1887
Volume 7, Issue 3-4
  • ISSN 1879-9264
  • E-ISSN: 1879-9272
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Language tests often fail to diagnose specific language impairment (SLI) in bilinguals due to the confounding impact of SLI and bilingualism on language. The present study focuses on the contribution of executive functioning to the diagnosis of SLI in bilinguals. Performance of monolingual and bilingual children with and without SLI on a task tapping response inhibition was assessed. Results revealed a negative effect of SLI but no effect of bilingualism. The diagnostic accuracy of the task was also estimated. Sensitivity and specificity were low, suggesting that response inhibition could not be considered as a reliable clinical marker of SLI. Although the results at the group level showed that children with SLI seemed more at risk than their typically-developing peers for a deficit in response inhibition, our results on diagnostic accuracy challenge the hypothesis of a relationship between deficits in language and deficits in inhibition in children with SLI.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lab.15020.lal
2017-03-07
2024-10-13
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association
    American Psychiatric Association (1994) Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4 th ed. (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Baddeley, A. D.
    (2003) Working memory and language: An overview. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(3), 189–208. doi: 10.1016/S0021‑9924(03)00019‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00019-4 [Google Scholar]
  3. Baddeley, A. D. , & Hitch, G.
    (1974) Working memory. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 8, 47–89. doi: 10.1016/S0079‑7421(08)60452‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-7421(08)60452-1 [Google Scholar]
  4. Bialystok, E.
    (1999) Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70(3), 636–644. doi: 10.1111/1467‑8624.00046
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00046 [Google Scholar]
  5. (2001) Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511605963
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511605963 [Google Scholar]
  6. (2006) Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 60(1), 68. doi: 10.1037/cjep2006008
    https://doi.org/10.1037/cjep2006008 [Google Scholar]
  7. Bialystok, E. , Craik, F. I. , Grady, C. , Chau, W. , Ishii, R. , Gunji, A. , & Pantev, C.
    (2005) Effect of bilingualism on cognitive control in the Simon task: Evidence from MEG. NeuroImage, 24(1), 40–49. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.044
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.044 [Google Scholar]
  8. Bialystok, E. , Craik, F. I. , Klein, R. , & Viswanathan, M.
    (2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and aging, 19(2), 290. doi: 10.1037/0882‑7974.19.2.290
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290 [Google Scholar]
  9. Bialystok, E. , & Viswanathan, M.
    (2009) Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112(3), 494–500. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bishop, D. V. M.
    (1992) The underlying nature of specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33(1), 3–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1469‑7610.1992.tb00858.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb00858.x [Google Scholar]
  11. (1994) Grammatical errors in specific language impairment: Competence or performance limitations?. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15(4), 507–550. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400006895
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400006895 [Google Scholar]
  12. Bishop, D. V. M. , & Norbury, C. F.
    (2005) Executive functions in children with communication impairments, in relation to autistic symptomatology I: Generativity. Autism, 9(1), 7–27. doi: 10.1177/1362361305049027
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361305049027 [Google Scholar]
  13. Bishop, D. V. M.
    (2014) Ten questions about terminology for children with unexplained language problems. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 49(4), 381–415. doi: 10.1111/1460‑6984.12101
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12101 [Google Scholar]
  14. Blasi, G. , Goldberg, T. E. , Weickert, T. , Das, S. , Kohn, P. , Zoltick, B. , Bertolino, A. , Callicott, J.H. , Weinberger, D.R. , & Mattay, V. S.
