1887
Volume 25, Issue 3
  • ISSN 1572-0373
  • E-ISSN: 1572-0381

Abstract

Abstract

This study explores how inter-turn speech pauses influence the perception of cognitive states such as knowledge, confidence, and willingness to grant requests in conversational settings. Longer pauses are typically associated with lower competence and willingness, but Matzinger et al. (2023) discovered that this attribution varies when non-native speakers are involved. They found that listeners were more tolerant of long pauses from non-native than from native speakers when assessing their willingness to grant requests. This may result from the fact that listeners may attribute long pauses to the additional cognitive load non-native speakers face during cognitive processing and response formulation in a second language. This tolerance towards long pauses by non-native speakers did not extend to judgments about non-native speakers’ knowledge and confidence — potentially because knowledge questions are less socially engaging than requests. Here, we replicated and extended Matzinger et al.’s (2023) experiment, which focussed on speakers of Polish, to a cross-cultural context with speakers of Chinese. Our results confirmed that non-native accent mediates perceptions of willingness, but not knowledge or confidence. These findings suggest that inter-turn speech pauses play a nuanced role in cognitive state attribution of native and non-native speakers and that cultural factors minimally influence these perceptions. This may indicate that the mechanisms involved are rooted in evolutionarily fundamental aspects of human social communication and cognition.

Available under the CC BY 4.0 license.
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/is.24044.mat
2025-06-27
2025-07-19
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/is.24044.mat.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1075/is.24044.mat&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

References

  1. Abreu, F. & Pika, S.
    (2022) Turn-taking Skills in Mammals: A Systematic Review Into Development and Acquisition. Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution, 101. 10.3389/fevo.2022.987253
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fevo.2022.987253 [Google Scholar]
  2. Alim, H. S., Rickford, J. R. & Ball, A. F.
    (Eds.) (2016) Raciolinguistics: How Language Shapes Our Ideas about Race. Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190625696.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190625696.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  3. Atkinson, J. M. & Drew, P.
    (1979) Order in Court: The Organization of Verbal Interaction in Juridical Settings. Macmillan. 10.1007/978‑1‑349‑04057‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-04057-5 [Google Scholar]
  4. Baayen, R. H.
    (2008) Analyzing Linguistic Data. Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511801686
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511801686 [Google Scholar]
  5. Badihi, G., Graham, K. E., Grund, C., Safryghin, A., Soldati, A., Donnellan, E., Hashimoto, C., Mine, J. G., Piel, A. K., Stewart, F., Slocombe, K. E., Wilke, C., Townsend, S. W., Zuberbühler, K., Zulberti, C., & Hobaiter, C.
    (2024) Chimpanzee gestural exchanges share temporal structure with human language. Current Biology, 34(14), R673–R674. 10.1016/j.cub.2024.06.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2024.06.009 [Google Scholar]
  6. Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C. & Tily, H. J.
    (2013) Random Effects Structure for Confirmatory Hypothesis Testing: Keep It Maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 681, 255–78. 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 [Google Scholar]
  7. Bartón, K.
    (2018) MuMIn: Multi-Model Inference. R Package Version 1.42.1. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/package=MuMIn (accessed on2 January 2023).
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B. M. & Walker, S. C.
    (2015) Fitting Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using Lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 671, 1–48. 10.18637/jss.v067.i01
    https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 [Google Scholar]
  9. Boduch-Grabka, K. & Lev-Ari, S.
    (2021) Exposing Individuals to Foreign Accent Increases their Trust in What Nonnative Speakers Say. Cognitive Science, 451, e13064. 10.1111/cogs.13064
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13064 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bosker, H. R., Quené, H., Sanders, T. & De Jong, N. H.
