1887
Volume 9, Issue 2
  • ISSN 1879-9264
  • E-ISSN: 1879-9272
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

The aim of this experimental study is to examine the development of Greek gender in bilingual English-Greek and German-Greek children. Four gender production tasks were designed, two targeting gender assignment eliciting determiners and two targeting gender agreement eliciting predicate adjectives for real and novel nouns. Participant performance was assessed in relation to whether the ‘other’ language was a gender language or not (English vs. German) along with the role of the bilinguals’ Greek vocabulary knowledge and language input. The results are argued to contribute significantly to disentangling the role of crosslinguistic influence in gender assignment and agreement by bringing together a variety of input measures such as early and current amount of exposure to Greek, the role of area of residence (i.e. whether Greek is the minority or the majority language), the effect of maternal education and the amount of exposure to Greek in a school setting.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lab.16033.kal
2017-10-16
2025-02-16
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Argyri, F., & Sorace, A.
    (2007) Crosslinguistic influence and language dominance in older bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 79–99. doi:  10.1017/S1366728906002835
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728906002835 [Google Scholar]
  2. Armon-Lotem, S., Walters, J., & Gagarina, N.
    (2011) The impact of internal and external factors on linguistic performance in the home language and in L2 among Russian – Hebrew and Russian – German preschool children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 291–317. doi:  10.1075/lab.1.3.04arm
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.3.04arm [Google Scholar]
  3. Bauch, H. J.
    (1971) Zum Informationsgehalt der Kategorie Genus im Deutschen, Englischen und Polnischen. Wissenschaftliche Zeitschrift Der Universität Rostock: Gesellschafts-und Sprachwissenschaftliche Reihe, 20(6), 411–419.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Blair, C., Granger, D. A., Willoughby, M., Mills‐Koonce, R., Cox, M., Greenberg, M. T., Kivlighan, K. T. & Fortunato, C. K.
    (2011) Salivary cortisol mediates effects of poverty and parenting on executive functions in early childhood. Child Development, 82(6), 1970–1984. doi:  10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2011.01643.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01643.x [Google Scholar]
  5. Bialystok, E.
    (2001) Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition. Cambridge University Press. doi:  10.1017/CBO9780511605963
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511605963 [Google Scholar]
  6. Blom, E., Polišenská, D., & Weerman, F.
    (2008) Articles, adjectives and age of onset: The acquisition of Dutch grammatical gender. Second Language Research, 24(3), 297–331. doi:  10.1177/0267658308090183
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658308090183 [Google Scholar]
  7. Blom, E.
    (2010) Effects of input on the early grammatical development of bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 14, 422–446. doi:  10.1177/1367006910370917
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006910370917 [Google Scholar]
  8. Bloomfield, L.
    (1933) Language. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Bošković, Z.
    (2009a) Unifying first and last conjunct agreement. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 27(3), 455–496. doi:  10.1007/s11049‑009‑9072‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11049-009-9072-6 [Google Scholar]
  10. (2009b) On valued uninterpretable features. In: Proceedings of NELS 39, Cornell.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F.
    (2002) Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53(1), 371–399. doi:  10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233 [Google Scholar]
  12. Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E.
    (2014) Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition, 130(3), 278–288. doi:  10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.015
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.015 [Google Scholar]
  13. Cantone, K., Kupisch, T., Müller, N., & Schmitz, K.
    (2008) Rethinking language dominance in bilingual children. Linguistische Berichte, 215, 307–343.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Carroll, S. E.
    (1989) Second-language acquisition and the computational paradigm. Language Learning, 39, 535–594. doi:  10.1111/j.1467‑1770.1989.tb00902.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1989.tb00902.x [Google Scholar]
  15. Carstens, V.
    (2000) Concord in minimalist theory. Linguistic Inquiry, 31, 319–355. doi:  10.1162/002438900554370
    https://doi.org/10.1162/002438900554370 [Google Scholar]
  16. Chomsky, N.
    (1995) The Minimalist Program. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. (2001) Derivation by phase. In: Kenstovicz, M. (Ed.), Ken Hale: A Life in Language. The MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, pp.1–54.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Chondrogianni, V., & Marinis, T.
    (2011) Differential effects of internal and external factors on the development of vocabulary, tense morphology, and morphosyntax in successive bilingual children. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 318–342. doi:  10.1075/lab.1.3.05cho
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.3.05cho [Google Scholar]
  19. Cohen, J.
    (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Cobo-Lewis, A. B., Pearson, B. Z., Eilers, R. E., & Umbel, V. C.
