1887
Volume 12, Issue 3
  • ISSN 1879-9264
  • E-ISSN: 1879-9272

Abstract

Abstract

Previous studies have reported perceptual advantages, such as when discriminating non-native linguistic or musical pitch differences, among first-year infants growing up in bilingual over monolingual environments. It is unclear whether such effects should be attributed to bilinguals’ enhanced perceptual sensitivity and/or cognitive abilities, and whether such effects would extend to adulthood. Twenty-four Dutch, 24 Dutch simultaneous bilingual (DSB), and 24 Chinese Mandarin speakers were examined by three sets of tasks assessing their linguistic pitch and music perception, executive function, as well as interactions across these modalities. Results showed degrees of advantages for DSB and Chinese participants’ over their Dutch peers in lexical tone discrimination and pitch-related music tasks. In tasks related to executive function, no difference was observed between DSB and Dutch participants, while Chinese participants’ performances were modulated by cognitive interference of language processing. Findings suggest that listeners’ enhanced sensitivity to linguistic and musical pitch may stem from acoustic (DSB) and experience (Chinese) rather than cognitive factors. Moreover, Dutch participants showed robust correlations between their linguistic and musical pitch perception, followed by limited correlations in DSB, and virtually no correlation among Chinese participants, illustrating how distinct language experiences can lead to specific pitch perception patterns between language and music.

Available under the CC BY-NC 4.0 license.
Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lab.19037.liu
2020-11-19
2024-09-14
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

/deliver/fulltext/lab.19037.liu.html?itemId=/content/journals/10.1075/lab.19037.liu&mimeType=html&fmt=ahah

References

  1. Abercombie, D.
    (1967) Elements of general phonetics. Aldine Pub. Company.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Abboub, N. , Bijeljac-Babic, R. , Serres, J. , & Nazzi, T.
    (2015) On the importance of being bilingual: Word stress processing in a context of segmental variability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 132, 111–120. 10.1016/j.jecp.2014.12.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.12.004 [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson, P.
    (2002) Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child neuropsychology, 8(2), 71–82. 10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724
    https://doi.org/10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724 [Google Scholar]
  4. Antoniou, M.
    (2019) The advantages of bilingualism debate. Annual Review of Linguistics, 5, 395–415. 10.1146/annurev‑linguistics‑011718‑011820
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011718-011820 [Google Scholar]
  5. Antoniou, M. , Liang, E. , Ettlinger, M. , & Wong, P. C.
    (2015) The bilingual advantage in phonetic learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18(4), 683–695. 10.1017/S1366728914000777
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000777 [Google Scholar]
  6. Arnett, J. A. , & Labovitz, S. S.
    (1995) Effect of physical layout in performance of the Trail Making Test. Psychological Assessment, 7(2), 220. 10.1037/1040‑3590.7.2.220
    https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.7.2.220 [Google Scholar]
  7. Bak, T. H. , Nissan, J. J. , Allerhand, M. M. , & Deary, I. J.
    (2014) Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging?. Annals of Neurology, 75(6), 959–963. 10.1002/ana.24158
    https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.24158 [Google Scholar]
  8. Bialystok, E.
    (1999) Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70(3), 636–644. 10.1111/1467‑8624.00046
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00046 [Google Scholar]
  9. (2009) Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(01), 3–11. 10.1017/S1366728908003477
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728908003477 [Google Scholar]
  10. (2017) The bilingual adaptation: how minds accommodate experience. Psychological bulletin, 143(3), 233. 10.1037/bul0000099
    https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000099 [Google Scholar]
  11. Bialystok, E. , Craik, F. I. , Klein, R. , & Viswanathan, M.
    (2004) Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and aging, 19(2), 290. 10.1037/0882‑7974.19.2.290
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290 [Google Scholar]
  12. Bialystok, E. , & DePape, A. M.
    (2009) Musical expertise, bilingualism, and executive functioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 565.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Bialystok, E. , Martin, M. M. , & Viswanathan, M.