    (2014) The perception of fluency in native and nonnative speech. Language Learning, 641, 579–614. 10.1111/lang.12067
    https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12067 [Google Scholar]
  11. Bradlow, A. R., Kim, M., & Blasingame, M.
    (2017) Language-independent talker-specificity in first-language and second-language speech production by bilingual talkers: L1 speaking rate predicts L2 speaking rate. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 141(2), 886. 10.1121/1.4976044
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4976044 [Google Scholar]
  12. Brennan, S. E., & Williams, M.
    (1995) The Feeling of Another′s Knowing: Prosody and Filled Pauses as Cues to Listeners about the Metacognitive States of Speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 341, 383–98. 10.1006/jmla.1995.1017
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1017 [Google Scholar]
  13. Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C.
    (1987) Politeness: Some universals in language usage. Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511813085
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511813085 [Google Scholar]
  14. Boersma, Paul & Weenink, David
    (2022) Praat: doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.3, retrieved fromwww.praat.org/
  15. Carstensen, A., Cao, A., Tan, A. W., Liu, D., Liu, Y., Bui, M. K.,
    (2024) Cognitive diversity in context: US-China differences in children’s reasoning, visual attention, and social cognition. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 461.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. CBOS (Centrum Badania Opinii Publicznej)
    CBOS (Centrum Badania Opinii Publicznej) (2020) Tożsamość narodowa i postrzeganie praw mniejszości narodowych i etnicznych. Warszawa: Fundacja Centrum Badania Opinii Społecznej.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Cenoz, J.
    (2000) Pauses and hesitation phenomena in second language production. ITL: Review of Applied Linguistics, 1271, 53–69. 10.1075/itl.127‑128.03cen
    https://doi.org/10.1075/itl.127-128.03cen [Google Scholar]
  18. Chang, C. B.
    (2012) Rapid and multifaceted effects of second-language learning on first-language speech production. Journal of Phonetics, 40(2), 249–268. 10.1016/j.wocn.2011.10.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.10.007 [Google Scholar]
  19. Chafe, W. L.
    (1980) Some reasons for hesitating. InH. W. Dechert & M. Raupach (Eds.) Temporal Variables in Speech: Studies in Honour of Frieda Goldman-Eisler (pp.169–180). De Gruyter Mouton. 10.1515/9783110816570.169
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110816570.169 [Google Scholar]
  20. Cohen, J.
    (1992) A Power Primer. Psychological Bulletin, 1121, 155–59. 10.1037/0033‑2909.112.1.155
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155 [Google Scholar]
  21. Corriveau, K. H., DiYanni, C. J., Clegg, J. M., Min, G., Chin, J., & Nasrini, J.
    (2017) Cultural differences in the imitation and transmission of inefficient actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1611, 1–18. 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.002 [Google Scholar]
  22. Crown, C. L., & Feldstein, S.
    (1985) Psychological correlates of silence and sound in conversational interaction. InD. Tannen & M. Saville-Troike (Eds.), Perspectives. on silence (pp.31–54). Ablex Publishing.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Coupland, N. & Bishopm, H.
    (2007) Ideologised values for British accents 1. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 111, 74–93. 10.1111/j.1467‑9841.2007.00311.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9841.2007.00311.x [Google Scholar]
  24. Davis, N.
    (2003) God’s Playground A History of Poland: Volume II: 1795 to the Present. Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. De Jong, N. H.
    (2016) Predicting pauses in L1 and L2 speech: The effects of utterance boundaries and word frequency. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 541, 113–32. 10.1515/iral‑2016‑9993
    https://doi.org/10.1515/iral-2016-9993 [Google Scholar]
  26. Dingemanse, M. & Liesenfeld, A.
    (2022) From text to talk: Harnessing conversational corpora for humane and diversity-aware language technology. InProceedings of the 60th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics 1 (Long Papers) (pp.5614–33). Association for Computational Linguistics. 10.18653/v1/2022.acl‑long.385
    https://doi.org/10.18653/v1/2022.acl-long.385 [Google Scholar]
  27. Dobson, A. J.
    (2002) An Introduction to Generalized Linear Models. Chapman and Hall.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Eelen, G.
    (2001) A critique of politeness theory. Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Feng, R.
