1887
Volume 12, Issue 6
  • ISSN 1879-9264
  • E-ISSN: 1879-9272
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

How parents talk to young children matters to language and cognitive development. In the early years the quantity, quality, and diversity inherent in language from parents in the home predict differences in vocabulary knowledge, school readiness, and later academic achievement. However, most of what is known about child-directed speech (CDS) comes from studies of monolingual parents, and little is known about features of speech from bilingual parents. Here, we asked whether degree of bilingualism assessed within a single parent might be positively associated with CDS features that are known to facilitate children’s lexical and grammatical structures across languages – parental partial repetitions. During unscripted narrations ( = 91) of a picture book to their toddlers in English, mothers who reported being more bilingually balanced used a higher proportion of self-repetitions (both single words and 2-word combinations) within a brief time-frame. At the same time, more bilingual mothers preserved the same degree of lexical diversity as more monolingual mothers. The results obtained even accounting for differences in socio-economic status. These findings are discussed in terms of adaptive strategies that bilingual parents may consciously or unconsciously adopt in bilingual language development

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lab.20011.li
2021-04-15
2025-02-19
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Abu Rabia, S.
    (2018) The effect of degrees of bilingualism on metacognitive linguistic skills. International Journal of Bilingualism, 23(5), 1064–1086. 10.1177/1367006918781060
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006918781060 [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson, J. A. , Mak, L. , Chahi, A. K. , & Bialystok, E.
    (2018) The language and social background questionnaire: Assessing degree of bilingualism in a diverse population. Behavior Research Methods, 50(1), 250–263. 10.3758/s13428‑017‑0867‑9
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0867-9 [Google Scholar]
  3. Anthony, J. J. R. , & Blumenfeld, H. K.
    (2019) Language dominance predicts cognate effects and inhibitory control in young adult bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(5), 1068–1084. 10.1017/S1366728918001013
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918001013 [Google Scholar]
  4. Bartolotti, J. , Marian, V. , Schroeder, S. R. , & Shook, A.
    (2011) Bilingualism and inhibitory control influence statistical learning of novel word forms. Frontiers in Psychology, 23(5), 1064–1086. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00324
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00324 [Google Scholar]
  5. Bedore, L. M. , Peña, E. D. , Summers, C. L. , Boerger, K. M. , Resendiz, M. D. , Greene, K. , Gillam, R. B.
    (2012) The measure matters: Language dominance profiles across measures in Spanish–English bilingual children. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(3), 616–629. 10.1017/S1366728912000090
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728912000090 [Google Scholar]
  6. Bialystok, E. , & Barac, R.
    (2012) Emerging bilingualism: Dissociating advantages for metalinguistic awareness and executive control. Cognition, 122(1), 67–73. 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.08.003 [Google Scholar]
  7. Birdsong, D.
    (2014) Dominance and age in bilingualism. Applied Linguistics, 35(4), 374–392. 10.1093/applin/amu031
    https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amu031 [Google Scholar]
  8. Bonfieni, M. , Branigan, H. P. , Pickering, M. J. , & Sorace, A.
    (2019) Language experience modulates bilingual language control: The effect of proficiency, age of acquisition, and exposure on language switching. Acta Psychologica, 193, 160–170. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.11.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.11.004 [Google Scholar]
  9. Bornstein, M. H. , Haynes, M. O. , & Painter, K. M.
    (1998) Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25(2), 367–393. 10.1017/S0305000998003456
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000998003456 [Google Scholar]
  10. Brito, N. , & Barr, R.
    (2012) Influence of bilingualism on memory generalization during infancy. Developmental Science, 15(6), 812–816. 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2012.1184.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.1184.x [Google Scholar]
  11. Brodsky, P. , & Waterfall, H. R.
    (2007) Characterizing motherese: On the computational structure of child-directed language. In D. S. McNamara & J. G. Trafton (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Annual Cognitive Science Society (pp.64–70). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. de Bruin, A.