    (2006) Brain regions underlying response inhibition and interference monitoring and suppression. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23(6), 1658–1664. doi: 10.1111/j.1460‑9568.2006.04680.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2006.04680.x [Google Scholar]
  15. Bonifacci, P. , Giombini, L. , Bellocchi, S. , & Contento, S.
    (2011) Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental Science, 14(2), 256–269. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2010.00974.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00974.x [Google Scholar]
  16. Bunge, S. A. , Dudukovic, N. M. , Thomason, M. E. , Vaidya, C. J. , & Gabrieli, J. D.
    (2002) Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: Evidence from fMRI. Neuron, 33(2), 301–311. doi: 10.1016/S0896‑6273(01)00583‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00583-9 [Google Scholar]
  17. Cardy, J. E. O. , Tannock, R. , Johnson, A. M. , & Johnson, C. J.
    (2010) The contribution of processing impairments to SLI: Insights from attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43(2), 77–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.09.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.09.003 [Google Scholar]
  18. Carlson, S. M. , & Meltzoff, A. N.
    (2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282–298. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2008.00675.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x [Google Scholar]
  19. Chapman, R. S.
    (1997) Language development in children and adolescents with Down syndrome. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 3(4), 307–312. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098‑2779(1997)3:4<307::AID‑MRDD5>3.0.CO;2‑K
    https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2779(1997)3:4<307::AID-MRDD5>3.0.CO;2-K [Google Scholar]
  20. Chevrie-Muller, C. , & Plaza, M.
    (2001) N-EEL: Nouvelles épreuves pour l’examen du langage [New tasks for the assemsent of language]. Paris: Éditions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée (ECPA).
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Conti‐Ramsden, G. , Botting, N. , & Faragher, B.
    (2001) Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741–748. doi: 10.1111/1469‑7610.00770
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00770 [Google Scholar]
  22. Costa, A. , Hernández, M. , & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
    (2008) Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106(1), 59–86. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013 [Google Scholar]
  23. Crago, M. , & Paradis, J.
    (2003) Two of a kind? Commonalities and variation in languages and language learners. In Y. Levy & J. Schaeffer (Eds.), Language competence across populations: Towards a definition of specific language impairment (pp.97–110). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Crutchley, A.
    (1999) Bilingual children with SLI attending language units: getting the bigger picture. Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 15(3), 201–217. doi: 10.1191/026565999672131522
    https://doi.org/10.1191/026565999672131522 [Google Scholar]
  25. Diamond, A.
    (2002) Normal development of prefrontal cortex from birth to young adulthood: Cognitive functions, anatomy, and biochemistry. In D. T. Stuss & R. T. Knight (Eds.), Principals of frontal lobe function (pp.466–503). London: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195134971.003.0029
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195134971.003.0029 [Google Scholar]
  26. De Bruin, A. , Treccani, B. , & Della Sala, S.
    (2014) Cognitive advantage in bilingualism: An example of publication bias?Psychological Science, 26(1), 99–107. doi: 10.1177/0956797614557866
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614557866 [Google Scholar]
  27. Deltour, J. J.
    (1992) Test de closure grammaticale (TCG) [Test of grammatical closure (TCG)]. Liège: Presse Universitaire de Liège.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Deltour, J. J. , & Hupkens, D.
    (1980) TVAP, Test de vocabulaire actif et passif [TVAP, Test of active and passive vocabulary]. Paris: Editions EAP.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Dollaghan, C. , & Campbell, T. F.
    (1998) Nonword repetition and child language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41(5), 1136–1146. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4105.1136 [Google Scholar]
  30. Dunn, L. M. , Thériault-Whalen, C. M. , & Dunn, L. M.
    (1993) Echelle de vocabulaire en images peabody: Adaptation française du Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test [Scale of vocabulary using peabody pictures : French adaptation of the peabody picture vocabulary test]. Toronto, Canada: Psycan.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Elin Thordardottir, E. T.
    (2015) Proposed diagnostic procedures for use in bilingual and cross-linguistic contexts. In S. Armon-Lotem , J. de Jong , & N. Meir (Eds.), Methods for assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from Specific Language Impairment (pp.331–358). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Elin Thordardottir, E. T. , & Brandeker, M.
