1887
Volume 24, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1572-0373
  • E-ISSN: 1572-0381
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Adult-infant early dyadic interactions have been extensively explored by developmental psychologists. Around the age of 2 months, infants already demonstrate complex, delicate and very sensitive behaviors that seem to express their ability to interact and share emotions with their caregivers. This paper presents 3 pilot studies of parent-infant dyadic interaction in various set-ups. The first two present longitudinal data collected on two infants aged between 1 and 6 months and their mothers. We analyzed the development of coordination between them, at the motor and at the vocal level. The 3rd pilot study aims to explore interpersonal coordination in both vocal behavior and motor activity for one infant and his mother at 2, 4 and 6 months. These pilot studies however leave a number of questions open concerning developmental changes and infants’ progressive mastery of interaction. We identify areas worth examining and try to tease out specific issues that may help develop new methodological pathways for the study of early naturalistic social interaction. We assume that a continuous, rather than discrete, approach would better capture the changes taking place in the various communicative modalities, while also displaying each dyad’s specificity and the narrative dimension of social engagement between infants and caregivers.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/is.22041.jov
2023-08-28
2025-04-17
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Abney, D. H., Warlaumont, A. S., Haussman, A., Ross, J. M., & Wallot, S.
    (2014) Using nonlinear methods to quantify changes in infant limb movements and vocalizations. Frontiers in psychology, 51, 771. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00771
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00771 [Google Scholar]
  2. Abu-Zhaya, R., Seidl, A., & Cristia, A.
    (2017) Multimodal infant-directed communication: How caregivers combine tactile and linguistic cues. Journal of Child Language, 44(5), 1088–1116. 10.1017/S0305000916000416
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000416 [Google Scholar]
  3. Ainsworth, M. D., Blehar, M., Waters, E., & Wall, S.
    (1978) Patterns of attachment. A Psychological Study of the Strange Situation. New-York, Taylor & Francis.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Als, H., Lawhon, G., Duffy, F. H., McAnulty, G. B., Gibes-Grossman, R., & Blickman, J. G.
    (1994) Individualized developmental care for the very low-birth-weight preterm infant: medical and neurofunctional effects. Jama, 272(11), 853–858. 10.1001/jama.1994.03520110033025
    https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.1994.03520110033025 [Google Scholar]
  5. Auer, P., Auer, P. L., Couper-Kuhlen, E., & Müller, F.
    (1999) Language in time: The rhythm and tempo of spoken interaction. Oxford University Press on Demand.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Bahrick, L. E., & Lickliter, R.
    (2002) Intersensory redundancy guides early perceptual and cognitive development. Advances in child development and behavior, 31, 153–187.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. (2012) The role of intersensory redundancy in early perceptual, cognitive, and social development. InA. Bremner, D. J. Lewkowicz, & C. Spence (Eds.), Multisensory development (pp.183–205), Oxford University Press: Oxford. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586059.003.0008
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199586059.003.0008 [Google Scholar]
  8. Bateson, G.
    (1958) Schizophrenic distortions of communication. InC. A. Whitacker (Ed), Psychotherapy of Chronic Schizophrenic Patients. Boston.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Bateson, M. C.
    (1975) Mother-infant exchanges: the epigenesis of conversational interaction. Ann. N. Y.Acad. Sci.2631, 101–113. 10.1111/j.1749‑6632.1975.tb41575.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1975.tb41575.x [Google Scholar]
  10. (1979) The epigenesis of conversational interaction. InM. Bullowa (Ed.), Before Speech (pp.63–77). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Beebe, B., Messinger, D., Bahrick, L. E., Margolis, A., Buck, K. A., & Chen, H.
    (2016) A systems view of mother–infant face-to-face communication. Developmental Psychology, 52(4), 556. 10.1037/a0040085
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0040085 [Google Scholar]
  12. Beebe, B., & Gerstman, L. J.
    (1980) The “packaging” of maternal stimulation in relation to infant facial-visual engagement: A case study at four months. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 26(4), 321–339.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Bell, M. A.
    (2020) Mother-child behavioral and physiological synchrony. InAdvances in Child Development and Behavior (Vol.581, pp.163–188). 10.1016/bs.acdb.2020.01.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.acdb.2020.01.006 [Google Scholar]
  14. Bergeson, T. R., & Trehub, S. E.
    (2007) Signature tunes in mothers’ speech to infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 30(4), 648–654. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.03.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.03.003 [Google Scholar]
  15. Bernstein, N. A.
    (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Oxford: Pergamon Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Bigelow, A. E., & Rochat, P.