    (2002) Effects of bilingualism and bilingual education on oral and written Spanish skills: A multifactor study of standardized test outcomes. InD. K. Oller and R. E. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Corbett, G.
    (1991) Gender. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:  10.1017/CBO9781139166119
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139166119 [Google Scholar]
  22. Cornips, L., & Hulk, A.
    (2006) External and internal factors in bilingual and bidialectal language development: Grammatical gender of the Dutch definite determiner. InC. Lefebvre, L. White, and C. Jourdan (Eds.), L2 acquisition and creole genesis: Dialogues. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi:  10.1075/lald.42.21cor
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.42.21cor [Google Scholar]
  23. De Houwer, A.
    (1990) The acquisition of two languages from birth: A case study. Cambridge University Press. doi:  10.1017/CBO9780511519789
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511519789 [Google Scholar]
  24. (2007) Parental language input patterns and children’s bilingual use. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 411–424. doi:  10.1017/S0142716407070221
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716407070221 [Google Scholar]
  25. Döpke, S.
    (1998) Competing language structures: The acquisition of verb placement by bilingual German-English children. Journal of child language, 25(3), 555–584. doi:  10.1017/S0305000998003584
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000998003584 [Google Scholar]
  26. Eichler, N., Jansen, V., & Müller, N.
    (2012) Gender acquisition in bilingual children: French – German, Italian – German, Spanish – German and Italian – French. International Journal of Bilingualism, 17(5), 550–570. doi:  10.1177/1367006911435719
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006911435719 [Google Scholar]
  27. Featherston, S., & Winkler, S.
    (Eds.) (2009) The Fruits of Empirical Linguistics 1: Process (Vol.101). Walter de Gruyter. doi:  10.1515/9783110216141
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110216141 [Google Scholar]
  28. Gathercole, V. C. M.
    (2002a) Command of the mass/count distinction in bilingual and monolingual children: An English morphosyntactic distinction. InD. K. Oller and R. E. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. 10.21832/9781853595721‑009
    https://doi.org/10.21832/9781853595721-009 [Google Scholar]
  29. (2002b) Grammatical gender in bilingual and monolingual children: A Spanish morphosyntactic distinction. InD. K. Oller and R. E. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters. 10.21832/9781853595721‑010
    https://doi.org/10.21832/9781853595721-010 [Google Scholar]
  30. Gathercole, V. C. M., & Thomas, E. M.
    (2005) Minority language survival: Input factors influencing the acquisition of Welsh. InJ. Cohen, K. McAlister, K. Rolstad and J. MacSwan (Eds.), Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Genesee, F., & Nicoladis, E.
    (2007) Bilingual first-language acquisition. InE. Hoff and P. McCardle (Eds.), Handbook of language development. Oxford: Blackwell. doi:  10.1002/9780470757833.ch16
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470757833.ch16 [Google Scholar]
  32. Goldberg, H., Paradis, J., & Crago, M.
    (2008) Lexical acquisition over time in minority L1 children learning English as L2. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 41–65. 10.1017/S014271640808003X
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271640808003X [Google Scholar]
  33. Grosjean, F.
    (1989) Neurolinguists, beware! The bilingual is not two monolinguals in one person. Brain and language, 36(1), 3–15. doi:  10.1016/0093‑934X(89)90048‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0093-934X(89)90048-5 [Google Scholar]
  34. Hawkins, R. & Tsimpli, I. M.
    (2009) Explaining target- and non-target performance in the acquisition of Gender concord by L2 speakers. Paper presented atISB7, Utrecht, July 2009.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Hayes, D. P., & Ahrens, M. G.
    (1988) Vocabulary simplification for children: A special case of ‘motherese’?. Journal of Child Language, 15(02), 395–410. doi:  10.1017/S0305000900012411
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900012411 [Google Scholar]
  36. Hoeppner, W.
    (1980) Derivative Wortbildung der deutschen Gegenwartssprache und ihre algorithmische Analyse (Vol.128). Narr.
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Hoff-Ginsberg, E.
    (1991) Mother‐child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62(4), 782–796. doi:  10.2307/1131177
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1131177 [Google Scholar]
  38. Hoff, E.
    (2006) How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental review, 26(1), 55–88.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Jansen, V.