    (2022) Cognitive Factors Influencing Utterance Fluency in L2 Dialogues: Monadic and Non-monadic Perspectives. Frontiers in Psychology, 131, 926367. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.926367
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.926367 [Google Scholar]
  30. Field, A., Miles, J. & Field, Z.
    (2012) Discovering Statistics Using R. Los Angeles: SAGE.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Finger, H., Goeke, C., Diekamp, D., Standvoß, K. & König, P.
    (2017) LabVanced: A unified JavaScript framework for online studies. In International Conference on Computational Social Science (Cologne) (pp.1–3). University of Osnabrück.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Fox Tree, J. E.
    (2002) Interpreting pauses and ums at turn exchanges. Discourse Processes, 341, 37–55. 10.1207/S15326950DP3401_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326950DP3401_2 [Google Scholar]
  33. Gleitman, L. R., January, D., Nappa, R. & Trueswell, J. C.
    (2007) On the give and take between event apprehension and utterance formulation. Journal of Memory and Language, 571, 544–69. 10.1016/j.jml.2007.01.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.01.007 [Google Scholar]
  34. Gluszek, A., & Dovidio, J. F.
    (2010) Speaking with a nonnative accent: Perceptions of bias, communication difficulties, and belonging in the United States. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 291, 224–34. 10.1177/0261927X09359590
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X09359590 [Google Scholar]
  35. Goffman, E.
    (1963) Embarrassment and Social Organization. InN. J. Smelser & W. T. Smelser (Eds.), Personality and social systems (pp.541–548). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.. 10.1037/11302‑050
    https://doi.org/10.1037/11302-050 [Google Scholar]
  36. Goupil, L., Ponsot, E., Richardson, D., Reyes, G., & Aucouturier, J.-J.
    (2021) Listeners’ perceptions of the certainty and honesty of a speaker are associated with a common prosodic signature. Nature Communications, 121, 1–17. 10.1038/s41467‑020‑20649‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-20649-4 [Google Scholar]
  37. Griffin, Z. M. & Bock, K.
    (2000) What the eyes say about speaking. Psychological Science, 111, 274–79. 10.1111/1467‑9280.00255
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00255 [Google Scholar]
  38. Gu, Y.
    (1990) Politeness phenomena in modern Chinese. Journal of Pragmatics, 14(2), 237–257. 10.1016/0378‑2166(90)90082‑O
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0378-2166(90)90082-O [Google Scholar]
  39. Guyer, J. J., Fabrigar, L. R. & Vaughan-Johnston, T. I.
    (2019) Speech rate, intonation, and pitch: Investigating the bias and cue effects of vocal confidence on persuasion. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 451,389–405. 10.1177/0146167218787805
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218787805 [Google Scholar]
  40. Hanzlíková, D., & Skarnitzl, R.
    (2017) Credibility of native and non-native speakers of English revisited: Do non-native listeners feel the same?Research in Language, 151, 285–98. 10.1515/rela‑2017‑0016
    https://doi.org/10.1515/rela-2017-0016 [Google Scholar]
  41. Harumi, S., & King, J.
    (2020) East Asian Perspectives on Silence in English Language Education: An Introduction. InJ. King & S. Harumi (Eds.) East Asian Perspectives on Silence in English Language Education (pp.1–16). Multilingual Matters. 10.2307/jj.22730507.8
    https://doi.org/10.2307/jj.22730507.8 [Google Scholar]
  42. Heldner, M. & Edlund, J.
    (2010) Pauses, gaps and overlaps in conversations. Journal of Phonetics, 381, 555–68. 10.1016/j.wocn.2010.08.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2010.08.002 [Google Scholar]
  43. Huang, L. F., & Gráf, T.
    (2020) Speech Rate and Pausing in English: Comparing Learners at Different Levels of Proficiency with Native Speakers. Taiwan Journal of TESOL, 17(1), 57–86.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Ji, L. J., Peng, K., & Nisbett, R. E.