    (2006) Two-month-old infants’ sensitivity to social contingency in mother–infant and stranger–infant interaction. Infancy, 9(3), 313–325. 10.1207/s15327078in0903_3
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0903_3 [Google Scholar]
  17. Bigelow, A. E., Power, M., Mcquaid, N., Ward, A., & Rochat, P.
    (2008) Distinguishing mother–infant interaction from stranger–infant interaction at 2, 4, and 6 months of age. Infancy, 13(2), 158–171. 10.1080/15250000701795614
    https://doi.org/10.1080/15250000701795614 [Google Scholar]
  18. Birdwhistell, R. L.
    (1970) Kinesics and Context: Essays on Body Motion Communication. New York: Ballentine Books
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Boersma, P., & Weenink, D.
    (2005) Praat. Doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.1).
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Brazelton, T. B.
    (1973) Effect of maternal expectations on early infant behavior. Early Child Development and Care, 2(3), 259–273. 10.1080/0300443730020301
    https://doi.org/10.1080/0300443730020301 [Google Scholar]
  21. Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B., & Main, M.
    (1974) The origins of reciprocity: The early mother–infant interaction. InM. Lewis & L. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver (pp.49–76). New York: Wiley.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Brockwell, P. J. and Davis, R. A.
    (1991) Time Series: Theory and Methods, 2nd Edition, Springer Verlag, NY, pp. 373–375, 405–417. 10.1007/978‑1‑4419‑0320‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0320-4 [Google Scholar]
  23. Brooks, R., & Meltzoff, A. N.
    (2014) Gaze following: A mechanism for building social connections between infants and adults. InM. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), The Herzliya series on personality and social psychology. Mechanisms of social connection: From brain to group (p.167–183). American Psychological Association. 10.1037/14250‑010
    https://doi.org/10.1037/14250-010 [Google Scholar]
  24. Bruner, J.
    (1983) Child’s Talk: Learning to Use Language. Oxford University Press
    [Google Scholar]
  25. (1990) Acts of Meaning. Harvard University Press
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Carretero Perez, S., & Español, S. A.
    (2016) Multimodal Study of Adult-Infant Interaction: A Review of Its Origins and Its Current Status. Paidéia, 26(65), 377–385. 10.1590/1982‑43272665201613
    https://doi.org/10.1590/1982-43272665201613 [Google Scholar]
  27. Cochet, H., Centelles, L., Jover, M., Plachta, S., Vauclair, J.
    (2015) Hand preferences in preschool children: Reaching, pointing and symbolic gestures. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 20(4), 501–516. 10.1080/1357650X.2015.1007057
    https://doi.org/10.1080/1357650X.2015.1007057 [Google Scholar]
  28. Cochet, H., Jover, M., & Vauclair, J.
    (2011) Hand preference for pointing gestures and bimanual manipulation around the vocabulary spurt period. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1101, 393–407. 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.009 [Google Scholar]
  29. Coco, M. I., & Dale, R.
    (2014) Cross-recurrence quantification analysis of categorical and continuous time series: an R package. Frontiers in psychology, 51, 510. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00510
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00510 [Google Scholar]
  30. Coco, M. I., Mønster, D., Leonardi, G., Dale, R., & Wallot, S.
    (2020) Unidimensional and Multidimensional Methods for Recurrence Quantification Analysis with crqa. arXiv preprint arXiv:2006.01954. 10.32614/RJ‑2021‑062
    https://doi.org/10.32614/RJ-2021-062 [Google Scholar]
  31. Condon, W. S.
    (1982) Cultural microrhythms. InM. Davis (Ed.), Interaction rhythms: Periodicity in communicative behavior (pp.31–52). New-York: Human Science.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Condon, W. S., & Ogston, W. D.