    (2013) The effect of bilingual exposure versus language impairment on nonword repetition and sentence imitation scores. Journal of Communication Disorders, 46(1), 1–16. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2012.08.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2012.08.002 [Google Scholar]
  33. Engel de Abreu, P. M. E. , Cruz-Santos, A. , Tourinho, C. J. , Martin, R. , & Bialystok, E.
    (2012) Bilingualism enriches the poor enhanced cognitive control in low-income minority children. Psychological Science, 23(11), 1364–1371. doi: 10.1177/0956797612443836
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612443836 [Google Scholar]
  34. Engel de Abreu, P.M.E. , Cruz-Santos, A. , & Puglisi, M.
    (2014) Specific language impairment in language‐minority children from low‐income families. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 49(6), 736–747. doi: 10.1111/1460‑6984.12107
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12107 [Google Scholar]
  35. Epstein, B. , Shafer, V. L. , Melara, R. D. , & Schwartz, R. G.
    (2014) Can children with SLI detect cognitive conflict? Behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(4), 1453–1467. doi: 10.1044/2014_JSLHR‑L‑13‑0234
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-L-13-0234 [Google Scholar]
  36. Friedman, N. P. , & Miyake, A.
    (2004) The relations among inhibition and interference control functions: a latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(1), 101. doi: 10.1037/0096‑3445.133.1.101
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.1.101 [Google Scholar]
  37. Greenslade, K. J. , Plante, E. , & Vance, R.
    (2009) The diagnostic accuracy and construct validity of the Structured Photographic Expressive Language Test Preschool. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 40(2), 150–160. doi: 10.1044/0161‑1461(2008/07‑0049)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008/07-0049) [Google Scholar]
  38. Grüter, T.
    (2005) Comprehension and production of French object clitics by child second language learners and children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26(3), 363–91. doi: 10.1017/S0142716405050216
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716405050216 [Google Scholar]
  39. Henry, L. A. , Messer, D. J. , & Nash, G.
    (2012) Executive functioning in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53(1), 37–45. doi: 10.1111/j.1469‑7610.2011.02430.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02430.x [Google Scholar]
  40. Hoff, E.
    (2006) How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002 [Google Scholar]
  41. Hoffman, L. M. , & Gillam, R. B.
    (2004) Verbal and spatial information processing constraints in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47(1), 114–125. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2004/011)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/011) [Google Scholar]
  42. Huizinga, M. , Dolan, C. V. , & Van der Molen, M. W.
    (2006) Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2017–2036. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010 [Google Scholar]
  43. Iluz-Cohen, P. , & Armon-Lotem, S.
    (2013) Language proficiency and executive control in bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16(4), 884–899. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000788
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000788 [Google Scholar]
  44. Im-Bolter, N. , Johnson J. , and Pascual Leone, J.
    (2006) Processing limitations in children with SLI: The role of executive function. Child Development, 77(6), 1822–1841. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2006.00976.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00976.x [Google Scholar]
  45. Jensen de Lopez, K. M. , & Baker, A. E.
    (2015) Executive functions in the assessment of bilingual children with language impairment. In S. Armon-Lotem , J. de Jong , & N. Meir (Eds.), Methods for assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from Specific Language Impairment (pp.277–297). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Joanisse, M. F. , & Seidenberg, M. S.
    (1998) Specific language impairment: A deficit in grammar or processing?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(7), 240–247. doi: 10.1016/S1364‑6613(98)01186‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01186-3 [Google Scholar]
  47. (2003) Phonology and syntax in specific language impairment: Evidence from a connectionist model. Brain and Language, 86(1), 40–56. doi: 10.1016/S0093‑934X(02)00533‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(02)00533-3 [Google Scholar]
  48. Kail, R.
    (1994) A method for studying the generalized slowing hypothesis in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 37(2), 418–421. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3702.418
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3702.418 [Google Scholar]
  49. Kapa, L. L. , & Plante, E.
    (2015) Executive Function in SLI: Recent Advances and Future Directions. Current Developmental Disorders Reports, 2(3), 245–252. doi: 10.1007/s40474‑015‑0050‑x
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s40474-015-0050-x [Google Scholar]
  50. Kaufman, A. S. , & Kaufman, N. L.
    (2004) K-ABC-II. Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children. Second edition. Manual. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Khomsi, A.