    (2000) Culture, control, and perception of relationships in the environment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(5), 943–955. 10.1037/0022‑3514.78.5.943
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.943 [Google Scholar]
  45. Ji, L. J., Nisbett, R. E., & Su, Y.
    (2001) Culture, change, and prediction. Psychological Science, 12(6), 450–456. 10.1111/1467‑9280.00384
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00384 [Google Scholar]
  46. Johnson, P. C. D.
    (2014) Extension of Nakagawa & Schielzeth’s R2GLMM to random slopes models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution51, 44–946. 10.1111/2041‑210X.12225
    https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12225 [Google Scholar]
  47. Kendrick, K. H., & Torreira, F.
    (2015) The timing and construction of preference: A quantitative study. Discourse Processe, 521, 255–89. 10.1080/0163853X.2014.955997
    https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2014.955997 [Google Scholar]
  48. Kitayama, S., & Salvador, C. E.
    (2024) Cultural psychology: Beyond east and west. Annual Review of Psychology, 75(1), 495–526. 10.1146/annurev‑psych‑021723‑063333
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-021723-063333 [Google Scholar]
  49. Koo, M., Choi, J. A., & Choi, I.
    (2018) Analytic versus holistic cognition: Constructs and measurement. InJ. Spencer-Rodgers & K. Peng (Eds.), The psychological and cultural foundations of East Asian cognition: Contradiction, change, and holism (pp.105–134). Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B.
    (2017) lmerTest Package: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. Journal of Statistical Software, 821, 1–26. 10.18637/jss.v082.i13
    https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13 [Google Scholar]
  51. Lehtonen, J. & Sajavaara, K.
    (1985) The Silent Finn. InD. Tannen & M. Saville-Troike (Eds.) Perspectives on Silence (pp193–201). Ablex Publishing.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Lev-Ari, S. & Keysar, B.
    (2010) Why don’t we believe non-native speakers? The influence of accent on credibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 461, 1093–96. 10.1016/j.jesp.2010.05.025
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.05.025 [Google Scholar]
  53. Levelt, W. M.
    (1999) Producing spoken language: A blueprint of the speaker. InC. M. Brown & P. Hagoort (Eds.) The Neurocognition of Language (pp.83–122). Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Levinson, S. C.
    (1983) Pragmatics. Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511813313
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511813313 [Google Scholar]
  55. Levinson, S.
    (2006) Cognition at the Heart of Human Interaction. Discourse Studies, 8 (1): 85–93. 10.1177/1461445606059557
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1461445606059557 [Google Scholar]
  56. Levinson, S. C.
    (2016) Turn-taking in Human Communication–Origins and Implications for Language Processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences201: 6–14. 10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.10.010 [Google Scholar]
  57. Levinson, S. C., & Torreira, F.
    (2015) Timing in turn-taking and its implications for processing models of language. Frontiers in Psychology, 61, 731. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00731
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00731 [Google Scholar]
  58. Levy, B. J., McVeigh, N. D., Marful, A., & Anderson, M. C.
    (2007) Inhibiting Your Native Language. Psychological Science, 18(1), 29–34. 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.2007.01844.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01844.x [Google Scholar]
  59. Linck, J. A., Kroll, J. F., & Sunderman, G.
    (2009) Losing Access to the Native Language While Immersed in a Second Language: Evidence for the Role of Inhibition in Second-Language Learning. Psychological Science, 20(12), 1507–1515. 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.2009.02480.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02480.x [Google Scholar]
  60. Lippi-Green, R.
    (2012) English with an Accent: Language, Ideology, and Discrimination in the United States. Routledge. 10.4324/9780203348802
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203348802 [Google Scholar]
  61. Liu, P., Rigoulot, S., & Pell, M. D.
    (2017) Cultural immersion alters emotion perception: Neurophysiological evidence from Chinese immigrants to Canada. Social Neuroscience, 12(6), 685–700. 10.1080/17470919.2016.1231713
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1231713 [Google Scholar]