    (2019) Not all bilinguals are the same: A call for more detailed assessments and descriptions of bilingual experiences. Behavioral Sciences, 9(3), 33. 10.3390/bs9030033
    https://doi.org/10.3390/bs9030033 [Google Scholar]
  13. De Houwer, A.
    (2009) Bilingual first language acquisition. Multilingual Matters. 10.21832/9781847691507
    https://doi.org/10.21832/9781847691507 [Google Scholar]
  14. Demetras, M. J. , Post, K. N. , & Snow, C. E.
    (1986) Feedback to first language learners: The role of repetitions and clarification questions. Journal of Child Language, 13(2), 275–292. 10.1017/S0305000900008059
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008059 [Google Scholar]
  15. Fernald, A. , & Hurtado, N.
    (2006) Names in frames: infants interpret words in sentence frames faster than words in isolation. Developmental Science, 9(3), F33–40. 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2006.00482.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2006.00482.x [Google Scholar]
  16. Flack, Z. M. , Field, A. P. , & Horst, J. S.
    (2018) The effects of shared storybook reading on word learning: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 54(7), 1334. 10.1037/dev0000512
    https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000512 [Google Scholar]
  17. Gathercole, V. C. M. , & Thomas, E. M.
    (2009) Bilingual first-language development: Dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12(2), 213–237. 10.1017/S1366728909004015
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728909004015 [Google Scholar]
  18. Goldstein, M. H. , Waterfall, H. R. , Lotem, A. , Halpern, J. Y. , Schwade, J. A. , Onnis, L. , & Edelman, S.
    (2010) General cognitive principles for learning structure in time and space. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(6), 249–258. 10.1016/j.tics.2010.02.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.02.004 [Google Scholar]
  19. Gollan, T. H. , Salmon, D. P. , Montoya, R. I. , & Galasko, D. R.
    (2011) Degree of bilingualism predicts age of diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease in low-education but not in highly educated Hispanics. Neuropsychologia, 49(14), 3826–3830. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.041
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.09.041 [Google Scholar]
  20. Gollan, T. H. , Weissberger, G. H. , Runnqvist, E. , Montoya, R. I. , & Cera, C. M.
    (2012) Self-ratings of spoken language dominance: A multilingual naming test (mint) and preliminary norms for young and aging Spanish–English bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15(3), 594–615. 10.1017/S1366728911000332
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000332 [Google Scholar]
  21. Goodman, J. C. , Dale, P. S. , & Li, P.
    (2008, June). Does frequency count? Parental input and the acquisition of vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 35(3), 515–531. 10.1017/S0305000907008641
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000907008641 [Google Scholar]
  22. Grosjean, F.
    (2012) Bilingual. Life and reality, Harvard: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Hart, B.
    (1991) Input frequency and children’s first words. First Language, 11(32), 289–300. 10.1177/014272379101103205
    https://doi.org/10.1177/014272379101103205 [Google Scholar]
  24. Hart, B. , & Risley, T. R.
    (2003) The early catastrophe: The 30 million word gap by age 3. American Educator, 27(1), 4–9.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Hayakawa, S. , & Marian, V.
    (2019) Consequences of multilingualism for neural architecture. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 15(1), 6. 10.1186/s12993‑019‑0157‑z
    https://doi.org/10.1186/s12993-019-0157-z [Google Scholar]
  26. Hoff, E.
    (2003) The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74(5), 1368–1378. 10.1111/1467‑8624.00612
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00612 [Google Scholar]
  27. (2006) How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88. 10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002 [Google Scholar]
  28. Hoff, E. , Burridge, A. , Ribot, K. M. , & Giguere, D.
    (2017) Language specificity in the relation of maternal education to bilingual children’s vocabulary growth. Developmental Psychology, 54(6), 1011. 10.1037/dev0000492
    https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000492 [Google Scholar]
  29. Hoff, E. , & Core, C.
    (2013) Input and language development in bilingually developing children. InSeminars in Speech and Language (Vol.34, No.4, p.215). NIH Public Access.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Hoff, E. , Core, C. , Place, S. , Rumiche, R. , Señor, M. , & Parra, M.