    (2000) Évaluation du langage oral: ELO [Assessment of spoken language : ELO]. Paris: Éditions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée (ECPA).
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Klenberg, L. , Korkman, M. , & Lahti-Nuuttila, P.
    (2001) Differential development of attention and executive functions in 3- to 12-year-old Finnish children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 20(1), 407–428. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2001_6
    https://doi.org/10.1207/S15326942DN2001_6 [Google Scholar]
  53. Kohnert, K.
    (2010) Bilingual children with primary language impairment: Issues, evidence and implications for clinical actions. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43(6), 456–473. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.02.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2010.02.002 [Google Scholar]
  54. Kohnert, K. , Windsor, J. , & Yim, D.
    (2006) Do language-based processing tasks separate children with language impairment from typical bilinguals?Learning Disabilities: Research & Practice, 21(1), 19–29.
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Kindlon, D. , Mezzacappa, E. , & Earls, F.
    (1995) Psychometric properties of impulsivity measures: Temporal stability, validity and factor structure. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 645–661. doi: 10.1111/j.1469‑7610.1995.tb02319.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1995.tb02319.x [Google Scholar]
  56. Kroll, J. F. , & Dijkstra, A.
    (2002) The bilingual lexicon. In R. Kaplan (Ed.), Handbook of Applied Linguistics (pp.301–321). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Lakshmanan, U.
    (2012) Child second language acquisition. In P. Robinson (Ed.), The Routledge Encyclopedia of Second Language Acquisition (pp.71–75). London: Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Lecocq, P.
    (1996) L'ÉCOSSE, une épreuve de compréhension syntaxico-sémantique : Adaptation française du Test of Reception of Grammar (TROG) [The ECOSSE, a task of syntactic and semantic understanding : French adaptation of the Test of Reception of Grammar (TROG)]. Paris: Presses Universitaires Septentrion.
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Leonard, L. B.
    (2014) Children with specific language impairment. Second edition. Cambridge: MIT press.
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Logan, G. D. , Schachar, R. J. , & Tannock, R.
    (1997) Impulsivity and inhibitory control. Psychological Science, 8(1), 60–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.1997.tb00545.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00545.x [Google Scholar]
  61. Luk, G. , Anderson, J. A. , Craik, F. I. , Grady, C. , & Bialystok, E.
    (2010) Distinct neural correlates for two types of inhibition in bilinguals: Response inhibition versus interference suppression. Brain and Cognition, 74(3), 347–357. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2010.09.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2010.09.004 [Google Scholar]
  62. Martin-Rhee, M. M. , & Bialystok, E.
    (2008) The development of two types of inhibitory control in monolingual and bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 11(1), 81–93. doi: 10.1017/S1366728907003227
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728907003227 [Google Scholar]
  63. Marton, K. , Kelmenson, L. , & Pinkhasova, M.
    (2007) Inhibition control and working memory capacity in children with SLI. Psikhologyah: ketav'et mada'i Yisre'eli le-'iyun ule-mehkar, 50(2), 110.
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Marton, K. , Campanelli, L. , Scheuer, J. , Yoon, J. , & Eichorn, N.
    (2012) Executive function profiles in children with and without specific language impairment. Rivista di psicolinguistica applicata, 12(3), 57.
    [Google Scholar]
  65. McGregor, K. K. , Newman, R. M. , Reilly, R. M. , & Capone, N. C.
    (2002) Semantic representation and naming in children with speci c language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language & Hearing Research, 45(5), 998–1014. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2002/081)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2002/081) [Google Scholar]
  66. Messer, D. , & Dockrell, J. E.
    (2006) Children’s naming and word- finding difficulties: Descriptions and explanations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49(2), 309–324. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2006/025)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/025) [Google Scholar]
  67. Messer, M. H. , Leseman, P. P. , Boom, J. , & Mayo, A. Y.
    (2010) Phonotactic probability effect in nonword recall and its relationship with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 105(4), 306–323. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2009.12.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.12.006 [Google Scholar]
  68. Mezzacappa, E.
    (2004) Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: Developmental properties and sociodemographic correlates in an epidemiological sample of young, urban children. Child Development, 75(5), 1373–1386. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2004.00746.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00746.x [Google Scholar]
  69. Miles, J. , & Shevlin, M.
    (2001) Applying regression and correlation: A guide for students and researchers. London: Sage.