    (2009) Determiners in first language acquisition of monolingual English and German children – A corpus-based investigation (Unpublished master’s thesis). Bergische Universität Wuppertal, Germany.
  40. Jia, G., & Aaronson, D.
    (2003) A longitudinal study of Chinese children and adolescents learning English in the United States. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(1), 131–161. doi:  10.1017/S0142716403000079
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716403000079 [Google Scholar]
  41. Jia, G., & Fuse, A.
    (2007) Acquisition of English grammatical morphology by native Mandarin speaking children and adolescents: Age-related differences. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 1280–1299. doi:  10.1044/1092‑4388(2007/090)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/090) [Google Scholar]
  42. Konta, E.
    (2013) Η κατάκτηση της ελληνικής από παιδιά με μητρική γλώσσα την τουρκική: στοιχεία από την ονοματική συμφωνία και τη μορφολογία. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki. doi:  10.12681/eadd/29880
    https://doi.org/10.12681/eadd/29880
  43. Köpcke, K. M.
    (1982) Untersuchungen zum Genussystem der deutschen Gegenwartssprache [Research on the gender system of present-day German]. Tübingen, Germany: Niemeyer. doi:  10.1515/9783111676562
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783111676562 [Google Scholar]
  44. Kuchenbrandt, I.
    (2005) Gender acquisition in bilingual Spanish. InJ. Cohen, K. T. McAlister, K. Rolstad, and J. MacSwan (Eds.) Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism (ISB4) (pp.1252–1263). Somerville: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Kupisch, T.
    (2005) Acceleration in bilingual first language acquisition. InTwan Gaerts, Ivo Van Ginneken and Haike Jacobs (Eds.), Languages and Linguistic Theory, Selected Papers from Going Romance 2003. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp.143–157. doi:  10.1075/cilt.270.09kup
    https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.270.09kup [Google Scholar]
  46. (2007) Determiners in Bilingual German-Italian children: what they tell us about the relation between language influence and language dominance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition10(1), 75–78. doi:  10.1017/S1366728906002823
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728906002823 [Google Scholar]
  47. Kupisch, T., Müller, N., & Cantone, K. F.
    (2002) Gender in monolingual and bilingual first-language acquisition. Lingue e Linguaggio, 1, 107–150.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Kupisch, T., Akpinar, D., & A. Stöhr
    (2013) Gender assignment and gender agreement in adult bilinguals and second language learners of French. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3(2), 150–179. doi:  10.1075/lab.3.2.02kup
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.3.2.02kup [Google Scholar]
  49. Lang, A.
    (1976) The semantic base of gender in German. Lingua, 40(1), 55–68. doi:  10.1016/0024‑3841(76)90032‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(76)90032-2 [Google Scholar]
  50. La Morgia, F.
    (2011) Bilingual first-language acquisition: The nature of the weak language and the role of the input. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Dublin City University.
  51. Lindholm-Leary, K.
    (2014) Bilingual and biliteracy skills in young Spanish-speaking low-SES children: impact of instructional language and primary language proficiency. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 17(2), 144–159. doi:  10.1080/13670050.2013.866625
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2013.866625 [Google Scholar]
  52. Locke, A., Ginsborg, J., & Peers, I.
    (2002) Development and disadvantage: implications for the early years and beyond. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 37(1), 3–15. doi:  10.1080/13682820110089911
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820110089911 [Google Scholar]
  53. Marinis, T.
    (2003) The Acquisition of the DP in Modern Greek. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishers. doi:  10.1075/lald.31
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.31 [Google Scholar]
  54. Mastropavlou, M.
    (2006) The role of phonological salience and feature interpretability in the grammar of typically developing and language-impaired children. Unpublished PhD dissertation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki.
  55. Mastropavlou, M., & Tsimpli, I. M.
    (2011) The role of suffixes in grammatical gender assignment in Modern Greek: A psycholinguistic study. Journal of Greek Linguistics, 11, 27–55.
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Mattheoudakis, M., Chatzidaki, A. & Maligkoudi, C.
    (2014) Bilingual Education & Types of Bilingualism. InWorkshop Language Knowledge and Development in native and non-native speakers. Aristotle University of Thessaloniki, Greece.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Meisel, J. M.