    (1967) A segmentation of behavior. Journal of psychiatric research, 5(3), 221–235. 10.1016/0022‑3956(67)90004‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(67)90004-0 [Google Scholar]
  33. Condon, W. S., & Sander, L. W.
    (1974a) Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: Interactional participation and language acquisition. Science, 183(4120), 99–101. 10.1126/science.183.4120.99
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.183.4120.99 [Google Scholar]
  34. (1974b) Synchrony demonstrated between movements of the neonate and adult speech. Child Development, 45(2), 456–462. 10.2307/1127968
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1127968 [Google Scholar]
  35. Cornejo, C., Hurtado, E., Cuadros, Z., Torres-Araneda, A., Paredes, J., Olivares, H.,
    (2018) Dynamics of simultaneous and imitative bodily coordination in trust and distrust. Front. Psychol. 91:1546. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01546
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01546 [Google Scholar]
  36. Cuardros, Z., Hurtado, E., & Cornejo, C.
    (2019) Measuring Dynamics of Infant-Adult Synchrony Through Mocap. Frontiers in Psychology, 101, 2839. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02839
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02839 [Google Scholar]
  37. Darwin, C.
    (1877) A biographical sketch of an infant. Mind, 2(7), 285–294. 10.1093/mind/os‑2.7.285
    https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/os-2.7.285 [Google Scholar]
  38. De Ruiter, J. P., Mitterer, H., & Enfield, N. J.
    (2006) Projecting the end of a speaker’s turn: A cognitive cornerstone of conversation. Language, 82(3), 515–535. 10.1353/lan.2006.0130
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2006.0130 [Google Scholar]
  39. Delafield-Butt, J. T., & Trevarthen, C.
    (2015) The ontogenesis of narrative: from moving to meaning. Frontiers in psychology, 61, 1157. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01157
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01157 [Google Scholar]
  40. Delaherche, E., Chetouani, M., Mahdhaoui, A., Saint-Georges, C., Viaux, S., & Cohen, D.
    (2012) Interpersonal synchrony: A survey of evaluation methods across disciplines. IEEE Transactions on Affective Computing, 3(3), 349–365. 10.1109/T‑AFFC.2012.12
    https://doi.org/10.1109/T-AFFC.2012.12 [Google Scholar]
  41. DiCesare, G., Di Dio, C., Rochat, M., Sinigaglia, C., Bruschweiler-Stern, N., Ster, D., & Rizzolatti, G.
    (2014) The neural correlates of vitality form recognition: An fMRI study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(7), 951–960.
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Dixon, S. D., Yogman, M., Tronick, E., Adamson, L., Als, H., & Brazelton, T. B.
    (1981) Early infant social interaction with parents and strangers. Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 20(1), 32–52. 10.1016/S0002‑7138(09)60715‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0002-7138(09)60715-9 [Google Scholar]
  43. Dominguez, S., Devouche, E., Apter, G., & Gratier, M.
    (2016) The roots of turn-taking in the neonatal period. Infant and Child Development, 25(3), 240–255. 10.1002/icd.1976
    https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1976 [Google Scholar]
  44. Farran, L. K., Yoo, H., Lee, C. C., Bowman, D. D., & Oller, D. K.
    (2019) Temporal Coordination in Mother-Infant Vocal Interaction: A Cross-Cultural Comparison. Frontiers in psychology, 101, 2374. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02374
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02374 [Google Scholar]
  45. Feldman, A. G., Goussev, V., Sangole, A., & Levin, M. F.
    (2007) Threshold position control and the principle of minimal interaction in motor actions. Progress in brain research, 1651, 267–281. 10.1016/S0079‑6123(06)65017‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(06)65017-6 [Google Scholar]
  46. Feldman, R.
    (2012) Parent–infant synchrony: A biobehavioral model of mutual influences in the formation of affiliative bonds. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 77(2), 42–51. 10.1111/j.1540‑5834.2011.00660.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00660.x [Google Scholar]
  47. Feldman, R., Greenbaum, C. W., Yirmiya, N., & Mayes, L. C.
    (1996) Relations between cyclicity and regulation in mother-infant interaction at 3 and 9 months and cognition at 2 years. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 17(3), 347–365. 10.1016/S0193‑3973(96)90031‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(96)90031-3 [Google Scholar]
  48. Feldstein, S., Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Crown, C. L., Jasnow, M., Fox, H., & Gordon, S.
    (1993) Coordinated interpersonal timing in adult-infant vocal interactions: A cross-site replication. Infant Behavior and Development, 16(4), 455–470. 10.1016/0163‑6383(93)80004‑R
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(93)80004-R [Google Scholar]
