
  66. Vromen, J.
    (2017) Ultimate and proximate explanations of strong reciprocity. History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences, 39, 25. doi:  10.1007/s40656‑017‑0151‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s40656-017-0151-4 [Google Scholar]
  67. Wang, Z. , Jusup, M. , Wang, R-W. , Shi, L. , Iwasa, Y. , Moreno, Y. , & Kurths, J.
    (2017) Onymity promotes cooperation in social dilemma experiments. Science Advances, 3, e1601444. doi:  10.1126/sciadv.1601444
    https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.1601444 [Google Scholar]
  68. Wiessner, P.
    (2005) Norm enforcement among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen: A case of strong reciprocity?Human Nature, 16, 115–145. doi:  10.1007/s12110‑005‑1000‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-005-1000-9 [Google Scholar]
  69. Yoeli, E. , Hoffman, M. , Rand, D. G. , & Nowak, M. A.
    (2013) Powering up with indirect reciprocity in a large-scale field experiment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110 (Supplement 2), 10424–10429. doi:  10.1073/pnas.1301210110
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1301210110 [Google Scholar]
  70. Young, P.
    (2003) The power of norms. In P. Hammerstein (Ed.), Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation (pp.389–399). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Zablocki, B.
    (1980) Alienation and charisma: A study of contemporary American communes. New York: Free Press.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/is.19011.qir
Loading
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error