    (2005) Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9(1), 103–119. 10.1177/13670069050090010701
    https://doi.org/10.1177/13670069050090010701 [Google Scholar]
  14. Bidelman, G. M. , & Chung, W. L.
    (2015) Tone-language speakers show hemispheric specialization and differential cortical processing of contour and interval cues for pitch. Neuroscience, 305, 384–392. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.08.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.08.010 [Google Scholar]
  15. Bidelman, G. M. , Gandour, J. T. , & Krishnan, A.
    (2011a) Cross-domain effects of music and language experience on the representation of pitch in the human auditory brainstem. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(2), 425–434. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21362
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21362 [Google Scholar]
  16. (2011b) Musicians and tone-language speakers share enhanced brainstem encoding but not perceptual benefits for musical pitch. Brain and Cognition, 77(1), 1–10. 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.07.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2011.07.006 [Google Scholar]
  17. Bidelman, G. M. , Hutka, S. , & Moreno, S.
    (2013) Tone language speakers and musicians share enhanced perceptual and cognitive abilities for musical pitch: evidence for bidirectionality between the domains of language and music. PloS one, 8(4), e60676. 10.1371/journal.pone.0060676
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0060676 [Google Scholar]
  18. Boersma, P. , & Weenink, D.
    (2009) Praat: doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.1. 05) [Computer program]. RetrievedMay 1, 2009.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Bowie, C. R. , & Harvey, P. D.
    (2006) Administration and interpretation of the Trail Making Test. Nature Protocols, 1(5), 2277–2281. 10.1038/nprot.2006.390
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.390 [Google Scholar]
  20. Burnham, D. K. , & Brooker, R.
    (2002) Absolute pitch and lexical tones: Tone perception by non-musician, musician, and absolute pitch non-tonal language speakers. In J. Hansen , & B. Pellom (Eds.), Seventh International Conference on Spoken Language Processing.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Carlson, S. M. , & Meltzoff, A. N.
    (2008) Bilingual experience and executive functioning in young children. Developmental Science, 11(2), 282–298. 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2008.00675.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00675.x [Google Scholar]
  22. Chen, A. , & Kager, R.
    (2016) Discrimination of lexical tones in the first year of life. Infant and Child Development, 25(5), 426–439. 10.1002/icd.1944
    https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1944 [Google Scholar]
  23. Chen, A. , Liu, L. , & Kager, R.
    (2015) Cross-linguistic perception of Mandarin tone sandhi. Language Sciences, 48, 62–69. 10.1016/j.langsci.2014.12.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2014.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  24. (2016) Cross-domain correlation in pitch perception, the influence of native language. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 31(6), 751–760. 10.1080/23273798.2016.1156715
    https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2016.1156715 [Google Scholar]
  25. Cooper, A. , & Wang, Y.
    (2012) The influence of linguistic and musical experience on Cantonese word learning. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 131(6), 4756–4769. 10.1121/1.4714355
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4714355 [Google Scholar]
  26. Costa, A. , Hernández, M. , Costa-Faidella, J. , & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
    (2009) On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don’t. Cognition, 113(2), 135–149. 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001 [Google Scholar]
  27. Crinion, J. , Turner, R. , Grogan, A. , Hanakawa, T. , Noppeney, U. , Devlin, J. T. ,... & Usui, K.
    (2006) Language control in the bilingual brain. Science, 312(5779), 1537–1540. 10.1126/science.1127761
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1127761 [Google Scholar]
  28. De Bruin, A. , Treccani, B. , & Della Sala, S.
    (2014) Cognitive advantage in bilingualism an example of publication bias?. Psychological Science, 26(1), 99–107. 10.1177/0956797614557866
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614557866 [Google Scholar]
