1887
Volume 1, Issue 1
  • ISSN 2542-3835
  • E-ISSN: 2542-3843
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

This study investigates whether highly-proficient Vietnamese-English bilinguals can activate associations in a native-like way from studying L2 words, whether these L2 associations interact with L1 words, and whether this depends on when they started learning L2 English. The results suggest that early bilinguals have native-like L2 English associative networks and these networks are interconnected with L1 Vietnamese words. Late bilinguals, in contrast, seem to indicate that their L2 English associative networks might be activated less automatically, and they are not so strongly connected with L1 words. This pattern of results are discussed in terms of how age of L2 exposure might affect the development of L2 associative networks.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/jsls.17020.huy
2018-05-07
2025-04-19
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Altarriba, J. , & Basnight-Brown, D. M.
    (2009) An overview of semantic processing in bilinguals: Methods and findings. In A. Pavlenko (Ed.), The bilingual mental lexicon: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp.79–99). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Anastasi, J. S. , Rhodes, M. G. , Marquez, S. , & Velino, V.
    (2005) The incidence of false memories in native and non-native speakers. Memory, 13, 815–828. doi: 10.1080/09658210444000421
    https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210444000421 [Google Scholar]
  3. Basnight-Brown, D. M. , & Altarriba, J.
    (2007) Differences in semantic and translation priming across languages: The role of language direction and language dominance. Memory and Cognition, 35, 953–967. doi: 10.3758/BF03193468
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193468 [Google Scholar]
  4. Birdsong, D. , & Molis, M.
    (2001) On the evidence for maturational constraints in second-language acquisition. Journal of Language and Memory, 44, 235–249. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2750
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2750 [Google Scholar]
  5. Cabeza, R. , & Lennartson, E. R.
    (2005) False memory across languages: Implicit associative response vs fuzzy trace views. Memory, 13, 1–5. doi: 10.1080/09658210344000161
    https://doi.org/10.1080/09658210344000161 [Google Scholar]
  6. Collins, A. M. , & Loftus, E. F.
    (1975) A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407–428. doi: 10.1037/0033‑295X.82.6.407
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407 [Google Scholar]
  7. Coltheart, M.
    (1980) Iconic memory and visible persistence. Perception & Psychophysics, 27, 183–228. doi: 10.3758/BF03204258
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03204258 [Google Scholar]
  8. De Groot, A. M. B.
    (1992) Determinants of word translation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 1001–1018.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. De Groot, A. M. B. , & Nas, G. L.
    (1991) Lexical representation of cognates and noncognates in compound bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 90–123. doi: 10.1016/0749‑596X(91)90012‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90012-9 [Google Scholar]
  10. Dehaene, S. , Naccache, L. , Cohen, L. , Le Bihan, D. , Mangin, J. F. , Poline, J. B. , & Rivière, D.
    (2001) Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nature Neuroscience, 4, 752–758. doi: 10.1038/89551
    https://doi.org/10.1038/89551 [Google Scholar]
  11. De Wit, B. , & Kinoshita, S.
    (2015) The masked semantic effect is task dependent: Reconsidering the automatic spreading activation process. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41, 1062–1075.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Dimitropoulou, M. , Duñabeitia, J. A. , & Carreiras, M.
    (2011) Two words, one meaning: Evidence of automatic co-activation of translation equivalents. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00188
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00188 [Google Scholar]
  13. Ferré, P. , Sánchez-Casas, R. , & Guasch, M.
    (2006) Can a horse be a donkey? Semantic and form interference effects in translation recognition in early and late proficient and nonproficient Spanish-Catalan bilinguals. Language Learning, 56(4), 571–608. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9922.2006.00389.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2006.00389.x [Google Scholar]
  14. Finkbeiner, M.
    (2005) Task-dependent L2-L1 translation priming: An investigation of the separate memory systems account. In J. Cohen , K. McAlister , K. Rolstad , & J. MacSwan (Eds.), ISB4: Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism (pp. 741–750). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Finkbeiner, M. , Forster, K. , Nicol, J. , & Nakamura, K.