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Miyake, A. , & Friedman, N. P.
    (2012) The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411429458 [Google Scholar]
  71. Miyake, A. , Friedman, N. P. , Emerson, M. J. , Witzki, A. H. , Howerter, A. , & Wager, T. D.
    (2000) The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
    https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734 [Google Scholar]
  72. Montgomery, J. W.
    (2000) Verbal working memory and sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(2), 293–308. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4302.293
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4302.293 [Google Scholar]
  73. Montgomery, J. W. , Magimairaj, B. M. , & Finney, M. C.
    (2010) Working memory and specific language impairment: An update on the relation and perspectives on assessment and treatment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology: 19(1), 78–94. doi: 10.1044/1058‑0360(2009/09‑0028)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2009/09-0028) [Google Scholar]
  74. Nigg, J. T.
    (2000) On inhibition/disinhibition in developmental psychopathology: views from cognitive and personality psychology and a working inhibition taxonomy. Psychological bulletin, 126(2), 220. doi: 10.1037/0033‑2909.126.2.220
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.220 [Google Scholar]
  75. Noterdaeme, M. , Amorosa, H. , Mildenberger, K. , Sitter, S. , & Minow, F.
    (2001) Evaluation of attention problems in children with autism and children with a specific language disorder. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 10(1), 58–66. doi: 10.1007/s007870170048
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s007870170048 [Google Scholar]
  76. Paradis, J.
    (2004) The relevance of specific language impairment in understanding the role of transfer in second language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(1), 67–82. doi: 10.1017/S0142716404001043
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001043 [Google Scholar]
  77. (2011) Individual differences in child English second language acquisition: Comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(3), 213–237. doi: 10.1075/lab.1.3.01par
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.3.01par [Google Scholar]
  78. Paradis, J. , & Crago, M.
    (2000) Tense and temporality: Similarities and differences between language-impaired and second-language children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43(4), 834–848. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4304.834
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4304.834 [Google Scholar]
  79. (2004) Comparing L2 and SLI grammars in child French: Focus on DP. In P. Prévost & J. Paradis (Eds.), The acquisition of French in different contexts: Focus on functional categories (pp.89–107). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/lald.32.06par
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.32.06par [Google Scholar]
  80. Paradis, J. , Rice, M. L. , Crago, M. , & Marquis, J.
    (2008) The acquisition of tense in English: Distinguishing child second language from first language and specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29(4), 689–722. doi: 10.1017/S0142716408080296
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716408080296 [Google Scholar]
  81. Pearson, B. Z. , Fernandez, S. C. , Lewedeg, V. , & Oller, D. K.
    (1997) The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18(1), 41–58. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400009863
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400009863 [Google Scholar]
  82. Pennington, B. F. , & Ozonoff, S.
    (1996) Executive functions and developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37(1), 51–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1469‑7610.1996.tb01380.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x [Google Scholar]
  83. Pierart, B. , Comblain, A. , Grégoire, J. , Mousty, P. , & Noël, M. P.
    (1997) ISADYLE: Instrument pour le screening et l'examen approfondi des dysfonctionnements du langage chez l'enfant [ISADYLE : Instrument for the screening and the in-depth assesment of child language dysfunctions]. InMeeting annuel de la Société Belge de Psychologie.
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Plante, E. , & Vance, R.