  29. Delogu, F. , Lampis, G. , & Belardinelli, M. O.
    (2006) Music-to-language transfer effect: May melodic ability improve learning of tonal languages by native nontonal speakers?. Cognitive Processing, 7(3), 203–207. 10.1007/s10339‑006‑0146‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-006-0146-7 [Google Scholar]
  30. (2010) From melody to lexical tone: Musical ability enhances specific aspects of foreign language perception. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(1), 46–61. 10.1080/09541440802708136
    https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440802708136 [Google Scholar]
  31. Dick, A. S. , Garcia, N. L. , Pruden, S. M. , Thompson, W. K. , Hawes, S. W. , Sutherland, M. T. ,... & Gonzalez, R.
    (2019) No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nature human behaviour, 3(7), 692–701. 10.1038/s41562‑019‑0609‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0609-3 [Google Scholar]
  32. Donnelly, S. , Brooks, P. J. , & Homer, B. D.
    (2015, July). Examining the bilingual advantage on conflict resolution tasks: A meta-analysis. In D. Noelle , & R. Dale (Eds.), Proceedings of the 37th Annual Conference of The Cognitive Science Society, Pasadena, CA.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Duñabeitia, J. A. , Hernández, J. A. , Antón, E. , Macizo, P. , Estévez, A. , Fuentes, L. J. , & Carreiras, M.
    (2014) The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited. Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 234–251. 10.1027/1618‑3169/a000243
    https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000243 [Google Scholar]
  34. Francis, A. L. , Ciocca, V. , Ma, L. , & Fenn, K.
    (2008) Perceptual learning of Cantonese lexical tones by tone and non-tone language speakers. Journal of Phonetics, 36(2), 268–294. 10.1016/j.wocn.2007.06.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2007.06.005 [Google Scholar]
  35. Gandour, J. , Tong, Y. , Wong, D. , Talavage, T. , Dzemidzic, M. , Xu, Y. , Lie, X. , & Lowe, M.
    (2004) Hemispheric roles in the perception of speech prosody. Neuroimage, 23(1), 344–357. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.004 [Google Scholar]
  36. Gerrits, E. , & Schouten, M. E. H.
    (2004) Categorical perception depends on the discrimination task. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(3), 363–376. 10.3758/BF03194885
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194885 [Google Scholar]
  37. Gioia, G. A. , Isquith, P. K. , & Guy, S. C.
    (2001) Assessment of executive functions in children with neurological impairment. In R. J. Simeonsson , & L. Rosenthal (Eds.), Psychological and Developmental Assessment: Children with Disabilities and Chronic Conditions (317–356). New York: Guilford Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Hallé, P. A. , Chang, Y. C. , & Best, C. T.
    (2004) Identification and discrimination of Mandarin Chinese tones by Mandarin Chinese vs. French listeners. Journal of Phonetics, 32(3), 395–421. 10.1016/S0095‑4470(03)00016‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(03)00016-0 [Google Scholar]
  39. Hannon, E. E. , & Trainor, L. J.
    (2007) Music acquisition: effects of enculturation and formal training on development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(11), 466–472. 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.008 [Google Scholar]
  40. Hay, J. F. , Graf Estes, K. , Wang, T. , & Saffran, J. R.
    (2015) From flexibility to constraint: The contrastive use of lexical tone in early word learning. Child Development, 86(1), 10–22. 10.1111/cdev.12269
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12269 [Google Scholar]
  41. Hopp, H. , Vogelbacher, M. , Kieseier, T. , & Thoma, D.
    (2019) Bilingual advantages in early foreign language learning: Effects of the minority and the majority language. Learning and Instruction, 61, 99–110. 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.02.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.02.001 [Google Scholar]
  42. Huang, T. , & Johnson, K.
    (2011) Language specificity in speech perception: Perception of Mandarin tones by native and nonnative listeners. Phonetica, 67(4), 243–267. 10.1159/000327392
    https://doi.org/10.1159/000327392 [Google Scholar]
  43. Kaufman, A. S. , Flanagan, D. P. , Alfonso, V. C. , & Mascolo, J. T.
    (2006) Test review: Wechsler intelligence scale for children, (WISC-IV). Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 24(3), 278–295. 10.1177/0734282906288389
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282906288389 [Google Scholar]