    (2009) Second-language acquisition in early childhood. Zeitschrift Für Sprachwissenschaft, 28, 5–34.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Mills, A. E.
    (1986) Acquisition of the natural-gender rule in English and German. Linguistics, 24(1), 31–46. doi:  10.1515/ling.1986.24.1.31
    https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1986.24.1.31 [Google Scholar]
  59. Montrul, S. A., Foote, R., & Perpiñán, S.
    (2008) Gender agreement in adult second-language learners and Spanish-heritage speakers: The effects of age and context of acquisition. Language Learning, 58, 503–553. doi:  10.1111/j.1467‑9922.2008.00449.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2008.00449.x [Google Scholar]
  60. Montrul, S., & Potowski, K.
    (2007) Command of gender agreement in school-age Spanish – English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11, 301–328. doi:  10.1177/13670069070110030301
    https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069070110030301 [Google Scholar]
  61. Montrul, S. A.
    (2016) Dominance and Profociency in early and late bilingualism. InC. Silva-Corvalan & J. Treffers-Daller (Eds.), Language Dominance in Bilinguals: Issues of Measurement and Operationalization. Cambridge University Press. 15–35. doi:  10.1017/CBO9781107375345.002
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107375345.002 [Google Scholar]
  62. Müller, N.
    (1987) Der Genuserwerb im Französischen und Deutschen: Eine empirische Untersuchung eines bilingualen Kindes (Unpublished master’s thesis). University of Hamburg, Germany.
  63. (1990) Developing two gender-assignment systems simultaneously. InJ. M. Meisel (Ed.), Two First Languages: Early Grammatical Development in Bilingual Children. Dordrecht: Foris. doi:  10.1515/9783110846065.193
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110846065.193 [Google Scholar]
  64. (1998) Transfer in Bilingual First Language Acquisition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition1(3), 151–171. doi:  10.1017/S1366728998000261
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728998000261 [Google Scholar]
  65. Müller, N. & Hulk, A.
    (2001) Crosslinguistic Influence in Bilingual Language Acquisition: Italian and French as Recipient Languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition4(1), 1–21. doi:  10.1017/S1366728901000116
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728901000116 [Google Scholar]
  66. Oller, D. K., & Eilers, R. E.
    (Eds.) (2002) Language and literacy in bilingual children (Vol.2). Multilingual Matters. 10.21832/9781853595721
    https://doi.org/10.21832/9781853595721 [Google Scholar]
  67. Paradis, J.
    (2010) Bilingual children’s acquisition of English verb morphology: Effects of language exposure, structure complexity, and task type. Language Learning, 60, 651–680. doi:  10.1111/j.1467‑9922.2010.00567.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00567.x [Google Scholar]
  68. (2011) Individual differences in child English second-language acquisition: Comparing child-internal and child-external factors. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1, 213–237. doi:  10.1075/lab.1.3.01par
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.1.3.01par [Google Scholar]
  69. Paradis, J., & Genesee, F.
    (1996) Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or independent?Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 1–25. doi:  10.1017/S0272263100014662
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100014662 [Google Scholar]
  70. Petermann, F.
    (2010) SET 5–10. Sprachstandserhebungstest für Kinder im Alter zwischen 5 und 10 Jahren. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Place, S., & Hoff, E.
    (2011) Properties of dual language exposure that influence two-year-olds’ bilingual proficiency. Child Development, 82, 1834–1849. doi:  10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2011.01660.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01660.x [Google Scholar]
  72. Qi, C. H., Kaiser, A. P., Milan, S., & Hancock, T.
    (2006) Language performance of low-income African American and European American preschool children on the PPVT – III. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 37(1), 5–16. doi:  10.1044/0161‑1461(2006/002)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2006/002) [Google Scholar]
  73. Ralli, A.
    (2002) The role of morphology in gender determination: Evidence from Modern Greek. Linguistics, 40, 519–551. doi:  10.1515/ling.2002.022
    https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.2002.022 [Google Scholar]
  74. Renfrew, C. E.
    (2001) Word Finding Vocabulary Test. 4th Edition. Bicester, UK: Speechmark.
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Rhoades, B. L., Greenberg, M. T., Lanza, S. T., & Blair, C.