    (2012) Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 39(1), 1–27. 10.1017/S0305000910000759
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000910000759 [Google Scholar]
  31. Hoff, E. , Welsh, S. , Place, S. , & Ribot, K.
    (2014) Properties of dual language input that shape bilingual development and properties of environments that shape dual language input. In T. Grüter & J. Paradis (Eds.), Input and experience in bilingual development (pp.119–140). John Benjamins. 10.1075/tilar.13.07hof
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.13.07hof [Google Scholar]
  32. Incera, S. , & McLennan, C. T.
    (2018) Bilingualism and age are continuous variables that influence executive function. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 25(3), 443–463. 10.1080/13825585.2017.1319902
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13825585.2017.1319902 [Google Scholar]
  33. Jones, G. , & Rowland, C. F.
    (2017) Diversity not quantity in caregiver speech: Using computational modeling to isolate the effects of the quantity and the diversity of the input on vocabulary growth. Cognitive Psychology, 98, 1–21. 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.07.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2017.07.002 [Google Scholar]
  34. Kostyrka-Allchorne, K. , Cooper, N. R. , & Simpson, A.
    (2017) The relationship between television exposure and children’s cognition and behavior: A systematic review. Developmental Review, 44, 19–58. 10.1016/j.dr.2016.12.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2016.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  35. Küntay, A. , & Slobin, D. I.
    (1996) Listening to a Turkish mother: Some puzzles for acquisition. In D. Slobin , J. Gerhardt , A. Kyratzis , & J. Guo (Eds.), Social interaction, social context, and language: Essays in honor of Susan Ervin-Tripp (pp.265–286). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. (2002) Putting interaction back into child language: Examples from Turkish. Psychology of Language and Communication, 6(1), 5–14.
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Levickis, P. , Reilly, S. , Girolametto, L. , Ukoumunne, O. C. , & Wake, M.
    (2014) Maternal behaviors promoting language acquisition in slow-to-talk toddlers: prospective community-based study. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 35(4), 274–281. 10.1097/DBP.0000000000000056
    https://doi.org/10.1097/DBP.0000000000000056 [Google Scholar]
  38. Luk, G. , & Bialystok, E.
    (2013) Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605–621. 10.1080/20445911.2013.795574
    https://doi.org/10.1080/20445911.2013.795574 [Google Scholar]
  39. MacWhinney, B.
    (2014) The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk, volume II: The database. Psychology Press. 10.4324/9781315805641
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315805641 [Google Scholar]
  40. Mayor, J. , Arias-Trejo, N. , & Alva Canto, E. A.
    (2018) The impact of household size on lexical typicality: An early link between language and social cognition?Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1445. 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01445
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01445 [Google Scholar]
  41. McCarthy, P. M. , & Jarvis, S.
    (2010) MTLD, vocd-D, and HD-D: A validation study of sophisticated approaches to lexical diversity assessment. Behavior Research Methods, 42(2), 381–392. 10.3758/BRM.42.2.381
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.2.381 [Google Scholar]
  42. McCarthy, P. M. , & Javis, S.
    (2007) VOCD: A theoretical and empirical evaluation. Language Testing, 24(4), 459–488. 10.1177/0265532207080767
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0265532207080767 [Google Scholar]
  43. McDuffie, A. , & Yoder, P.
    (2010) Types of parent verbal responsiveness that predict language in young children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(4), 1026–1039. 10.1044/1092‑4388(2009/09‑0023)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/09-0023) [Google Scholar]
  44. Newman, R. S. , Rowe, M. L. , & Bernstein Ratner, N.
    (2016) Input and uptake at 7 months predicts toddler vocabulary: the role of child-directed speech and infant processing skills in language development. Journal of Child Language, 43(5), 1158–1173. 10.1017/S0305000915000446
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000915000446 [Google Scholar]
  45. Newport, E. , Gleitman, H. , & Gleitman, L.
    (1977) Mother, I’d rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. In C. E. Snow (Ed.) Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp.109–150).
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Onnis, L. , Esposito, G. , Venuti, P. , & Edelman, S.
    (2021) Parental speech to typical and atypical populations: A study on linguistic partial repetition. Language Sciences, 83, 101311.   10.1016/j.langsci.2020.101311
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2020.101311 [Google Scholar]
  47. Onnis, L. , Chun, W. E. , & Lou-Magnuson, M.
    (2018) Improved statistical learning abilities in adult bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 21(2), 427–433. 10.1017/S1366728917000529
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728917000529 [Google Scholar]
  48. Onnis, L. , & Edelman, S.
    (2019) Local versus global statistical learning in language.   10.31234/osf.io/xe5uw
    https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/xe5uw [Google Scholar]
  49. Onnis, L. , Waterfall, H. R. , & Edelman, S.
    (2008) Learn locally, act globally: Learning language from variation set cues. Cognition, 109(3), 423–430. 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.10.004 [Google Scholar]
  50. Ordin, M. , Polyanskaya, L. , & Soto, D.
    (2020) Metacognitive processing in language learning tasks is affected by bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(3), 529.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Pearson, B. Z. , Fernandez, S. C. , Lewedeg, V. , & Oller, D. K.
