1887
Volume 24, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1572-0373
  • E-ISSN: 1572-0381
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Pet-directed speech (PDS) is often produced by humans when addressing dogs. Similar to infant-directed speech, PDS is marked by a relatively higher and more modulated fundamental frequency () than is adult-directed speech. We tested the prediction that increasing eye size in dogs, one facial feature of neoteny (juvenilisation), would elicit exaggerated prosodic qualities or pet-directed speech. We experimentally manipulated eye size in photographs of twelve dog breeds by −15%, +15% and +30%. We first showed that dogs with larger eyes were indeed perceived as younger. We then recorded men and women speaking towards these photographs, who also rated these images for cuteness. Linear mixed-effects models demonstrated that increasing eye size by 15% significantly increased pitch range ( range) and variability (CV) among women only. Cuteness ratings did not vary with eye size, due to a possible ceiling effect across eye sizes. Our results offer preliminary evidence that large eyes can elicit pet-directed speech and suggest that PDS may be modulated by perceived juvenility rather than cuteness. We discuss these findings in the context of inter-species vocal communication.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/is.22032.for
2023-08-28
2025-06-24
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Archer, J.
    (1997) Why do people love their pets?Evolution and Human Behavior, 18(4), 237–259. 10.1016/S0162‑3095(99)80001‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0162-3095(99)80001-4 [Google Scholar]
  2. Archer, J. & Monton, S.
    (2011) Preferences for infant facial features in pet dogs and cats. Ethology, 1171: 217–226. 10.1111/j.1439‑0310.2010.01863.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01863.x [Google Scholar]
  3. Belyaev, D. K.
    (1979) Destabilizing selection as a factor in domestication. J. Hered. 701, 301–308. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a109263
    https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.jhered.a109263 [Google Scholar]
  4. Ben-Aderet, T., Gallego-Abenza, M., Reby, D., & Mathevon, N.
    (2017) Dog-directed speech: why do we use it and do dogs pay attention to it?. Proc. R. Soc.B, 284(1846), 20162429. 10.1098/rspb.2016.2429
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2016.2429 [Google Scholar]
  5. Benders, T.
    (2017) Six-Month-Old Infants Prefer Speech with Raised Formant Frequencies. Infancy, 22(6), 778–789. 10.1111/infa.12190
    https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12190 [Google Scholar]
  6. Benders, T., StGeorge, J., & Fletcher, R.
    (2021) Infant-directed speech by Dutch fathers: Increased pitch variability within and across utterances. Language Learning and Development, 17(3), 292–325. 10.1080/15475441.2021.1876698
    https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2021.1876698 [Google Scholar]
  7. Benjamin, A., & Slocombe, K.
    (2018) ‘Who’s a good boy?!’Dogs prefer naturalistic dog-directed speech. Animal Cognition, 21(3), 353–364. 10.1007/s10071‑018‑1172‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-018-1172-4 [Google Scholar]
  8. Biersack, S., Kempe, V., & Knapton, L.
    (2005) Fine-tuning speech registers: a comparison of the prosodic features of child-directed and foreigner-directed speech. InNinth European Conference on Speech Communication and Technology. 10.21437/Interspeech.2005‑46
    https://doi.org/10.21437/Interspeech.2005-46 [Google Scholar]
  9. Boersma, P., & Weenink, D.
    (2009) Praat: doing phonetics by computer (5.3.17; p.www.praat.org/). www.praat.org
  10. Borgi, M., & Cirulli, F.
    (2013) Children’s preferences for infantile features in dogs and cats. Human-Animal Interaction Bulletin, 1(2), 1–15. 10.1079/hai.2013.0010
    https://doi.org/10.1079/hai.2013.0010 [Google Scholar]
  11. Borgi, M., Cogliati-Dezza, I., Brelsford, V., Meints, K., & Cirulli, F.
    (2014) Baby schema in human and animal faces induces cuteness perception and gaze allocation in children. Frontiers in Psychology, 51, 411. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00411
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00411 [Google Scholar]
  12. Burnham, D., Kitamura, C., & Vollmer-Conna, U.
    (2002) What’s new, pussycat? On talking to babies and animals. Science, 296(5572), pp. 1435–1435. 10.1126/science.1069587
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1069587 [Google Scholar]
  13. Cristia, A.
    (2013) Input to language: The phonetics and perception of infant-directed speech. Language and Linguistics Compass, 7(3), 157–170. 10.1111/lnc3.12015
    https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12015 [Google Scholar]
  14. de Mouzon, C., Gonthier, M., & Leboucher, G.