  44. Kim, K. H. , Relkin, N. R. , Lee, K. M. , & Hirsch, J.
    (1997) Distinct cortical areas associated with native and second languages. Nature, 388(6638), 171–174. 10.1038/40623
    https://doi.org/10.1038/40623 [Google Scholar]
  45. Klein, D. , Zatorre, R. J. , Milner, B. , & Zhao, V.
    (2001) A cross-linguistic PET study of tone perception in Mandarin Chinese and English speakers. Neuroimage, 13(4), 646–653. 10.1006/nimg.2000.0738
    https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2000.0738 [Google Scholar]
  46. Koelsch, S. , Gunter, T. C. , Cramon, D. Y. V. , Zysset, S. , Lohmann, G. , & Friederici, A. D.
    (2002) Bach speaks: a cortical “language-network” serves the processing of music. Neuroimage, 17(2), 956–966. 10.1006/nimg.2002.1154
    https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1154 [Google Scholar]
  47. Krizman, J. , Marian, V. , Shook, A. , Skoe, E. , & Kraus, N.
    (2012) Subcortical encoding of sound is enhanced in bilinguals and relates to executive function advantages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(20), 7877–7881. 10.1073/pnas.1201575109
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1201575109 [Google Scholar]
  48. Kuhl, P. K. , Stevens, E. , Hayashi, A. , Deguchi, T. , Kiritani, S. , & Iverson, P.
    (2006) Infants show a facilitation effect for native language phonetic perception between 6 and 12 months. Developmental Science, 9(2), F13–F21. 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2006.00468.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00468.x [Google Scholar]
  49. Kuhl, P. K. , Conboy, B. T. , Coffey-Corina, S. , Padden, D. , Rivera-Gaxiola, M. , & Nelson, T.
    (2008) Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: new data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1493), 979–1000. 10.1098/rstb.2007.2154
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2154 [Google Scholar]
  50. Lee, C. Y. , & Hung, T. H.
    (2008) Identification of Mandarin tones by English-speaking musicians and nonmusicians. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 124(5), 3235–3248. 10.1121/1.2990713
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2990713 [Google Scholar]
  51. Lehtonen, M. , Soveri, A. , Laine, A. , Järvenpää, J. , De Bruin, A. , & Antfolk, J.
    (2018) Is bilingualism associated with enhanced executive functioning in adults? A meta-analytic review. Psychological bulletin, 144(4), 394. 10.1037/bul0000142
    https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000142 [Google Scholar]
  52. Lezak, M. D. , Howieson, D. B. , & Loring, D. W.
    (2004) Neuropsychological evaluation. New York: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Liu, F. , Jiang, C. , Thompson, W. , Xu, Y. , Yang, Y. , & Stewart, L.
    (2012) The mechanism of speech processing in congenital amusia: Evidence from Mandarin speakers. Plos One, 7, e30374.
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Liu, L.
    (2014) The Effects of Bilingualism on Infant Language Development: The Acquisition of Sounds and Words. PhD dissertation, Utrecht University (ISBN78-94-6093-129-1).
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Liu, L. , Chen, A. , & Kager, R.
    (2017) Tone perception in Mandarin and Dutch adult listeners. Language and Linguistics, 18(4), 622–646.
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Liu, L. , & Kager, R. W. J.
    (2014) Perception of tones by infants learning a non-tone language. Perception of tones by infants learning a non-tone language. Cognition, 133(2), 185–194. 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.06.004 [Google Scholar]
  57. (2015) Understanding phonological acquisition through phonetic perception: The influence of exposure and acoustic salience. Phonological Studies, 18, 51–58.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. (2016) Perception of a native vowel contrast by Dutch monolingual and bilingual infants: A bilingual perceptual lead. International Journal of Bilingualism, 20(3), 335–345. 10.1177/1367006914566082
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006914566082 [Google Scholar]
  59. (2017a) Perception of tones by bilingual infants learning non-tone languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(3), 561–575. 10.1017/S1366728916000183
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000183 [Google Scholar]
  60. (2017b) Enhanced music sensitivity in 9-month-old bilingual infants. Cognitive Processing, 18(1), 55–56. 10.1007/s10339‑016‑0780‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-016-0780-7 [Google Scholar]
  61. Liu, L. , & Weidemann, G.
    (2017) Is it wise to raise your child bilingually?. China Language Strategies, 5(1), Nanjing University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Lukasik, K. M. , Lehtonen, M. , Soveri, A. , Waris, O. , Jylkkä, J. , & Laine, M.