  49. Fogel, A., & Thelen, E.
    (1987) Development of early expressive and communicative action: Reinterpreting the evidence from a dynamic systems perspective. Developmental psychology, 23(6), 747. 10.1037/0012‑1649.23.6.747
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.6.747 [Google Scholar]
  50. Fogel, A., Nwokah, E., Dedo, J. Y., Messinger, D., Dickson, K. L., Matusov, E., & Holt, S. A.
    (1992) Social process theory of emotion: A dynamic systems approach. Social Development, 1(2), 122–142. 10.1111/j.1467‑9507.1992.tb00116.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.1992.tb00116.x [Google Scholar]
  51. Franchak, J. M., Kretch, K. S., Soska, K. C., & Adolph, K. E.
    (2011) Head-mounted eye tracking: A new method to describe infant looking. Child development, 82(6), 1738–1750. 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2011.01670.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01670.x [Google Scholar]
  52. Garfinkel, H.
    (1967) Studies in Ethno- methodology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Gentilucci, M., & Volta, R. D.
    (2008) Spoken language and arm gestures are controlled by the same motor control system. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(6), 944–957. 10.1080/17470210701625683
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210701625683 [Google Scholar]
  54. Gibson, J. J.
    (1966) The senses considered as perceptual systems. Houghton Mifflin.Goffman 1967
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Goffman, E.
    (1967) Interaction Ritual: Essays in Face to Face Behavior. Garden City, NY: Doubleday
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Goldstein, M. H., Schwade, J. A., & Bornstein, M. H.
    (2009) The value of vocalizing: Five-month-old infants associate their own noncry vocalizations with responses from caregivers. Child development, 80(3), 636–644. 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2009.01287.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01287.x [Google Scholar]
  57. Goodwin, C.
    (1979) The Interactive Construction of a Sentencein Natural Conversation. Everyday Language: Studies in Ethnomethodology. G. Psathas. New York, Irvington Publishers: 97–121.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Gratier, M.
    (2003) Expressive timing and interactional synchrony between mothers and infants: cultural similarities, cultural differences, and the immigration experience. Cognitive Development. 181, 533–554. 10.1016/j.cogdev.2003.09.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2003.09.009 [Google Scholar]
  59. Gratier, M., and Devouche, E.
    (2011) Imitation and repetition of prosodic contour in vocal interaction at 3 months. Developmental Psychology. 471, 67–76. 10.1037/a0020722
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020722 [Google Scholar]
  60. Gratier, M., Devouche, E., Guellai, B., Infanti, R., Yilmaz, E., & Parlato-Oliveira, E.
    (2015) Early development of turn-taking in vocal interaction between mothers and infants. Frontiers in psychology, 6(1167), 1–10. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01167
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01167 [Google Scholar]
  61. Gratier, M., & Trevarthen, C.
    (2008) Musical narrative and motives for culture in mother infant vocal interaction. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15(10–11), 122–158.
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Guellaï, B., Callin, A., Bevilacqua, F., Schwarz, D., Pitti, A., Boucenna, S., & Gratier, M.
    (2019) Sensus Communis: Some perspectives on the origins of non-synchronous cross-sensory associations. Frontiers in psychology, 101, 523. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00523
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00523 [Google Scholar]
  63. Hammal, Z., Cohn, J. F., & Messinger, D. S.
    (2015) Head movement dynamics during play and perturbed mother-infant interaction. IEEE transactions on affective computing, 6(4), 361–370. 10.1109/TAFFC.2015.2422702
    https://doi.org/10.1109/TAFFC.2015.2422702 [Google Scholar]
  64. Hammal, Z., Cohn, J. F., Messinger, D. S., Mattson, W. I., & Mahoor, M. H.
    (2013) Head movement dynamics during normal and perturbed parent-infant interaction. InProceedings of the International Conference on Affective Computing and Intelligent Interaction (pp.276–282). Los Alamitos, CA: IEEE. 10.1109/ACII.2013.52
    https://doi.org/10.1109/ACII.2013.52 [Google Scholar]
  65. Harder, S., Lange, T., Hansen, G. F., Væver, M., & Køppe, S.
    (2015) A longitudinal study of coordination in mother–infant vocal interaction from age 4 to 10 months. Developmental psychology, 51(12), 1778. 10.1037/a0039834
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039834 [Google Scholar]
  66. Hilbrink, E. E., Gattis, M., & Levinson, S. C.
    (2015) Early developmental changes in the timing of turn-taking: a longitudinal study of mother–infant interaction. Frontiers in psychology, 61, 1492. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01492
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01492 [Google Scholar]
  67. Hoehl, S., Fairhurst, M., & Schirmer, A.
    (2020) Interactional synchrony: signals, mechanisms and benefits. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 16(1–2), 5–18. 10.1093/scan/nsaa024
    https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa024 [Google Scholar]
  68. Iverson, J. M., & Thelen, E.
    (1999) Hand, mouth and brain. The dynamic emergence of speech and gesture. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 6(11–12), 19–40.