    (2023) Discrimination of cat-directed speech from human-directed speech in a population of indoor companion cats (Felis catus). Animal cognition, 26(2), 611–619. 10.1007/s10071‑022‑01674‑w
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-022-01674-w [Google Scholar]
  15. Fernald, A., Taeschner, T., Dunn, J., Papousek, M., de Boysson-Bardies, B., & Fukui, I.
    (1989) A cross-language study of prosodic modifications in mothers’ and fathers’ speech to preverbal infants. Journal of child language, 16(3), 477501. 10.1017/S0305000900010679
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900010679 [Google Scholar]
  16. Gergely, A., Faragó, T., Galambos, Á., & Topál, J.
    (2017) Differential effects of speech situations on mothers’ and fathers’ infant-directed and dog-directed speech: An acoustic analysis. Scientific Reports, 7(13739). 10.1038/s41598‑017‑13883‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-13883-2 [Google Scholar]
  17. Glocker, M. L., Langleben, D. D., Ruparel, K., Loughead, J. W., Gur, R. C., & Sachser, N.
    (2009) Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology, 115(3), 257–263. 10.1111/j.1439‑0310.2008.01603.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01603.x [Google Scholar]
  18. Golinkoff, R. M., Can, D. D., Soderstrom, M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K.
    (2015) (Baby) talk to me: The social context of infant-directed speech and its effects on early language acquisition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(5), 339–344. 10.1177/0963721415595345
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721415595345 [Google Scholar]
  19. Hecht, J., & Horowitz, A.
    (2015) Seeing dogs: Human preferences for dog physical attributes. Anthrozoös, 28(1), 153–163. 10.2752/089279315X14129350722217
    https://doi.org/10.2752/089279315X14129350722217 [Google Scholar]
  20. Hirsh-Pasek, K., & Treiman, R.
    (1982) Doggerel: Motherese in a new context. Journal of Child Language, 91, 229–237. 10.1017/S0305000900003731
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900003731 [Google Scholar]
  21. Hollien, H.
    (2014) Vocal fold dynamics for frequency change. Journal of Voice, 28(4), 395–405. 10.1016/j.jvoice.2013.12.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2013.12.005 [Google Scholar]
  22. Jardat, P., Calandreau, L., Ferreira, V., Gouyet, C., Parias, C., Reigner, F., & Lansade, L.
    (2022) Pet-directed speech improves horses’ attention toward humans. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 1–8. 10.1038/s41598‑022‑08109‑z
    https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-08109-z [Google Scholar]
  23. Jeannin, S., Gilbert, C., Amy, M., & Leboucher, G.
    (2017) Pet-directed speech draws adult dogs’ attention more efficiently than Adult-directed speech. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 4980. 10.1038/s41598‑017‑04671‑z
    https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-04671-z [Google Scholar]
  24. Kaminski, J., Waller, B. M., Diogo, R., Hartstone-Rose, A., & Burrows, A. M.
    (2019) Evolution of facial muscle anatomy in dogs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(29), 14677–14681. 10.1073/pnas.1820653116
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1820653116 [Google Scholar]
  25. Lesch, R., Kotrschal, K., Schöberl, I., Beetz, A., Solomon, J., & Fitch, W. T.
    (2019) Talking to dogs: Companion animal-directed speech in a stress test. Animals, 9(7), 417. 10.3390/ani9070417
    https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9070417 [Google Scholar]
  26. Little, A. C.
    (2012) Manipulation of infant-like traits affects perceived cuteness of infant, adult and cat faces. Ethology, 118(8), 775–782. 10.1111/j.1439‑0310.2012.02068.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2012.02068.x [Google Scholar]
  27. Lorenz, K.
    (1943) Die angeborenen formen möglicher erfahrung. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie, 5(2), 235–409. 10.1111/j.1439‑0310.1943.tb00655.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.1943.tb00655.x [Google Scholar]
  28. (1970) Studies in animal and human behaviour, Volume 2. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. MacDorman, K. F., Green, R. D., Ho, C. C., & Koch, C. T.