    (2018) Bilingualism and working memory performance: Evidence from a large-scale online study. PloS one, 13(11). 10.1371/journal.pone.0205916
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0205916 [Google Scholar]
  63. Mattock, K. , & Burnham, D.
    (2006) Chinese and English infants’ tone perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization. Infancy, 10(3), 241–265. 10.1207/s15327078in1003_3
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in1003_3 [Google Scholar]
  64. McDowd, J. M. & Shaw, R. J.
    (2000) Attention and aging: A functional perspective.
    [Google Scholar]
  65. McMullen, E. & Saffran, J. R.
    (2004) Music and language: A developmental comparison. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21(3), 289–311. 10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.289
    https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2004.21.3.289 [Google Scholar]
  66. Mok, P.
    (2009) On the syllable-timing of Cantonese and Beijing Mandarin. Chinese Journal of Phonetics, 2, 148–154.
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Molnar, M. , Carreiras, M. , & Gervain, J.
    (2016) Language dominance shapes non-linguistic rhythmic grouping in bilinguals. Cognition, 152, 150–159. 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.023
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.023 [Google Scholar]
  68. Molnar, M. , Gervain, J. , & Carreiras, M.
    (2014) Within-rhythm class native language discrimination abilities of Basque-Spanish monolingual and bilingual infants at 3.5 months of age. Infancy, 19(3), 326–337. 10.1111/infa.12041
    https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12041 [Google Scholar]
  69. Moreno, S.
    (2009) Can music influence language and cognition?. Contemporary Music Review, 28(3), 329–345. 10.1080/07494460903404410
    https://doi.org/10.1080/07494460903404410 [Google Scholar]
  70. Nan, Y. , Sun, Y. , & Peretz, I.
    (2010) Congenital amusia in speakers of a tone language: Association with lexical tone agnosia. Brain, 133(9), 2635–2642. 10.1093/brain/awq178
    https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awq178 [Google Scholar]
  71. Nazzi, T. , Bertoncini, J. , & Mehler, J.
    (1998) Language discrimination by newborns: toward an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(3), 756.
    [Google Scholar]
  72. Nettl, B.
    (2000) An ethnomusicologist contemplates universals in musical sound and musical culture. In N. L. Wallin , B. Merker & S. Brown (Eds.), The Origins of Music (pp.463–472). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Paap, K. R. , & Greenberg, Z. I.
    (2013) There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66(2), 232–258. 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  74. Paap, K. R. , Johnson, H. A. , & Sawi, O.
    (2015) Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014 [Google Scholar]
  75. Patel, A. D.
    (2008) Science & music: talk of the tone. Nature, 453(7196), 726. 10.1038/453726a
    https://doi.org/10.1038/453726a [Google Scholar]
  76. Patel, A. D. , & Iversen, J. R.
    (2007) The linguistic benefits of musical abilities. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 369–372. 10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2007.08.003 [Google Scholar]
  77. Peretz, I. , Champod, A. S. , & Hyde, K.
    (2003) Varieties of musical disorders. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 999(1), 58–75. 10.1196/annals.1284.006
    https://doi.org/10.1196/annals.1284.006 [Google Scholar]
