1887
Volume 6, Issue 1-2
  • ISSN 1879-9264
  • E-ISSN: 1879-9272
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

We investigated age-related decline of bilingual language control. Thirteen older and 13 younger bilinguals performed a verbal fluency task (completing the same letter and semantic categories in each language and switching languages after every category), and a non-linguistic flanker task. In letter fluency, bilinguals produced fewer correct responses after switching languages, suggesting inhibition of the previously-used language. However, this testing-order effect did not differ between groups and older bilinguals produced few wrong-language intrusions, implying intact ability to apply inhibition in older age. In contrast, age-related deficits in the flanker task were robust, implying dissociations between language control and domain-general executive control. In semantic fluency, there were no testing-order effects but older bilinguals produced more intrusions than younger bilinguals, and more intrusions than in letter fluency. Thus, bilinguals may flexibly modulate the degree of inhibition when they can benefit from semantic priming between languages, but less efficiently so in older age.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lab.15003.iva
2016-02-19
2025-02-11
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Axelrod, B.N. , & Henry, R.R.
    (1992) Age-related performance on the Wisconsin card sorting, similarities, and controlled oral word association tests. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 6, 16–26. doi: 10.1080/13854049208404113
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13854049208404113 [Google Scholar]
  2. Bialystok, E. , Craik, F.I.M. , & Luk, G.
    (2008) Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 34, 859–873.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Bialystok, E. , Craik, F.I.M. , Klein, R. & Viswanathan, M.
    (2004) Bilingualism, aging and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. doi: 10.1037/0882‑7974.19.2.290
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290 [Google Scholar]
  4. Bolla, K. I. , Gray, S. , Resnick, S. M. , Galante, R. , & Kawas, C.
    (1998) Category and letter fluency in highly educated older adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 12, 330–338. doi: 10.1076/clin.12.3.330.1986
    https://doi.org/10.1076/clin.12.3.330.1986 [Google Scholar]
  5. Boone, K.B.
    (1999) Neuropsychological assessment of executive functions: Impact of age, education, gender, intellectual level, and vascular status on executive test scores. In B.L. Miller & J.L. Cummings (Eds.), The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders. The science and practice of neuropsychology series (pp.247–260). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Bopp, K.L. , Verhaeghen, P.
    (2005) Aging and verbal memory span: A meta-analysis. Journal of Gerontology, 60B, 223–233. doi: 10.1093/geronb/60.5.P223
    https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/60.5.P223 [Google Scholar]
  7. Brickman, A.M. , Paul, R.H. , Cohen, R.A. , Williams, L.M. , MacGregor, K.L. , Jefferson, A.L. , et al.
    (2005) Category and letter verbal fluency across the adult lifespan: Relationship to EEG theta power. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 561–573. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2004.12.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2004.12.006 [Google Scholar]
  8. Calabria, M. , Branzi, F.M. , Marne, P. , Hernández, M. , & Costa, A.
    (2015) Age-related effects over bilingual language control and executive control. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 65–78. doi: 10.1017/S1366728913000138
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000138 [Google Scholar]
  9. Calabria, M. , Hernández, M. , Branzi, F.M. , & Costa, A.
    (2011) Qualitative differences between bilingual language control and executive control: Evidence from task-switching. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, Article 399.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Cohen, J. , MacWhinney, B. , Flatt, M. , & Provost, J.
    (1993) PsyScope: An interactive graphical system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers. Behavioral Research Methods, Instrumentation and Computation, 25, 257–271. doi: 10.3758/BF03204507
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03204507 [Google Scholar]
  11. Costa, A. , Hernández, M. , & Sebastian-Galles, N.
    (2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 59–86. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013 [Google Scholar]
  12. Costa, A. , & Santesteban, M.
    (2004) Lexical access in bilingual speech production: Evidence from language switching in highly proficient bilinguals and L2 learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 491–511. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.002 [Google Scholar]
  13. Costa, A. , Hernández, M. , Costa-Faidella, J. , & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
    (2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don‘t. Cognition, 113, 135–149. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001 [Google Scholar]
  14. Crossley, M. , D'Arcy, C. , & Rawson, N. S.
    (1997) Letter and category fluency in community-dwelling Canadian seniors: A comparison of normal participants to those with dementia of the Alzheimer or vascular type. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 19, 52–62. doi: 10.1080/01688639708403836
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01688639708403836 [Google Scholar]
  15. Delis, D.C. , Kaplan, E. , & Kramer, J.H.
    (2001) Delis-Kaplan executive functioning system. San Antonio, TX: Psychology Corporation.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Fan, J. , McCandliss, B.D. , Sommer, T. , Raz, A. , & Posner, M.I.
    (2002) Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 340–347. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886
    https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361886 [Google Scholar]
  17. Folstein, M.F. , Folstein, S.E. , & McHugh, P.R.
    (1975) Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022‑3956(75)90026‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 [Google Scholar]
  18. Gollan, T.H. , Ferreira, V.S.
    (2009) Should I stay or should I switch? A cost–benefit analysis of voluntary language switching in young and aging bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 640–665.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Gollan, T.H. , Kleinman, D. , & Wierenga, C.E.