  62. Liu, P., Rigoulot, S., & Pell, M. D.
    (2016) Cultural immersion alters emotion perception: Neurophysiological evidence from Chinese immigrants to Canada. Social Neuroscience, 12(6), 685–700. 10.1080/17470919.2016.1231713
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1231713 [Google Scholar]
  63. Lundholm-Fors, K.
    (2015) Production and Perception of Pauses in Speech. [Doctoral dissertation, University of Gothenburg], Gothenburg University Publications Electronic Archive. hdl.handle.net/2077/39346
  64. Matzinger, T., Ritt, N. & Fitch, W. T.
    (2020) Non-native speaker pause patterns closely correspond to those of native speakers at different speech rates. PLoS ONE, 151: e0230710. 10.1371/journal.pone.0230710
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230710 [Google Scholar]
  65. Matzinger, T., Pleyer, M., & Żywiczyński, P.
    (2023) Pause Length and Differences in Cognitive State Attribution in Native and Non-Native Speakers. Languages, 8(1), 26. 10.3390/languages8010026
    https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8010026 [Google Scholar]
  66. Morris, M. W., & Peng, K.
    (1994) Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 949–971. 10.1037/0022‑3514.67.6.949
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.949 [Google Scholar]
  67. Nakagawa, S. & Schielzeth, H.
    (2013) A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 41, 133–42. 10.1111/j.2041‑210x.2012.00261.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2041-210x.2012.00261.x [Google Scholar]
  68. Nakagawa, S., Johnson, P. C. D. & Schielzeth, H.
    (2017) The coefficient of determination R2 and intra-class correlation coefficient from generalized linear mixed-effects models revisited and expanded. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 141, 20170213. 10.1098/rsif.2017.0213
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2017.0213 [Google Scholar]
  69. Nakane, I.
    (2007) Silence in Intercultural Communication. John Benjamins. 10.1075/pbns.166
    https://doi.org/10.1075/pbns.166 [Google Scholar]
  70. (2012) Silence. InC. B. Paulston, S. F. Kiesling, & E. S. Rangel (Eds.), The Handbook of Intercultural Discourse and Communication (pp.158–179). Blackwell. 10.1002/9781118247273.ch9
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118247273.ch9 [Google Scholar]
  71. Nisbett, R.
    (2003) The Geography of Thought: How Asians and Westerners Think Differently... and why. Simon and Schuster.
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Peltonen, P.
    (2017) Temporal fluency and problem-solving in interaction: An exploratory study of fluency resources in L2 dialogue. System, 701, 1–13. 10.1016/j.system.2017.08.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.system.2017.08.009 [Google Scholar]
  73. Pika, S., Wilkinson, R., Kendrick, K. H. & Vernes, S. C.
    (2018) Taking turns: bridging the gap between human and animal communication. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 2851, 28520180598.
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Powell, M. J.
    (2009) The BOBYQA algorithm for bound constrained optimization without derivatives. Cambridge NA Report NA2009/06, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, 261, 26–46.
    [Google Scholar]
  75. R Development Core Team
    R Development Core Team (2018) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
    [Google Scholar]
  76. Ramjattan, V. A.
    (2022) Accenting racism in labour migration. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 421, 87–92. 10.1017/S0267190521000143
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190521000143 [Google Scholar]
  77. Raupach, M.
    (1980) Temporal variables in first and second language speech and perception of fluency. InH. W. Dechert & M. Raupach (Eds.) Temporal Variables in Speech: Studies in Honour of Frieda Goldman-Eisler (pp.263–70). De Gruyter Mouton. 10.1515/9783110816570.263
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110816570.263 [Google Scholar]
  78. Reisman, J. M., & Yamokoski, T.
    (1974) Psychotherapy and Friendship: An Analysis of the Communications of Friends. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 21 (4), 269–73. 10.1037/h0036621
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0036621 [Google Scholar]
  79. Roberts, F. & Francis, A. L.
    (2013) Identifying a temporal threshold of tolerance for silent gaps after requests. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 1331, EL471–EL477. 10.1121/1.4802900
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4802900 [Google Scholar]