  78. Petitto, L. A. , Berens, M. S. , Kovelman, I. , Dubins, M. H. , Jasinska, K. , & Shalinsky, M.
    (2012) The “Perceptual Wedge Hypothesis” as the basis for bilingual babies’ phonetic processing advantage: New insights from fNIRS brain imaging. Brain and Language, 121(2), 130–143. 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.05.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.05.003 [Google Scholar]
  79. Pfordresher, P. Q. , & Brown, S.
    (2009) Enhanced production and perception of musical pitch in tone language speakers. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 71(6), 1385–1398. 10.3758/APP.71.6.1385
    https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.71.6.1385 [Google Scholar]
  80. Ressel, V. , Pallier, C. , Ventura-Campos, N. , Díaz, B. , Roessler, A. , Ávila, C. , & Sebastián-Gallés, N.
    (2012) An effect of bilingualism on the auditory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(47), 16597–16601. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1996‑12.2012
    https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1996-12.2012 [Google Scholar]
  81. Roncaglia-Denissen, P. , Roor, D. , Chen, A. , & Sadakata, M.
    (2016) Mastering languages with different rhythmic properties enhances musical rhythm perception. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 228.   10.3389/fnhum.2016.00288
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00288 [Google Scholar]
  82. Shunk, A. W. , Davis, A. S. , & Dean, R. S.
    (2006) TEST REVIEW: Dean C. Delis, Edith Kaplan & Joel H. Kramer, Delis Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS), The Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX, 2001. $415.00 (complete kit). Applied Neuropsychology, 13(4), 275–327. 10.1207/s15324826an1304_9
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324826an1304_9 [Google Scholar]
  83. Singh, L.
    (2017) Bilingual infants demonstrate advantages in learning words in a third language. Child Development, 89(4), e397–e413. 10.1111/cdev.12852
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12852 [Google Scholar]
  84. Singh, L. , & Chee, M.
    (2016) Rise and fall: effects of tone and intonation on spoken word recognition in early childhood. Journal of Phonetics, 55, 109–118. 10.1016/j.wocn.2015.12.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2015.12.005 [Google Scholar]
  85. Singh, L. , Fu, C. S. , Tay, Z. W. , & Golinkoff, R. M.
    (2017) Novel word learning in bilingual and monolingual infants: Evidence for a bilingual advantage. Child Development, 89(3), e183–e198. 10.1111/cdev.12747
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12747 [Google Scholar]
  86. Soley, G. , & Hannon, E. E.
    (2010) Infants prefer the musical meter of their own culture: A cross-cultural comparison. Developmental Psychology, 46(1), 286–292. 10.1037/a0017555
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017555 [Google Scholar]
  87. Song, J. H. , Skoe, E. , Wong, P. C. , & Kraus, N.
    (2008) Plasticity in the adult human auditory brainstem following short-term linguistic training. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(10), 1892–1902. 10.1162/jocn.2008.20131
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2008.20131 [Google Scholar]
  88. Stroop, J. R.
    (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 18(6), 643. 10.1037/h0054651
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651 [Google Scholar]
  89. Tervaniemi, M. A. , Kujala, A. , Alho, K. , Virtanen, J. , Ilmoniemi, R. J. , & Näätänen, R.
    (1999) Functional specialization of the human auditory cortex in processing phonetic and musical sounds: a magnetoencephalographic (MEG) study. Neuroimage, 9(3), 330–336. 10.1006/nimg.1999.0405
    https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1999.0405 [Google Scholar]
  90. Thompson, W. F. , Schellenberg, E. G. , & Husain, G.
    (2004) Decoding speech prosody: Do music lessons help?. Emotion, 4(1), 46. 10.1037/1528‑3542.4.1.46
    https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.4.1.46 [Google Scholar]
  91. Tombaugh, T. N.
    (2004) Trail Making Test A and B: normative data stratified by age and education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(2), 203–214. 10.1016/S0887‑6177(03)00039‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0887-6177(03)00039-8 [Google Scholar]