    (2009) Too real for comfort? Uncanny responses to computer generated faces. Computers in Human Behavior, 25(3), 695–710. 10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.026
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2008.12.026 [Google Scholar]
  30. Masataka, N.
    (1998) Perception of motherese in Japanese sign language by 6-monthold hearing infants. Developmental Psychology, 341, 241–246. 10.1037/0012‑1649.34.2.241
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.34.2.241 [Google Scholar]
  31. McGreevy, P. D., & Nicholas, F. W.
    (1999) Some practical solutions to welfare problems in dog breeding. Animal Welfare, 81, 329–341. 10.1017/S0962728600021965
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600021965 [Google Scholar]
  32. Pettijohn, T. F., & Tesser, A.
    (2005) Threat and social choice: When eye size matters. The Journal of Social Psychology, 145(5), 547–570. 10.3200/SOCP.145.5.547‑570
    https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.145.5.547-570 [Google Scholar]
  33. Pittenger, J. B., Shaw, R. E., & Mark, L. S.
    (1979) Perceptual information for the age level of faces as a higher order invariant of growth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5(3), 478.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Prato-Previde, E., Fallani, G., & Valsecchi, P.
    (2006) Gender differences in owners interacting with pet dogs: an observational study. Ethology, 112(1), 64–73. 10.1111/j.1439‑0310.2006.01123.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2006.01123.x [Google Scholar]
  35. Price, E. O.
    (1999) Behavioral development in animals undergoing domestication. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 65(3), 245–271. 10.1016/S0168‑1591(99)00087‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00087-8 [Google Scholar]
  36. Serpell, J.
    (2003) Anthropomorphism and anthropomorphic selection – beyond the “cute response”. Society & Animals, 11(1), 83–100. 10.1163/156853003321618864
    https://doi.org/10.1163/156853003321618864 [Google Scholar]
  37. Seyama, J. I., & Nagayama, R. S.
    (2007) The uncanny valley: Effect of realism on the impression of artificial human faces. Presence, 16(4), 337–351. 10.1162/pres.16.4.337
    https://doi.org/10.1162/pres.16.4.337 [Google Scholar]
  38. Sternglanz, S. H., Gray, J. L., & Murakami, M.
    (1977) Adult preferences for infantile facial features: An ethological approach. Animal Behaviour, 251, 108–115. 10.1016/0003‑3472(77)90072‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(77)90072-0 [Google Scholar]
  39. Trainor, L. J., & Desjardins, R. N.
    (2002) Pitch characteristics of infant-directed speech affect infants’ ability to discriminate vowels. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(2), 335–340. 10.3758/BF03196290
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196290 [Google Scholar]
  40. Trut, L. N.
    (1999) Early Canid Domestication: The Farm-Fox Experiment: Foxes bred for tamability in a 40-year experiment exhibit remarkable transformations that suggest an interplay between behavioral genetics and development. American Scientist, 87(2), 160–169. 10.1511/1999.20.160
    https://doi.org/10.1511/1999.20.160 [Google Scholar]
  41. Uther, M., Knoll, M. A., & Burnham, D.
    (2007) Do you speak E-NG-LI-SH? A comparison of foreigner-and infant-directed speech. Speech Communication, 49(1), 2–7. 10.1016/j.specom.2006.10.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2006.10.003 [Google Scholar]
  42. Waller, B. M., Peirce, K., Caeiro, C. C., Scheider, L., Burrows, A. M., McCune, S., & Kaminski, J.
    (2013) Paedomorphic facial expressions give dogs a selective advantage. PLoS one, 8(12), e82686. 10.1371/journal.pone.0082686
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0082686 [Google Scholar]
  43. Weirich, M., & Simpson, A.
    (2019) Effects of Gender, Parental Role, and Time on Infant- and Adult-Directed Read and Spontaneous Speech. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 62 (11), 1–14. 10.1044/2019_JSLHR‑S‑19‑0047
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-19-0047 [Google Scholar]
  44. Woo, B. M., & Schaller, M.
    (2020) “Parental” responses to human infants (and puppy dogs): Evidence that the perception of eyes is especially influential, but eye contact is not. PloS one, 15(5), e0232059. 10.1371/journal.pone.0232059
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0232059 [Google Scholar]
  45. Xu, N., Burnham, D., Kitamura, C., & Vollmer-Conna, U.
    (2013) Vowel hyperarticulation in parrot-, dog-and infant-directed speech. Anthrozoös, 26(3), 373–380. 10.2752/175303713X13697429463592
    https://doi.org/10.2752/175303713X13697429463592 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/is.22032.for
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/is.22032.for
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error