  80. Roberts, F., Margutti, P. & Takano, S.
    (2011) Judgments concerning the valence of inter-turn silence across speakers of American English, Italian, and Japanese. Discourse Processes, 481, 331–54. 10.1080/0163853X.2011.558002
    https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2011.558002 [Google Scholar]
  81. Roberts, S. G., Torreira, F., and Levinson, S. C.
    (2015) The effects of processing and sequence organization on the timing of turn taking: a corpus study. Frontiers in Psychology, 61, 509. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00509
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00509 [Google Scholar]
  82. Roessel, J., Schoel, C. & Stahlberg, D.
    (2020) Modern notions of accent-ism: Findings, conceptualizations, and implications for interventions and research on nonnative accents. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 391, 87–111. 10.1177/0261927X19884619
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X19884619 [Google Scholar]
  83. R Development Core Team
    R Development Core Team (2023) R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Sacks, H.
    (1992) Lectures on Conversations. Oxford: Blackwell, vols. I and II1.
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Sacks, H. & Schegloff, E.
    (1973) Opening up Closings. Semiotica, 71, 289–327.
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Sbranna, S., Cangemi, F. & Grice, M.
    (2020) Quantifying L2 interactional competence. InL. Romito (Ed.) La variazione linguistica in condizioni di contatto: contesti acquisizionali, lingue, dialetti e minoranze in italia e nel mondo [Language change under contact conditions: acquisitional contexts, languages, dialects and minorities in Italy and around the world] (pp.383–405). Milan: Officinaventuno. 10.17469/O2107AISV000018. https://www.aisv.it/StudiAISV/2020/vol_7/018_SbrannaCangemiGrice.pdf
    https://doi.org/10.17469/O2107AISV000018 [Google Scholar]
  87. Sbranna, S.
    (2023) Developing linguistic competence in second language learning: Fluency and prosody in Italian learners of German. Unpublished PhD Thesis, University of Cologne. https://osf.io/9ca6m/
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Schwartz, S.
    (2008) Cultural value orientations: Nature & implications of national differences. Psychology. Journal of Higher School of Economics, 5(2), 37–67.
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Schielzeth, H. & Forstmeier, W.
    (2009) Conclusions beyond support: Overconfident estimates in Mixed Models. Behavioral Ecology, 201, 416–20. 10.1093/beheco/arn145
    https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arn145 [Google Scholar]
  90. Shigemitsu, Y.
    (2005) Different interpretations of pauses in natural conversation--Japanese, Chinese and Americans. 東京工芸大学工学部紀要. 人文・社会編, 28(2), 8–14.
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Stivers, T.
    (2010) An overview of the question–response system in American English conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 421, 2772–81. 10.1016/j.pragma.2010.04.011
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2010.04.011 [Google Scholar]
  92. Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., Englert, E., Hayashi, M., Heinemann, T., Hoymann, G., Rossano, F., de Ruiter, J. P., Yoon, K.-E.,
    (2009) Universals and cultural variation in turn-taking in conversation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1061, 10587–92. 10.1073/pnas.0903616106
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0903616106 [Google Scholar]
  93. Suzuki, S., Kormos, J. & Uchihara, T.
    (2021) The relationship between utterance and perceived fluency: A meta-analysis of correlational studies. The Modern Language Journal, 1051, 435–63. 10.1111/modl.12706
    https://doi.org/10.1111/modl.12706 [Google Scholar]
  94. Tannen, D.
    (1985) Silence: Anything but. InD. Tannen and M. Saville-Troike (Eds.) Perspectives on Silence (pp.93–11). Ablex.
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Tavakoli, P.