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., Feldstein, S., Crown, C. L., Jasnow, M. D., Rochat, P.,
    (2001) Rhythms of dialogue in infancy: coordinated timing in development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 661, 1–8. 10.1111/1540‑5834.00137
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1540-5834.00137 [Google Scholar]
  70. Jover, M., Vidal, E., Gulgieri, E., Scola, C.
    (2020) Are global movements a cue for understanding the infant’s mood?Graphic presentation at thevirtual International Conference on Infant Studies (ICIS), July 6–9 2020.
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Jover, M., Cellier, M., Scola, C.
    (2019) Infants’ motor activity during a mother infant interaction alternating silent and singing phases. Journal of motor learning and development, 71426–436. 10.1123/jmld.2019‑0001
    https://doi.org/10.1123/jmld.2019-0001 [Google Scholar]
  72. Kantz, H., & Schreiber, T.
    (2004) Nonlinear time series analysis (Vol. 7). Cambridge university press.
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Keller, H., Lohaus, A., Völker, S., Cappenberg, M., & Chasiotis, A.
    (1999) Temporal contingency as an independent component of parenting behavior. Child Development, 70(2), 474–485. 10.1111/1467‑8624.00034
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00034 [Google Scholar]
  74. Kendon, A.
    (1972) Some relationships between body motion and speech. InA. Seigman and B. Pope, editors, Studies in Dyadic Communication. Elmsford, New York: Pergamon Press. 10.1016/B978‑0‑08‑015867‑9.50013‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-08-015867-9.50013-7 [Google Scholar]
  75. Lamb, M. E.
    (1977) Father-infant and mother-infant interaction in the first year of life. Child development, 167–181. 10.2307/1128896
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1128896 [Google Scholar]
  76. Leclère, C., Viaux, S., Avril, M., Achard, C., Chetouani, M., Missonnier, S., & Cohen, D.
    (2014) Why synchrony matters during mother-child interactions: a systematic review. PloS one, 9(12). 10.1371/journal.pone.0113571
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0113571 [Google Scholar]
  77. Legerstee, M.
    (1990) Infants use multimodal information to imitate speech sounds. Infant behavior and development, 13(3), 343–354. 10.1016/0163‑6383(90)90039‑B
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(90)90039-B [Google Scholar]
  78. Leonardi, G., Nomikou, I., Rohlfing, K. J., & Rączaszek-Leonardi, J.
    (2016) Vocal interactions at the dawn of communication: the emergence of mutuality and complementarity in mother-infant interaction. In2016 Joint IEEE International Conference on Development and Learning and Epigenetic Robotics (ICDL-EpiRob) (pp.288–293). IEEE. 10.1109/DEVLRN.2016.7846835
    https://doi.org/10.1109/DEVLRN.2016.7846835 [Google Scholar]
  79. Malloch, S. N.
    (1999) Mothers and infants and communicative musicality. Musicae scientiae, 3(1_suppl), 29–57. 10.1177/10298649000030S104
    https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649000030S104 [Google Scholar]
  80. Malloch, S., & Trevarthen, C.
    (2009) Communicative musicality. Exploring the basis of human.
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Maurer, D., & Mondloch, C. J.
    (2005) Neonatal synesthesia: A reevaluation. Synesthesia: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience, 193–213.
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Meltzoff, A. N., & Borton, R. W.
    (1979) Intermodal matching by human neonates. Nature, 282(5737), 403–404. 10.1038/282403a0
    https://doi.org/10.1038/282403a0 [Google Scholar]
  83. Moreno-Núñez, A., Rodríguez, C., & Del Olmo, M. J.
    (2017) Rhythmic ostensive gestures: How adults facilitate infants’ entrance into early triadic interactions. Infant Behavior and Development, 491, 168–181. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.09.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2017.09.003 [Google Scholar]
  84. Murray, L., De Pascalis, L., Bozicevic, L., Hawkins, L., Sclafani, V., & Ferrari, P. F.
    (2016) The functional architecture of mother-infant communication, and the development of infant social expressiveness in the first two months. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 1–9. 10.1038/srep39019
    https://doi.org/10.1038/srep39019 [Google Scholar]
  85. Murray, L., and Trevarthen, C.
    (1985) Emotional regulations of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. InT. M. Fieldand, N. A. Fox (Eds.) Social Perception in Infants, Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publishing Corporation, pp.177–197.