    (1997) The relation of input factors to lexical learning by bilingual infants. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18(1), 41–58. 10.1017/S0142716400009863
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400009863 [Google Scholar]
  52. Romeo, R. R. , Segaran, J. , Leonard, J. A. , Robinson, S. T. , West, M. R. , Mackey, A. P. , Gabrieli, J. D.
    (2018) Language exposure relates to structural neural connectivity in childhood. Journal of Neuroscience, 38(36), 7870–7877. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0484‑18.2018
    https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0484-18.2018 [Google Scholar]
  53. Rowe, M. L.
    (2012) A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 1762–1774. 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2012.01805.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.x [Google Scholar]
  54. Schwab, J. F. , & Lew-Williams, C.
    (2016) Repetition across successive sentences facilitates young children’s word learning. Developmental Psychology, 52(6), 879–886. 10.1037/dev0000125
    https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000125 [Google Scholar]
  55. Schwab, J. F. , Rowe, M. L. , Cabrera, N. , & Lew-Williams, C.
    (2018) Fathers’ repetition of words is coupled with children’s vocabularies. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 166, 437–450. 10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.012
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.012 [Google Scholar]
  56. Sperry, D. E. , Sperry, L. L. , & Miller, P. J.
    (2018) Reexamining the verbal environments of children from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Child Development, 90(4), 1303–1318. 10.1111/cdev.13072
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13072 [Google Scholar]
  57. Tal, S. , & Arnon, I.
    (2018) SES effects on the use of variation sets in child-directed speech. Journal of Child Language, 45(6), 1423–1438. 10.1017/S0305000918000223
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000918000223 [Google Scholar]
  58. Taumoepeau, M.
    (2016) Maternal expansions of child language relate to growth in children’s vocabulary. Language Learning and Development, 12(4), 429–446. 10.1080/15475441.2016.1158112
    https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2016.1158112 [Google Scholar]
  59. Tomasello, M.
    (2009) The usage-based theory of language acquisition. In E. L. Bavin (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of child language (pp.69–87). Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511576164.005
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511576164.005 [Google Scholar]
  60. Treffers-Daller, J.
    (2019) What defines language dominance in bilinguals?Annual Review of Linguistics, 5, 375–393. 10.1146/annurev‑linguistics‑011817‑045554
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-linguistics-011817-045554 [Google Scholar]
  61. Vernon-Feagans, L. , Pancsofar, N. , Willoughby, M. , Odom, E. , Quade, A. , Cox, M. , & Family Life Key Investigators
    (2008) Predictors of maternal language to infants during a picture book task in the home: Family SES, child characteristics and the parenting environment. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29(3), 213–226. 10.1016/j.appdev.2008.02.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2008.02.007 [Google Scholar]
  62. Wang, T. , & Saffran, J. R.
    (2014) Statistical learning of a tonal language: The influence of bilingualism and previous linguistic experience. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 953. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00953
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00953 [Google Scholar]
  63. Waterfall, H. R.
    (2006) A little change is a good thing: feature theory, language acquisition and variation sets (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Chicago.
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Waterfall, H. R. , Sandbank, B. , Onnis, L. , & Edelman, S.
    (2010) An empirical generative framework for computational modeling of language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 37(3), 671–703. 10.1017/S0305000910000024
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000910000024 [Google Scholar]
  65. Whitehurst, G. J. , Falco, F. L. , Lonigan, C. J. , Fischel, J. E. , DeBaryshe, B. D. , Valdez-Menchaca, M. C. , & Caulfield, M.
    (1988) Accelerating language development through picture book reading. Developmental Psychology, 24(4), 552. 10.1037/0012‑1649.24.4.552
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.24.4.552 [Google Scholar]
  66. Yip, V. , & Matthews, S.
    (2007) Relative clauses in Cantonese-English bilingual children: Typological challenges and processing motivations. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 29(2), 277–300. 10.1017/S0272263107070143
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263107070143 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.20011.li
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.20011.li
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error