    (2004) The role of polysemy in masked semantic and translation priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 1–22. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.01.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.01.004 [Google Scholar]
  16. Flege, J. E. , Yeni-Komshian, G. H. , & Liu, S.
    (1999) Age constraints on second-language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 78–104. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1999.2638
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1999.2638 [Google Scholar]
  17. Fodor, J. A.
    (1983) The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. Cambridge, MA: The MIT press.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Forster, K. I.
    (1998) The pros and cons of masked priming. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27, 203–233. doi: 10.1023/A:1023202116609
    https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023202116609 [Google Scholar]
  19. Forster, K. I. , & Davis, C.
    (1984) Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 680–698.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Forster, K. I. , & Forster, J. C.
    (2003) DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 116–124. doi: 10.3758/BF03195503
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195503 [Google Scholar]
  21. Forster, K. I. , Mohan, K. , & Hector, J.
    (2003) The mechanics of masked priming. In S. Kinoshita & S. Lupker (Eds.), Masked priming: The state of the art (pp.3–37). Hove: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Francis, W.
    (2005) Bilingual semantic and conceptual representation. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp.251–267). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Francis, W. S. , & Gutiérrez, M.
    (2012) Bilingual recognition memory: Stronger performance but weaker levels-of-processing effects in the less fluent language. Memory & Cognition, 40, 496–503. doi: 10.3758/s13421‑011‑0163‑3
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-011-0163-3 [Google Scholar]
  24. Francis, W. S. , & Strobach, N.
    (2013) The bilingual L2 advantage in recognition memory. Psychonomics Bulletin & Review, 20, 1296–1303. doi: 10.3758/s13423‑013‑0427‑y
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0427-y [Google Scholar]
  25. Frenck-Mestre, C. , & Prince, P.
    (1997) Second language autonomy. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 481–501. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1997.2526
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2526 [Google Scholar]
  26. Gallo, D. A. , & Roediger III, H. L.
    (2002) Variability among word lists in eliciting memory illusions: Evidence for associative association and monitoring. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 469–497. doi: 10.1016/S0749‑596X(02)00013‑X
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00013-X [Google Scholar]
  27. Gollan, T. H. , Forster, K. I. , & Frost, R.
    (1997) Translation priming with different scripts: Masked priming with cognates and noncognates in Hebrew-English bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Learning Memory and Cognition, 23, 1122–1139. doi: 10.1037/0278‑7393.23.5.1122
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.23.5.1122 [Google Scholar]
  28. Gomez, P. , Perea, M. , & Ratcliff, R.
    (2013) A diffusion model of masked versus unmasked priming: Are they qualitatively different?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39, 1731–1740.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Grainger, J. , & Beauvillain, C.
    (1988) Associative priming in bilinguals: Some limits of interlingual facilitation effects. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 42, 261–273. doi: 10.1037/h0084193
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084193 [Google Scholar]
  30. Graves, D. F. , & Altarriba, J.
    (2014) False memories in bilingual speakers. In R. R. Heredia & J. Altarriba (Eds.), Foundations of bilingual memory (pp.205–221). New York, NY: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978‑1‑4614‑9218‑4_10
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-9218-4_10 [Google Scholar]
  31. Griffiths, T. L. , Steyvers, M. , & Tennenbaum, J. B.
    (2007) Topics in semantic representation. Psychological Review, 114, 211–244. doi: 10.1037/0033‑295X.114.2.211
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.114.2.211 [Google Scholar]
  32. Howe, M. L.
    (2007) Children’s emotional false memories. Research Report, 18, 856–860.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. (2011) Nature of early memory: An adaptive theory of the genesis and development of memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195381412.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195381412.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  34. Howe, M. L. , Gagnon, N. , & Thouas, L.