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Nomikou, I., Leonardi, G., Radkowska, A., Rączaszek-Leonardi, J., & Rohlfing, K. J.
    (2017) Taking up an active role: emerging participation in early mother–infant interaction during peekaboo routines. Frontiers in psychology, 81, 1656. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01656
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01656 [Google Scholar]
  87. Nomikou, I., Leonardi, G., Rohlfing, K. J., & Rączaszek-Leonardi, J.
    (2016) Constructing interaction: the development of gaze dynamics. Infant and Child Development, 25(3), 277–295. 10.1002/icd.1975
    https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1975 [Google Scholar]
  88. Northrup, J. B., & Iverson, J. M.
    (2020) The development of mother–infant coordination across the first year of life. Developmental Psychology, 56(2), 221–236. 10.1037/dev0000867
    https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000867 [Google Scholar]
  89. Oller, D. K.
    (2000) The emergence of the speech capacity. Psychology Press. 10.4324/9781410602565
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410602565 [Google Scholar]
  90. Papoušek, H. & Papoušek, M.
    (1979) The infant’s fundamental adaptive response system in social interaction. InE. B. Thoman (ed.), Origins of the infant’s social responsiveness (pp.175–208). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Papoušek, M.
    (1995) Origins of reciprocity and mutuality in prelinguistic parent-infant ‘dialogues’. Mutualities in dialogue, 58–81. InMarková, I., Graumann, C. G., Graumann, C. F., & Foppa, K. (Eds.). Mutualities in dialogue. Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  92. Piaget, J.
    (1952) The origins of intelligence in children. International Universities Press, New York. 10.1037/11494‑000
    https://doi.org/10.1037/11494-000 [Google Scholar]
  93. Prechtl, H. F.
    (1974) The behavioural states of the newborn infant (a review). Brain Research, 76(2), 185–212. 10.1016/0006‑8993(74)90454‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0006-8993(74)90454-5 [Google Scholar]
  94. Provenzi, L., Scotto di Minico, G., Giusti, L., Guida, E., & Müller, M.
    (2018) Disentangling the dyadic dance: theoretical, methodological and outcomes systematic review of mother-infant dyadic processes. Frontiers in psychology, 91, 348. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00348
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00348 [Google Scholar]
  95. Reddy, V.
    (2007) Experiencing the social: A second-person approach. Social life and social knowledge: Toward a process account of development. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Reddy, V., Markova, G., & Wallot, S.
    (2013) Anticipatory adjustments to being picked up in infancy. PloS one, 8(6), e65289. 10.1371/journal.pone.0065289
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0065289 [Google Scholar]
  97. Rochat, P. R.
    (2001) Social contingency detection and infant development. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 65(3: Special issue), 347–360. 10.1521/bumc.65.3.347.19847
    https://doi.org/10.1521/bumc.65.3.347.19847 [Google Scholar]
  98. Rochat, P., Neisser, U., & Marian, V.
    (1998) Are young infants sensitive to interpersonal contingency?Infant Behavior and Development, 211, 355–366. 10.1016/S0163‑6383(98)90012‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(98)90012-1 [Google Scholar]
  99. Rochat, P., Querido, J. G., & Striano, T.
    (1999) Emerging sensitivity to the timing and structure of protoconversation in early infancy. Developmental psychology, 35(4), 950. 10.1037/0012‑1649.35.4.950
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.950 [Google Scholar]
  100. Rohlfing, K. J., & Nomikou, I.
    (2014) Intermodal synchrony as a form of maternal responsiveness: association with language development. Language, Interaction and Acquisition, 5(1), 117–136. 10.1075/lia.5.1.06roh
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lia.5.1.06roh [Google Scholar]
  101. Rönnqvist, L., & von Hofsten, C.
    (1994) Neonatal finger and arm movements as determined by a social and an object context. Early Development and Parenting, 3(2), 81–94. 10.1002/edp.2430030205
    https://doi.org/10.1002/edp.2430030205 [Google Scholar]
  102. R Studio Team
    R Studio Team (2020) RStudio: Integrated Development for R. RStudio, PBC, Boston, MAURLwww.rstudio.com/
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Sacks, H., Schegloff, E. A., Jefferson, G.