    (2014) What’s easier: Doing what you want, or being told what to do? Cued versus voluntary language and task switching. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143, 2167–2195. doi: 10.1037/a0038006
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038006 [Google Scholar]
  20. Gollan, T.H. , Montoya, R.I. , Werner, G.
    (2002) Semantic and letter fluency in Spanish-English bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 16, 562–576. doi: 10.1037/0894‑4105.16.4.562
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.16.4.562 [Google Scholar]
  21. Gollan, T.H. , Sandoval, T. , & Salmon, D.P.
    (2011) Cross-language intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the link between executive control and language selection. Psychological Science, 22, 1155–1164. doi: 10.1177/0956797611417002
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417002 [Google Scholar]
  22. Gollan, T.H. , Schotter, E.R. , Gomez, J. , Murillo, M. , & Rayner, K.
    (2014) Multiple levels of bilingual language control: Evidence from language intrusions in reading aloud. Psychological Science, 25, 585–595. doi: 10.1177/0956797613512661
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613512661 [Google Scholar]
  23. Gollan, T.H. , Weissberger, G. , Runnqvist, E. , Montoya, R.I. , & Cera, C.M.
    (2012) Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A multi-lingual naming test (MINT) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish-English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15, 594–615. doi: 10.1017/S1366728911000332
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000332 [Google Scholar]
  24. Goral, M. , Campanelli, L. , & Spiro III, A.
    (2015) Language dominance and inhibition abilities in bilingual older adults. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 79–89. doi: 10.1017/S1366728913000126
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000126 [Google Scholar]
  25. Green, D. W.
    (1998) Mental control of the bilingual lexicosemantic system. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 67–81. doi: 10.1017/S1366728998000133
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728998000133 [Google Scholar]
  26. Hasher, L. & Zacks, R.T.
    (1988) Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 22, 193–225. doi: 10.1016/S0079‑7421(08)60041‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60041-9 [Google Scholar]
  27. Henry, J. D. , Crawford, J. R. , & Phillips, L. H.
    (2004) Verbal fluency performance in dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: A meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia, 42, 1212–1222. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.02.001 [Google Scholar]
  28. Hernandez, A. E. & Kohnert, K. J.
    (1999) Aging and language switching in bilinguals. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 6, 69–83. doi: 10.1076/anec.6.2.69.783
    https://doi.org/10.1076/anec.6.2.69.783 [Google Scholar]
  29. Hernández, M. , Martin, C.D. , Barceló, F. , & Costa, A.
    (2013) Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching?Journal of Memory and Language, 69, 257–276. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.004 [Google Scholar]
  30. Kleinman, D. & Gollan, T.H.
    (submitted). Speaking two languages for the price of one: Bypassing language control mechanisms via accessibility-driven switches.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Kozora, E. , Cullum, C.M.
    (1995) Generative naming in normal aging: Total output and qualitative changes using phonemic and semantic constraints. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 9, 313–320. doi: 10.1080/13854049508400495
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13854049508400495 [Google Scholar]
  32. Ljungberg, J.K. , Hansson, P. , Andrés, P. , Josefsson, M. , & Nilsson, L.-G.
    (2013) A longitudinal study of memory advantages in bilinguals. PLoS ONE, 8, e73029. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0073029
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0073029 [Google Scholar]
  33. Luo, L. , Luk, G. , & Bialystok, E.
    (2010) Effect of language proficiency and executive control on verbal fluency performance in bilinguals. Cognition, 114, 29–41. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.014
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.014 [Google Scholar]
  34. Martin, A. , Wiggs, C.L. , Lalonde, F. , & Mack, C
    (1994) Word retrieval to letter and semantic cues:A double dissociations in normal subjects using interference tasks. Neuropsychologia, 32, 1487–1492. doi: 10.1016/0028‑3932(94)90120‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(94)90120-1 [Google Scholar]
  35. Mattis, S.
    (1988) Dementia rating scale: Professional manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Meuter, R. F. , & Allport, A.
    (1999) Bilingual language switching in naming: Asymmetrical costs of language selection. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 25–40. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1998.2602
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2602 [Google Scholar]
  37. Myerson, J. , Hale, S. , Wagstaff, D. , Poon, L.W. , & Smith, G.A.
    (1990) The information-loss model: A mathematical theory of age-related cognitive slowing. Psychological Review, 97, 475–487. doi: 10.1037/0033‑295X.97.4.475
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.4.475 [Google Scholar]
  38. Paap, K.R. , & Greenberg, Z.I.
    (2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  39. Portocarrero, J.S. , Burright, R.G. , & Donovick. P.J.