    (2011) Demographic and familial predictors of early executive function development: Contribution of a person-centered perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108(3), 638–662. doi:  10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.08.004 [Google Scholar]
  76. Silva-Corvalán, C. & Treffers-Daller, J.
    (2016) Language Dominance in Bilinguals: Issues of Measurement and Operationalization. Cambridge University Press. doi:  10.1017/CBO9781107375345
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107375345 [Google Scholar]
  77. Stanovich, K. E., & Cunningham, A. E.
    (1993) Where does knowledge come from? Specific associations between print exposure and information acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85(2), 211. doi:  10.1037/0022‑0663.85.2.211
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.85.2.211 [Google Scholar]
  78. Stephany, U.
    (1997) The acquisition of Greek. In: Slobin, D. I. (Ed.), The Cross-linguistic Study of Language Acquisition, vol.4. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp.183–333.
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Stephany, U. & Christofidou, A.
    (2008) The Acquisition of Greek Case, Number, and Gender: A Usage-Based Approach. Arbeitspapier 55 (Neue Folge). Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft, University of Cologne.
    [Google Scholar]
  80. Szagun, G., Stumper, B., Sondag, N., & Franik, M.
    (2007) The acquisition of gender marking by young German-speaking children: Evidence for learning guided by phonological regularities. Journal of Child Language, 34(03), 445–471. doi:  10.1017/S0305000906007951
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000906007951 [Google Scholar]
  81. Tsimpli, I. M.
    (2003) Features in L1 and L2 acquisition: Evidence from Greek clitics and determiners. Acquisition et Interaction en Langue Étrangère, 20, 87–128.
    [Google Scholar]
  82. (2014) Early, late or very late? Timing acquisition and bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4(3), 283–313. doi:  10.1075/lab.4.3.01tsi
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.4.3.01tsi [Google Scholar]
  83. Tsimpli, I. M., & Hulk, A.
    (2013) Grammatical gender and the notion of default: Insights from language acquisition. Lingua, 137, 128–144. doi:  10.1016/j.lingua.2013.09.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2013.09.001 [Google Scholar]
  84. Unsworth, S., Argyri, F., Cornips, L., Hulk, A., Sorace, A. & Tsimpli, I. M.
    (2011) Bilingual acquisition of Greek voice morphology and Dutch gender: What do they have in common?InN. Danis, K. Mesh & H. Sung (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, (pp.590–602). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  85. Unsworth, S.
    (2013) Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 86–110. doi:  10.1017/S1366728912000284
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000284 [Google Scholar]
  86. Unsworth, S., Argyri, F., Cornips, L., Hulk, A., Sorace, A. & Tsimpli, I. M.
    (2014) On the role of age of onset and input in early child bilingualism in Greek and Dutch. Applied Psycholinguistics, 34(4), 765–805. doi:  10.1017/S0142716412000574
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000574 [Google Scholar]
  87. Unsworth, S.
    (2016) Amount of exposure as a proxy for dominance in bilingual language acquisition. InC. Silva-Corvalan & J. Treffers-Daller (Eds.), Language Dominance in Bilinguals: Issues of Measurement and Operationalization. Cambridge University Press. 156–173. doi:  10.1017/CBO9781107375345.008
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107375345.008 [Google Scholar]
  88. Varlokosta, S.
    (1995) Greek as a second language in minority schools of Thrace: Learning gender compared to learning number and case within the NP. Unpublished manuscript.
    [Google Scholar]
  89. (2011) The role of morphology in grammatical gender assignment: A psycholinguistic study in Greek. InA. Galani, G. Hicks, and G. Tsoulas (Eds.), Morphology and its interfaces. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing. doi:  10.1075/la.178.17var
    https://doi.org/10.1075/la.178.17var [Google Scholar]
  90. Vogindroukas, I., Protopapas, A., & Sideridis, G.
    (2009) Experiment on the Expressive Vocabulary (Greek version of Renfrew Word Finding Vocabulary Test). Chania, Crete: Glafki.
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Yip, V., & Matthews, S.
    (2000) Syntactic transfer in a Cantonese-English bilingual child. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 3(3), 193–208. doi:  10.1017/S136672890000033X
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S136672890000033X [Google Scholar]
  92. (2006) Assessing language dominance in bilingual acquisition: A case for mean length utterance differentials. Language Assessment Quarterly: An International Journal, 3(2), 97–116. doi:  10.1207/s15434311laq0302_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15434311laq0302_2 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.16033.kal
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.16033.kal
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): crosslinguistic influence; Greek gender; input measures; vocabulary
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error