  92. Trainor, L. J. , Tsang, C. D. , & Cheung, V. H.
    (2002) Preference for sensory consonance in 2-and 4-month-old infants. Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 20(2), 187–194. 10.1525/mp.2002.20.2.187
    https://doi.org/10.1525/mp.2002.20.2.187 [Google Scholar]
  93. Tsao, F. M.
    (2017) Perceptual Improvement of lexical tones in infants: effects of tone language experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 558. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00558
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00558 [Google Scholar]
  94. Valian, V.
    (2015) Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism, 18(1), 3–24. 10.1017/S1366728914000522
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000522 [Google Scholar]
  95. Vega-Mendoza, M. , West, H. , Sorace, A. , & Bak, T. H.
    (2015) The impact of late, non-balanced bilingualism on cognitive performance. Cognition, 137, 40–46. 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.008 [Google Scholar]
  96. von Bastian, C. C. , Souza, A. S. , & Gade, M.
    (2016) No evidence for bilingual cognitive advantages: A test of four hypotheses. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(2), 246. 10.1037/xge0000120
    https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000120 [Google Scholar]
  97. Wallentin, M. , Nielsen, A. H. , Friis-Olivarius, M. , Vuust, C. , & Vuust, P.
    (2010) The Musical Ear Test, a new reliable test for measuring musical competence. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(3), 188–196. 10.1016/j.lindif.2010.02.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2010.02.004 [Google Scholar]
  98. Wang, Y. , Jongman, A. , & Sereno, J. A.
    (2001) Dichotic perception of Mandarin tones by Chinese and American listeners. Brain and Language, 78(3), 332–348. 10.1006/brln.2001.2474
    https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2001.2474 [Google Scholar]
  99. Wang, M. , Perfetti, C. A. , & Liu, Y.
    (2005) Chinese–English biliteracy acquisition: Cross-language and writing system transfer. Cognition, 97(1), 67–88. 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.10.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2004.10.001 [Google Scholar]
  100. Wayland, R. P. , & Guion, S. G.
    (2004) Training English and Chinese Listeners to Perceive Thai Tones: A Preliminary Report. Language Learning, 54(4), 681–712. 10.1111/j.1467‑9922.2004.00283.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2004.00283.x [Google Scholar]
  101. Werker, J. F.
    (1986) The effect of multilingualism on phonetic perceptual flexibility. Applied Psycholinguistics, 7(2), 141–155. 10.1017/S0142716400007360
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400007360 [Google Scholar]
  102. Werker, J. F. , & Tees, R. C.
    (2002) Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 25(1), 121–133. 10.1016/S0163‑6383(02)00093‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00093-0 [Google Scholar]
  103. Winkler, I. , Háden, G. P. , Ladinig, O. , Sziller, I. , & Honing, H.
    (2009) Newborn infants detect the beat in music. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(7), 2468–2471. 10.1073/pnas.0809035106
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0809035106 [Google Scholar]
  104. Wong, P. C.
    (2002) Hemispheric specialization of linguistic pitch patterns. Brain research bulletin, 59(2), 83–95. 10.1016/S0361‑9230(02)00860‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-9230(02)00860-2 [Google Scholar]
  105. Wong, P. C. , Skoe, E. , Russo, N. M. , Dees, T. , & Kraus, N.
    (2007) Musical experience shapes human brainstem encoding of linguistic pitch patterns. Nature Neuroscience, 10(4), 420. 10.1038/nn1872
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1872 [Google Scholar]
  106. Yeung, H. H. , Chen, K. H. , & Werker, J. F.
    (2013) When does native language input affect phonetic perception? The precocious case of lexical tone. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(2), 123–139. 10.1016/j.jml.2012.09.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.09.004 [Google Scholar]
  107. Yip, M.
    (2002) Tone. Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9781139164559
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139164559 [Google Scholar]
  108. Zatorre, R. J. , Belin, P. , & Penhune, V. B.
    (2002) Structure and function of auditory cortex: music and speech. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(1), 37–46. 10.1016/S1364‑6613(00)01816‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01816-7 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.19037.liu
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.19037.liu
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): bilingualism; enhanced sensitivity; executive function; lexical tone; musical pitch
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error