    (1974) A simplest systematics for the organization of turn-taking for conversation. Language501:696–735. 10.1353/lan.1974.0010
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1974.0010 [Google Scholar]
  104. Sander, L. W.
    (2012) Living systems, evolving consciousness, and the emerging person: A selection of papers from the life work of Louis Sander. Taylor & Francis. 10.4324/9780203837580
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203837580 [Google Scholar]
  105. Scola, C., Bourjade, M., & Jover, M.
    (2015) Social interaction is associated with changes in infants’ motor activity. Socioaffective Neuroscience and Psychology, 51, 28256. 10.3402/snp.v5.28256
    https://doi.org/10.3402/snp.v5.28256 [Google Scholar]
  106. Stern, D. N.
    (1974) Mother and infant at play: The dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal and gaze behaviors. InM. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver (pp.187–213). New York: Wiley.
    [Google Scholar]
  107. (2010) Forms of vitality: Exploring dynamic experience in psychology, the arts, psychotherapy, and development. Oxford University Press. 10.1093/med:psych/9780199586066.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/med:psych/9780199586066.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  108. Stern, D. N., Jaffe, J., Beebe, B., & Bennett, S. L.
    (1975) Vocalizing in unison and in alternation: Two modes of communication within the mother-infant dyad. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 263(1), 89–100. 10.1111/j.1749‑6632.1975.tb41574.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1975.tb41574.x [Google Scholar]
  109. Stern, D. N.
    (1985) Affect attunement. Frontiers of infant psychiatry, 21.
    [Google Scholar]
  110. Stevanovic, M., Himberg, T., Niinisalo, M., Kahri, M., Peräkylä, A., Sams, M., & Hari, R.
    (2017) Sequentiality, mutual visibility, and behavioral matching: Body sway and pitch register during joint decision making. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 1–21. 10.1080/08351813.2017.1262130
    https://doi.org/10.1080/08351813.2017.1262130 [Google Scholar]
  111. Streri, A.
    (1987) Tactile discrimination of shape and intermodal transfer in 2-to 3-month-old infants. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5(3), 213–220. 10.1111/j.2044‑835X.1987.tb01056.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1987.tb01056.x [Google Scholar]
  112. Thelen, E.
    (1981a) Kicking, rocking, and waving: Contextual analysis of rhythmical stereotypies in normal human infants. Animal Behaviour, 29(1), 3–11. 10.1016/S0003‑3472(81)80146‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0003-3472(81)80146-7 [Google Scholar]
  113. (1981a) Rhythmical behavior in infancy: An ethological perspective. Developmental psychology, 17(3), 237. 10.1037/0012‑1649.17.3.237
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.17.3.237 [Google Scholar]
  114. Thelen, E., Kelso, J. S., & Fogel, A.
    (1987) Self-organizing systems and infant motor development. Developmental review, 7(1), 39–65. 10.1016/0273‑2297(87)90004‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(87)90004-9 [Google Scholar]
  115. Thurman, S. L., & Corbetta, D.
    (2017) Spatial exploration and changes in infant–mother dyads around transitions in infant locomotion. Developmental Psychology, 53(7), 1207. 10.1037/dev0000328
    https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000328 [Google Scholar]
  116. Tolston, M., Ariyabuddhiphongs, K., Riley, M. A., & Shockley, K.
    (2014) Cross-recurrence quantification analysis of the influence of coupling constraints on interpersonal coordination and communication. InTranslational Recurrences (pp.157–171). Springer International Publishing. 10.1007/978‑3‑319‑09531‑8_10
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-09531-8_10 [Google Scholar]
  117. Trapletti, A., Hornik, K.
    (2019) Tseries: Time Series Analysis and Computational Finance. R package version 0.10–47.
    [Google Scholar]
  118. Trevarthen, C.
    (1974) Conversations with a two-month-old. New Scientist, 211, 230–235.
    [Google Scholar]
  119. (1977) Descriptive analyses of infant communicative behavior. InH. R. Schaffer (Ed.), Studies in mother–infant interaction (pp.227–270). London: Academic.