    (2011) Pausing patterns: Differences between L2 learners and native speakers. ELT Journal, 651, 71–79. 10.1093/elt/ccq020
    https://doi.org/10.1093/elt/ccq020 [Google Scholar]
  96. Timming, A. R.
    (2017) The effect of foreign accent on employability: A study of the aural dimensions of aesthetic labour in customer-facing and non-customer-facing jobs. Work, Employment and Society, 311, 409–28. 10.1177/0950017016630260
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0950017016630260 [Google Scholar]
  97. Trouvain, J., Fauth, C. & Möbius, B.
    (2016) Breath and Non-breath Pauses in Fluent and Disfluent Phases of German and French L1 and L2 Read Speech. Speech Prosody (SP8), 311, 31–35. 10.21437/SpeechProsody.2016‑7
    https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2016-7 [Google Scholar]
  98. Tsalikis, J., DeShields, O. W. Jr., & LaTour, M. S.
    (1991) The role of accent on the credibility and effectiveness of the salesperson. Journal of Personal Selling and Sales Management, 111, 31–41. 10.1080/08853134.1991.10753857
    https://doi.org/10.1080/08853134.1991.10753857 [Google Scholar]
  99. Tychmanowicz, A., Filipiak, S. & Sprynska, Z.
    Extravert individualists or introvert collectivists? Personality traits and individualism and collectivism in students in Poland and Ukraine. Curr Psychol401, 5947–5957 (2021) 10.1007/s12144‑019‑00480‑x
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-019-00480-x [Google Scholar]
  100. van Hell, J. G., & Dijkstra, T.
    (2002) Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 780–789. 10.3758/BF03196335
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196335 [Google Scholar]
  101. Van Maastricht, L., Zee, T., Krahmer, E., & Swerts, M.
    (2021) The interplay of prosodic cues in the L2: How intonation, rhythm, and speech rate in speech by Spanish learners of Dutch contribute to L1 Dutch perceptions of accentedness and comprehensibility. Speech Communication, 1331, 81–90. 10.1016/j.specom.2020.04.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2020.04.003 [Google Scholar]
  102. Van Os, M., De Jong, N. H., & Bosker, H. R.
    (2020) Fluency in dialogue: Turn-taking behavior shapes perceived fluency in native and nonnative speech. Language Learning, 701, 1183–217. 10.1111/lang.12416
    https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12416 [Google Scholar]
  103. Verga, L., Kotz, S. A., & Ravignani, A.
    (2023) The Evolution of Social Timing. Physics of Life Reviews, 461, 131–51. 10.1016/j.plrev.2023.06.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2023.06.006 [Google Scholar]
  104. Verouden, N. W. & van der Sanden, M. C. A.
    (2018) Silence in intercultural collaboration: A Sino-Dutch research centre. Advances in Applied Sociology, 81, 125–151. 10.4236/aasoci.2018.82008
    https://doi.org/10.4236/aasoci.2018.82008 [Google Scholar]
  105. Walker, Michael B., & Trimboli, C.
    1982 Smooth transitions in conversational interactions. The Journal of Social Psychology1171: 305–6. 10.1080/00224545.1982.9713444
    https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1982.9713444 [Google Scholar]
  106. Wardęga, J.
    (2017) Chińczycy w Polsce–niewidoczna mniejszość. Studia Migracyjne-Przegla ̨d Polonijny, 21, 121–41.
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Weilhammer, K. & Rabold, S.
    (2003) Durational aspects in turn taking. Proceedings of the international conference of phonetic sciences, 2145–2148.
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Wootton, A. J.
    (1981) The Management of Grantings and Rejections by Parents in Request Sequences. Semiotica, 371, 59–90. 10.1515/semi.1981.37.1‑2.59
    https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1981.37.1-2.59 [Google Scholar]
  109. Yamada, H.
    (1997) Different Games, Different Rules: Why American and Japanese Misunderstand Each Other. Oxford University Press. 10.1093/oso/9780195094886.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780195094886.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  110. Żywiczyński, P., Gontier, N. & Wacewicz, S.
    (2017) The Evolution of (Proto-)Language: Focus on Mechanisms. Language Sciences, 63 (September), 1–11. 10.1016/j.langsci.2017.06.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2017.06.004 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/is.24044.mat
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/is.24044.mat
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error