    (2007) Vocabulary and verbal fluency of bilingual and monolingual college students. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 415–422. doi: 10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.015
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2007.01.015 [Google Scholar]
  40. Poulisse, N.
    (1999) Slips of the tongue: Speech errors in first and second language production. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/sibil.20
    https://doi.org/10.1075/sibil.20 [Google Scholar]
  41. Prior, A. & Gollan, T.H.
    (2011) Good language-switchers are good task-switchers: Evidence from Spanish–English and Mandarin–English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 17, 682–691. doi: 10.1017/S1355617711000580
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711000580 [Google Scholar]
  42. Prior, A. , & Gollan, T.H.
    (2013) The elusive link between language control and executive control: A case of limited transfer, The Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 622–645. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.821993
    https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.821993 [Google Scholar]
  43. Rascovsky, K. , Salmon, D. P. , Hansen, L. A. , Thal, L. J. , & Galasko, D.
    (2007) Disparate letter and semantic category fluency deficits in autopsy-confirmed frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer's disease. Neuropsychology, 21, 20–30. doi: 10.1037/0894‑4105.21.1.20
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.21.1.20 [Google Scholar]
  44. Reitan, RM.
    (1958) Validity of the Trail Making Test to organic brain damage. Perceptual Motor Skills, 8, 271–276. doi: 10.2466/PMS.8.7.271‑276
    https://doi.org/10.2466/PMS.8.7.271-276 [Google Scholar]
  45. Rosselli, M. , Ardila, A. , Araujo, K. , Weekes, V.A. , Caracciolo, V. , Padilla, M. , et al.
    (2000) Verbal fluency and repetition skills in healthy older Spanish–English bilinguals. Applied Neuropsychology, 7, 1–24. doi: 10.1207/S15324826AN0701_3
    https://doi.org/10.1207/S15324826AN0701_3 [Google Scholar]
  46. Salthouse, T.A.
    (1985) A theory of cognitive aging. Amsterdam: North Holland.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Sandoval, T.
    (2010) The role of control in bilingual verbal fluency: Evidence from aging and Alzheimer’s Disease. Unpublished doctoral dissertation.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Sandoval, T.C. , Gollan, T.H. , Ferreira, V.S. , & Salmon, D.P.
    (2010) What causes the bilingual disadvantage in verbal fluency: The dual-task analogy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13, 231–252. doi: 10.1017/S1366728909990514
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909990514 [Google Scholar]
  49. Simon, J.R. , & Ruddell, A.P.
    (1967) Auditory S-R compatibility: The effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. Journal of Applied Psychology, 51, 300–304. doi: 10.1037/h0020586
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0020586 [Google Scholar]
  50. Strauss, E. , Sherman, E.M.S. , & Spreen, O.
    (2006) A compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (Third Ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Tao, L , Taft, M. & Gollan, T.H.
    (2015) The bilingual switching advantage: Sometimes related to bilingual proficiency, sometimes not. The Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 21, 531–544. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000521
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617715000521 [Google Scholar]
  52. Tombaugh, T. N. , Kozak, J. , & Rees, L.
    (1999) Normative data stratified by age and education for two measures of verbal fluency: FAS and animal naming. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 14, 167–177.
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Tomer, R. , Levin, B. E.
    (1993) Differential effects of aging on two verbal fluency tasks. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 76, 465–466. doi: 10.2466/pms.1993.76.2.465
    https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1993.76.2.465 [Google Scholar]
  54. Van Assche, E. , Duyck, W. , & Gollan, T.H.
    (2013) Whole-language and item-specific control in bilingual language production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1781–1792.
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Van der Lubbe, R. & Verleger, R.
    (2002) Aging and the Simon task. Psychophysiology, 39, 100–110. doi: 10.1111/1469‑8986.3910100
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3910100 [Google Scholar]
  56. Van Hell, J. G. , & Dijkstra, T.
    (2002) Foreign language knowledge can influence native language performance in exclusively native contexts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 780–789. doi: 10.3758/BF03196335
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196335 [Google Scholar]
  57. Verhaeghen, P.
    (2011) Aging and executive control: Reports of a demise greatly exaggerated. Psychological Science, 20, 174–180.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Verreyt, N. , Woumans, E. , Vandelanotte, D. , Szmalec, A. , & Duyck, W.
    (2016) The influence of language switching experience on the bilingual executive control advantage. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19, 181-190. doi: 10.1017/S1366728914000352
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000352 [Google Scholar]
  59. Wasylyshyn, C. , Verhaeghen, P. , & Sliwinski, M.
    (2011) Aging and task switching. A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 26, 15–20. doi: 10.1037/a0020912
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020912 [Google Scholar]
  60. Wechsler, D.
    (1997) Wechsler adult intelligence scale (Third Ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Weissberger, G. H. , Wierenga, C. E. , Bondi, M. W. , & Gollan, T. H.
    (2012) Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology and Aging, 27, 959–974. doi: 10.1037/a0028281
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028281 [Google Scholar]
  62. Zacks, R.T. , & Hasher, L.
    (1994) Directed ignoring: Inhibitory regulation of working memory. In D. Dagenbach & T.H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory Processes in Attention, Memory, and Language (pp.241–264). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.15003.iva
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.15003.iva
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error