    (1994) Selection of preschool language tests: A data-based approach. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 25(1), 15–24. doi: 10.1044/0161‑1461.2501.15
    https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461.2501.15 [Google Scholar]
  85. Raven, J. C. , Court, J. H. , & Raven, J.
    (1985) Raven’s progressive matrices. A manual for Raven’s Progressive Matrices and Vocabulary Scales. London: H. K. Lewis.
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Roello, M. , Ferretti, M. L. , Colonnello, V. , & Levi, G.
    (2015) When words lead to solutions: Executive function deficits in preschool children with specific language impairment. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 37, 216–222. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.11.017
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.11.017 [Google Scholar]
  87. Rowe, J. , Lavender, A. , & Turk, V.
    (2006) Cognitive executive function in Down’s syndrome. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45(1), 5–17. doi: 10.1348/014466505X29594
    https://doi.org/10.1348/014466505X29594 [Google Scholar]
  88. Rice, M. L. , & Wexler, K.
    (1996) A phenotype of specific language impairment: Extended optional infinitives. In M. L. Rice (Ed.), Toward a genetics of language (pp.215–237). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Russell, J. , Jarrold, C. , & Hood, B.
    (1999) Two intact executive capacities in children with autism: Implications for the core executive dysfunctions in the disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29(2), 103–112. doi: 10.1023/A:1023084425406
    https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023084425406 [Google Scholar]
  90. Schwartz, R. G.
    (2009) Specific language impairment. In R.G. Schwartz (Ed.), Handbook of Child Language Disorders (pp.3–43). New York: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Spaulding, T. J.
    (2010) Investigating mechanisms of suppression in preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(3), 725–738. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2009/09‑0041)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/09-0041) [Google Scholar]
  92. Tannock, R. , & Schachar, R.
    (1996) Executive dysfunction as an underlying mechanism of behavior and language problems in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. In J. H. Beitchman , N. J. Cohen , M. M. Konstantareas & R. Tannock (Eds.), Language, learning and behavioral disorders. Developmental, biological and clinical perspectives (pp.128–155). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Theakston, A. L. , Lieven, E. V. , & Tomasello, M.
    (2003) The role of the input in the acquisition of third person singular verbs in English. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46(4), 863–877. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2003/067)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/067) [Google Scholar]
  94. Thorn, A. S. C. , & Gathercole, S.E.
    (1999) Language-specific knowledge and short-term memory in bilingual and non-bilingual children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52(2), 303–324. doi: 10.1080/713755823
    https://doi.org/10.1080/713755823 [Google Scholar]
  95. Tomblin, J. B. , Records, N. L. , Buckwalter, P. , Zhang, X. , Smith, E. , & O’Brien, M.
    (1997) Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40(6), 1245–1260. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4006.1245
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4006.1245 [Google Scholar]
  96. Vugs, B. , Hendriks, M. , Cuperus, J. , & Verhoeven, L.
    (2014) Working memory performance and executive function behaviors in young children with SLI. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(1), 62–74. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2013.10.022
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2013.10.022 [Google Scholar]
  97. Wechsler, D.
    (1989) Wechsler preschool and primary scales of intelligence. New York, NY: The Psychological Corporation.
  98. Wilson, S. P. , & Kipp, K.
    (1998) The development of efficient inhibition: Evidence from directed-forgetting tasks. Developmental Review, 18(1), 86–123. doi: 10.1006/drev.1997.0445
    https://doi.org/10.1006/drev.1997.0445 [Google Scholar]
  99. Windsor, J. , Kohnert, K. , Lobitz, K. F. , Pham, G. T.
    (2010) Cross-language non-word repetition by bilingual and monolingual children. American Journal of Speech- Language Pathology, 19(4), 298–310. doi: 10.1044/1058‑0360(2010/09‑0064)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2010/09-0064) [Google Scholar]
  100. World Health Organization
    World Health Organization (1993) The ICD-10 classification for mental and behavioral disorders: Diagnostic criteria for research. Geneva: WHO.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.15020.lal
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.15020.lal
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): bilingualism; Executive functioning; SLI
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error