    [Google Scholar]
  120. (1979) Communication and cooperation in early infancy: a description of primary intersubjectivity. InM. Bullowa (Ed.) Before Speech: The Beginnings of Human Communication. London: Cambridge University Press, pp.99–136.
    [Google Scholar]
  121. (1985) Facial expressions of emotion in mother-infant interaction. Human neurobiology, 4(1), 21–32.
    [Google Scholar]
  122. (1993) The self-born in intersubjectivity: the psychology of an infant communicating. InU. Neisser (Ed), The Perceived Self: Ecological and Interpersonal Sources of Self-Knowledge. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press, pp.121–173.
    [Google Scholar]
  123. (1998) The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny, 15–46.
    [Google Scholar]
  124. (2009) Human biochronology: on the source and functions of ‘musicality. InR. Haas and V. Brandes (Eds), Music That Works: Contributions of Biology, Neurophysiology, Psychology, Sociology, Medicine and Musicology, Chap. 18. New York: Springer, pp.221–265. 10.1007/978‑3‑211‑75121‑3_16
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-211-75121-3_16 [Google Scholar]
  125. (1999) Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae scientiae, 3(1_suppl), 155–215. 10.1177/10298649000030S109
    https://doi.org/10.1177/10298649000030S109 [Google Scholar]
  126. Tronick, E., Adamson, L. B., Als, H., & Brazelton, T. B.
    (1975, April). Infant emotions in normal and pertubated interactions. Paper presented at thebiennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Denver, CO.
    [Google Scholar]
  127. Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, T. B.
    (1978) The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Journal of the American Academy of Child psychiatry, 17(1), 1–13. 10.1016/S0002‑7138(09)62273‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0002-7138(09)62273-1 [Google Scholar]
  128. Van Egeren, L. A., Barratt, M. S., & Roach, M. A.
    (2001) Mother–infant responsiveness: Timing, mutual regulation, and interactional context. Developmental psychology, 37(5), 684. 10.1037/0012‑1649.37.5.684
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.684 [Google Scholar]
  129. Varela, F., Thompson, E., Rosch, E.
    (1991) The embodied mind: cognitive science and human experience. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. 10.7551/mitpress/6730.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/6730.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  130. Vygostky, L.
    (1934–1962) Thought and language. Language and Thinking. Harmondsworth: Penguin Education.
    [Google Scholar]
  131. Walker, P., Bremner, J. G., Lunghi, M., Dolscheid, S., D. Barba, B., & Simion, F.
    (2018) Newborns are sensitive to the correspondence between auditory pitch and visuospatial elevation. Developmental psychobiology, 60(2), 216–223. 10.1002/dev.21603
    https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.21603 [Google Scholar]
  132. Walker-Andrews, A. S.
    (1986) Intermodal perception of expressive behaviors: Relation of eye and voice?Developmental Psychology, 221,373–377. 10.1037/0012‑1649.22.3.373
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.3.373 [Google Scholar]
  133. (1988) Infants’ perception of the affordances of expressive behaviors. InC. K. Rovee-Collier (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (pp.173–221). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
    [Google Scholar]
  134. Warlaumont, A. S., JRichards, S., Gilkerson, J., Oller, D. K.
    (2014) A social feedback loop for speech development and its reduction in autism. Psychological Science, 251, 1314–1324. 10.1177/0956797614531023
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614531023 [Google Scholar]
  135. Watzlawick, P., Bavelas, J. B., & Jackson, D. D.
    (1967) Pragmatics of Human Communication: A Study of Interactional Patterns, Pathologies, and Paradoxes. W.W.Norton.
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Weinberg, M. K., & Tronick, E. Z.
    (1996) Infant affective reactions to the resumption of maternal interaction after the still-face. Child development, 67(3), 905–914. 10.2307/1131869
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1131869 [Google Scholar]
  137. Wilson, M. & Wilson, T. P.
    (2005) An oscillatory model of the timing of turn taking. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, vol.12(6), 957–968. 10.3758/BF03206432
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206432 [Google Scholar]
  138. Wörmann, V., Holodynski, M., Kärtner, J., & Keller, H.
    (2012) A cross-cultural comparison of the development of the social smile: A longitudinal study of maternal and infant imitation in 6-and 12-week-old infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 35(3), 335–347. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.03.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.03.002 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/is.22041.jov
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/is.22041.jov
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): dyadic interaction; infant; movement; vocalization
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error