    (2008) Development of false memories in bilingual children and adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 669–681. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.09.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.09.001 [Google Scholar]
  35. Hutchison, K. A.
    (2003) Is semantic priming due to association strength or feature overlap? A microanalytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 785–813. doi: 10.3758/BF03196544
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196544 [Google Scholar]
  36. Jiang, N.
    (1999) Testing processing explanations for the asymmetry in masked cross-language priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 59–75. doi: 10.1017/S1366728999000152
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728999000152 [Google Scholar]
  37. Jiang, N. , & Forster, K.
    (2001) Cross-language priming asymmetries in lexical decision tasks and episodic recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 32–51. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2737
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2737 [Google Scholar]
  38. Johnson, J. S. , & Newport, E. L.
    (1989) Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of English as a second language. Cognitive Psychology, 21, 60–99. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0285(89)90003‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(89)90003-0 [Google Scholar]
  39. Kawasaki-Miyaji, Y. , Inoue, T. , & Yama, H.
    (2003) Cross-linguistic false recognition: How do Japanese-dominant bilinguals process two languages: Japanese and English?Psychologia, 46, 255–267. doi: 10.2117/psysoc.2003.255
    https://doi.org/10.2117/psysoc.2003.255 [Google Scholar]
  40. Keatley, C. W. , Spinks, J. A. , & de Gelder, B.
    (1994) Asymmetrical cross-language priming effects. Memory & Cognition, 22, 70–84. doi: 10.3758/BF03202763
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202763 [Google Scholar]
  41. Kim, J. , & Davis, C.
    (2003) Task effects in masked cross-script translation and phonological priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 484–499. doi: 10.1016/S0749‑596X(03)00093‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00093-7 [Google Scholar]
  42. Kotz, S. A. , & Elston-Güttler, K.
    (2004) The role of proficiency on processing categorical and associative information in the L2 as revealed by reaction times and event-related brain potentials. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 17, 215–235. doi: 10.1016/S0911‑6044(03)00058‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0911-6044(03)00058-7 [Google Scholar]
  43. Kroll, J. F. , & Stewart, E.
    (1994) Category interference in translation and picture naming: Evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 149–174. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1994.1008
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1994.1008 [Google Scholar]
  44. Lee, Y. , Chiang, W. , Hung, H.
    (2008) Lexical association and false memory for words in two cultures. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 37, 49–58. doi: 10.1007/s10936‑007‑9058‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-007-9058-7 [Google Scholar]
  45. Lucas, M.
    (2000) Semantic priming without association: A meta-analytic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 618–630. doi: 10.3758/BF03212999
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03212999 [Google Scholar]
  46. Marmolejo, G. , Diliberto-Macaluso, K. A. , & Altarriba, J.
    (2009) False memory in bilinguals: Does switching languages increase false memories?American Journal of Psychology, 122, 1–16.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Masson, M. E. J.
    (1995) A distributed memory model of semantic priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 21, 3–23.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. McNamara, T. P.
    (1992) Priming and constraints it places on theories of memory and retrieval. Psychological Review, 99, 650–662. doi: 10.1037/0033‑295X.99.4.650
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.99.4.650 [Google Scholar]
  49. Moss, H. E. , Hare, M. L. , Day, P. , & Tyler, L. K.
    (1994) A distributed memory model of the associative boost in semantic priming. Connection Science, 6, 413–427. doi: 10.1080/09540099408915732
    https://doi.org/10.1080/09540099408915732 [Google Scholar]
  50. Moss, H. E. , Ostrin, R. K. , Tyler, L. K. , & Marslen-Wilson, W. D.
    (1995) Accessing different types of lexical semantic information: Evidence from priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 863–883.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Neely, J. H.
    (1991) Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp.264–289). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Paradis, M.
    (1997) The cognitive neuropsychology of bilingualism. In A. M. B. de Groot & J. F. Kroll (Eds.), Tutorials in bilingualism: Psycholinguistic perspectives (pp.331–354). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Perea, M. , Duñabeitia, J. A. , & Carreiras, M.
