1887
Volume 21, Issue 3
  • ISSN 1572-0373
  • E-ISSN: 1572-0381
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

The present study examined 17-month-olds’ imitation in a third-party context. In four experiments, the infants watched while a reliable or an unreliable model demonstrated a novel action with an unfamiliar (Experiments 1 and 3) or a familiar (Experiments 2 and 4) object to another adult. In Experiments 3 and 4, the second adult imitated the model’s novel action. Neither the familiarity of the object or whether or not the second adult copied the model’s behavior influenced the likelihood of infant imitation. Findings showed that the infants in the reliable model condition were more willing to imitate the model’s action with the unfamiliar object. The results suggest that infants take into account the reliability of a model even when the model has not directly demonstrated her reliability to the infant.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/is.19016.ste
2021-02-09
2024-10-04
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Akhtar, N. , Jipson, J. , & Callanan, M. A.
    (2001) Learning words through overhearing. Child Development, 72(2), 416–430. 10.1111/1467‑8624.00287
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00287 [Google Scholar]
  2. Barr, R. , Rovee-Collier, C. , & Campanella, J.
    (2005) Retrieval protracts deferred imitation by 6-month-olds. Infancy, 7(3), 263–283. 10.1207/s15327078in0703_3
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0703_3 [Google Scholar]
  3. Brooker, I. , & Poulin-Dubois, D.
    (2013) Is a bird an apple? The effect of speaker labeling accuracy on infants’ word learning, imitation, and helping behaviors. Infancy, 18(S1), E46–E68. 10.1111/infa.12027
    https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12027 [Google Scholar]
  4. Brugger, A. , Lariviere, L. A. , Mumme, D. L. , & Bushnell, E. W.
    (2007) Doing the right thing: Infants’ selection of actions to imitate from observed event sequences. Child Development, 78(3), 806–824. 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2007.01034.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01034.x [Google Scholar]
  5. Clément, F. , Koenig, M. , & Harris, P.
    (2004) The ontogenesis of trust. Mind and Language, 19(4), 360–379. 10.1111/j.0268‑1064.2004.00263.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0268-1064.2004.00263.x [Google Scholar]
  6. Floor, P. , & Akhtar, N.
    (2006) Can 18-month-old infants learn words by listening in on conversations?Infancy, 9(3), 327–339. 10.1207/s15327078in0903_4
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0903_4 [Google Scholar]
  7. Gampe, A. , Liebal, K. , & Tomasello, M.
    (2012) Eighteen-month-olds learn novel words through overhearing. First Language, 32(3), 385–397. 10.1177/0142723711433584
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723711433584 [Google Scholar]
  8. Haun, D. B. M. , Rekers, Y. , & Tomasello, M.
    (2014) Children conform to the behavior of peers; Other great apes stick with what they know. Psychological Science, 25(12), 2160–2167. 10.1177/0956797614553235
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614553235 [Google Scholar]
  9. (2012) Majority biased transmission in chimpanzees and human children, but not orangutans. Current Biology, 22(8), 727–731. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.006 [Google Scholar]
  10. Herold, K. H. , & Akhtar, N.
    (2008) Imitative learning from a third-party interaction: Relations with self-recognition and perspective taking. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(2), 114–123. 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.004 [Google Scholar]
  11. Király, I. , Csibra, G. , & Gergely, G.
    (2013) Beyond rational imitation: Learning arbitrary means actions from communicative demonstrations. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 471–486. 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.12.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.12.003 [Google Scholar]
  12. Krogh-Jespersen, S. , & Echols, C. H.
    (2012) The influence of speaker reliability on first versus second label learning. Child Development, 83(2), 581–590. 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2011.01713.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01713.x [Google Scholar]
  13. Matheson, H. , Moore, C. , & Akhtar, N.
    (2013) The development of social learning in interactive and observational contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 161–172. 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.003 [Google Scholar]
  14. Meltzoff, A. N.
    (1988) Infant imitation and memory: Nine-month-olds in immediate and deferred tests. Child Development, 59(1), 217–225. 10.2307/1130404
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1130404 [Google Scholar]
  15. Nielsen, M.
    (2006) Copying actions and copying outcomes: Social learning through the second year. Developmental Psychology, 42(3), 555–565. 10.1037/0012‑1649.42.3.555
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.42.3.555 [Google Scholar]
  16. Nielsen, M. , & Blank, C.
    (2011) Imitation in young children: When who gets copied is more important than what gets copied. Developmental Psychology, 47(4), 1050–1053. 10.1037/a0023866
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023866 [Google Scholar]
  17. Nielsen, M. , Moore, C. , & Mohamedally, J.
    (2012) Young children overimitate in third-party contexts. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(1), 73–83. 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.01.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.01.001 [Google Scholar]
  18. Over, H. , & Carpenter, M.
    (2015) Children infer affiliative and status relations from watching others imitate. Developmental Science, 18(6), 917–925. 10.1111/desc.12275
    https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12275 [Google Scholar]
  19. Poulin-Dubois, D. , Brooker, I. , & Polonia, A.
    (2011) Infants prefer to imitate a reliable person. Infant Behavior and Development, 34(2), 303–309. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.01.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.01.006 [Google Scholar]
  20. Repacholi, B. M. , & Meltzoff, A. N.
    (2007) Emotional eavesdropping: Infants selectively respond to indirect emotional signals. Child Development, 78(2), 503–521. 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2007.01012.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01012.x [Google Scholar]
  21. Seehagen, S. , & Herbert, J. S.
    (2012) Selective imitation in 6-month-olds: The role of the social and physical context. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(3), 509–512. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.05.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.05.002 [Google Scholar]
  22. Shimpi, P. M. , Akhtar, N. , & Moore, C.
    (2013) Toddlers’ imitative learning in interactive and observational contexts: the role of age and familiarity of the model. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116(2), 309–323. 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.008 [Google Scholar]
  23. Shneidman, L. , Todd, R. , & Woodward, A. L.
    (2014) Why do directed interactions support imitative learning in young children?PLoS ONE, 9(10). 10.1371/journal.pone.0110891
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0110891 [Google Scholar]
  24. Stenberg, G.
    (2019) The situational context and the reliability of an adult model influence infants’ imitation. Interaction Studies, 20(2), 376–391. 10.1075/is.18065.ste
    https://doi.org/10.1075/is.18065.ste [Google Scholar]
  25. (2013) Do 12-month-old infants trust a competent adult?Infancy, 18(5), 873–904. 10.1111/infa.12011
    https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12011 [Google Scholar]
  26. Zmyj, N. , Buttelmann, D. , Carpenter, M. , & Daum, M. M.
    (2010) The reliability of a model influences 14-month-olds’ imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(4), 208–220. 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.03.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.03.002 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/is.19016.ste
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/is.19016.ste
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): imitation; infants; reliable model; third-party context; unreliable model
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error