    (2008) Masked associative/semantic priming effects across languages with highly proficient bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 916–930. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2008.01.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2008.01.003 [Google Scholar]
  54. Perea, M. , & Gotor, A.
    (1997) Associative and semantic priming effects occur at very short stimulus-onset asynchronies in lexical decision and naming. Cognition, 62, 223–240. doi: 10.1016/S0010‑0277(96)00782‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00782-2 [Google Scholar]
  55. Perea, M. , & Rosa, E.
    (2002) The effects of associative and semantic priming in the lexical decision task. Psychological research, 66(3), 180–194. doi: 10.1007/s00426‑002‑0086‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-002-0086-5 [Google Scholar]
  56. Roediger III, H. , & McDermott, K.
    (1995) Creating false memories: Remembering words not presented in lists. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 803–814.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Sabourin, L. , Brien, C. , & Burkholder, M.
    (2014) The effect of age of L2 acquisition on the organization of the bilingual lexicon: Evidence from masked priming. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17, 542–555. doi: 10.1017/S1366728913000643
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000643 [Google Scholar]
  58. Sahlin, B. H. , Harding, M. C. , & Seamon, J. G.
    (2005) When do false memories cross language boundaries in Spanish-English bilinguals?Memory & Cognition, 33, 1414–1421. doi: 10.3758/BF03193374
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193374 [Google Scholar]
  59. Schoonbaert, S. , Duyck, W. , Brysbaert, M. , & Hartsuiker, R. J.
    (2009) Semantic and translation priming from a first language to a second and back: Making sense of the findings. Memory & Cognition, 37, 569–586. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.5.569
    https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.37.5.569 [Google Scholar]
  60. Sereno, J. A.
    (1991) Graphemic, associative, and syntactic priming effects at a brief stimulus onset asynchrony in lexical decision and naming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 459–477.
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Sperling, G.
    (1960) The information available in brief visual presentations [Monograph]. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 74(11, Whole No.498), 1–29. doi: 10.1037/h0093759
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093759 [Google Scholar]
  62. Stadler, M. , Roediger III, H. , & McDermott, K.
    (1999) Norms for word lists that create false memories. Memory & Cognition, 27, 494–500. doi: 10.3758/BF03211543
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211543 [Google Scholar]
  63. Sunderman, G.
    (2011) Conceptual mediation in second language learners. In P. Trofimovich & K. McDonough (Eds.), Applying priming methods to L2 learning, teaching and research: Insights from Psycholinguistics (pp.219–237). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/lllt.30.15sun
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lllt.30.15sun [Google Scholar]
  64. Sunderman, G. , & Kroll, J. F.
    (2006) First language activation during second language lexical processing: An investigation of lexical form, meaning, and grammatical class. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 387–422. doi: 10.1017/S0272263106060177
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263106060177 [Google Scholar]
  65. Talamas, A. , Kroll, J. F. , & Dufour, R.
    (1999) From form to meaning: Stages in the acquisition of second-language vocabulary. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2, 45–58. doi: 10.1017/S1366728999000140
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728999000140 [Google Scholar]
  66. Thompson-Schill, S. L. , Kurtz, K. J. , & Gabrieli, J. D. E.
    (1998) Effects of semantic and associative relatedness on automatic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 440–458. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1997.2559
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2559 [Google Scholar]
  67. Van Hell, J. G. , & De Groot, A. M. B.
    (1998) Conceptual representation in bilingual memory: Effects of concreteness and cognate status in word association. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 193–211. doi: 10.1017/S1366728998000352
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728998000352 [Google Scholar]
  68. (2008) Sentence context modulates visual word recognition and translation in bilinguals. Acta Psychologica, 128, 431–451. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.03.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.03.010 [Google Scholar]
  69. Wang, X. , & Forster, K. I.
    (2010) Masked translation priming with semantic categorization: Testing the Sense Model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 327–340. doi: 10.1017/S1366728909990502
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990502 [Google Scholar]
  70. Williams, J. N.
    (1996) Is automatic priming semantic?European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 8, 113–161. doi: 10.1080/095414496383121
    https://doi.org/10.1080/095414496383121 [Google Scholar]
  71. Witzel, N. O. & Forster, K. I.
    (2012) How L2 words are stored: The episodic L2 hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1608–1621.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/jsls.17020.huy
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/jsls.17020.huy
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): DRM lists; early and late bilinguals; masked priming; speeded old-new task
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error