1887
Volume 13, Issue 3
  • ISSN 1879-9264
  • E-ISSN: 1879-9272
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Previous studies have shown that bilingual children typically score more poorly on nonword repetition (NWR) tasks than monolingual peers, which has been attributed to bilinguals’ lower proficiency in the language that the NWR task is based on. To enable fairer assessments of bilingual children, Cross-Linguistic NWR tasks (CL-NWR tasks) have been developed that are based on the linguistic properties of many languages. The aim of this study is to investigate whether young children’s performance on a CL-NWR is less dependent on existing knowledge of a specific language than performance on a Language-Specific (Dutch-based) NWR (LS-NWR). Bilingual and multilingual two- and three-year-olds ( = 216) completed a CL-NWR and LS-NWR, as well as a Dutch receptive vocabulary task. Parents reported the number of languages children spoke other than Dutch. Results of linear mixed-effect regressions showed that Dutch vocabulary scores related to performance on the CL-NWR task less strongly than to performance on the LS-NWR task. The number of non-Dutch languages spoken did not differentially relate to performance on the two tasks. These findings indicate that CL-NWR tasks – at least as used here – allow for more language-neutral NWR assessments within linguistically diverse samples, already at toddler age.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lab.20050.ver
2021-08-20
2024-10-06
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Adams, A. M., & Gathercole, S. E.
    (1996) Phonological memory and spoken language development in young children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 491, 216–233.   10.1080/713755610
    https://doi.org/10.1080/713755610 [Google Scholar]
  2. Archibald, L.
    (2008) The promise of nonword repetition as a clinical tool. Canadian Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 321, 21–28.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Antonijevic-Elliott, S., Lyons, R., Pat O’ Malley, M., Meir, N., Haman, E., Banasik, N., Carroll, C., McMenamin, R., Rodden, M., & Fitzmaurice, Y.
    (2020) Language assessment of monolingual and multilingual children using non-word and sentence repetition tasks, Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 341, 293–311.   10.1080/02699206.2019.1637458
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699206.2019.1637458 [Google Scholar]
  4. Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C. & Tily, H. J.
    (2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 681, 255–278.   10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 [Google Scholar]
  5. Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S.
    (2015) Fitting linear mixed-effect models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 671,   10.18637/jss.v067.i01
    https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01 [Google Scholar]
  6. Beers, M.
    (1995) The phonology of normally developing and language-impaired children. Doctoral dissertation, University of Amsterdam.
  7. Boerma, T., Chiat, S., Leseman, P. P. M., Timmermeister, M., Wijnen, F. N. K., & Blom, W. B. T.
    (2015) A cross-linguistic nonword repetition task as a diagnostic tool for bilingual children learning Dutch as a second language. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 581, 1747–1760.   10.1044/2015_JSLHR‑L‑15‑0058
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-15-0058 [Google Scholar]
  8. Brandeker, M., & Thordardottir, E.
    (2015) Language exposure in bilingual toddlers: Performance on nonword repetition and lexical tasks. American Journal of Speech- Language Pathology, 241, 126–138.   10.1044/2015_AJSLP‑13‑0106
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_AJSLP-13-0106 [Google Scholar]
  9. Briscoe, J., Bishop, D. V. M., & Norbury, C. F.
    (2001) Phonological processing, language, and literacy: A comparison of children with mild-to-moderate sensorineural hearing loss and those with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 421, 329–340.   10.1111/1469‑7610.00726
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00726 [Google Scholar]
  10. Chiat, S.
    (2015) Nonword repetition. InS. Armon-Lotem, J. de Jong, & N. Meir (Eds.), Methods for assessing multilingual children: Disentangling bilingualism from language impairment (pp. 125–150). Multilingual Matters. 10.21832/9781783093137‑008
    https://doi.org/10.21832/9781783093137-008 [Google Scholar]
  11. Chiat, S., & Polišenská, K.
    (2016) A framework for crosslinguistic nonword repetition tests: Effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on children’s performance. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 591, 1179–1189.   10.1044/2016_JSLHR‑L‑15‑0293
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2016_JSLHR-L-15-0293 [Google Scholar]
  12. Chiat, S., & Roy, P.
    (2007) The Preschool Repetition Test: An evaluation of performance in typically developing and clinically referred children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 501, 429–443.   10.1044/1092‑4388(2007/030)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/030) [Google Scholar]
  13. Coady, J. A., & Aslin, R. N.
    (2004) Young children’s sensitivity to probabilistic phonotactics in the developing lexicon. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 891, 183–213.   10.1016/j.jecp.2004.07.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2004.07.004 [Google Scholar]
  14. Coady, J. A., & Evans, J. L.
    (2008) Uses and interpretations of non-word repetition tasks in children with and without specific language impairment (SLI). International Journal of Communication Disorders, 431, 1–40.   10.1080/13682820601116485
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820601116485 [Google Scholar]
  15. Conti-Ramsden, G., & Hesketh, A.
    (2003) Risk markers for SLI: A study of young language- learning children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 381, 251–263.   10.1080/1368282031000092339
    https://doi.org/10.1080/1368282031000092339 [Google Scholar]
  16. Crawley, M. J.
    (2012) The R book. John Wiley & Sons. 10.1002/9781118448908
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118448908 [Google Scholar]
  17. Daelemans, W., Gillis, S., & Durieux, G.
    (1994) The acquisition of stress: A data-oriented approach. Computational Linguistics, 201, 421–451.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Dispaldro, M., Leonard, L. B., Deevy, P.
    (2013) Real word and nonword repetition in Italian- Speaking children with specific language impairment: A study of diagnostic accuracy, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 561, 323–336.   10.1044/1092‑4388(2012/11‑0304)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0304) [Google Scholar]
  19. Duncan, T. S., & Paradis, J.
    (2013) English language learners’ nonword repetition performance: The influence of age, L2, vocabulary size, length of L2 exposure, and L1 phonology. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 591, 39–48.   10.1044/2015_JSLHR‑L‑14‑0020
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2015_JSLHR-L-14-0020 [Google Scholar]
  20. Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. M.
    (2005) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III-NL. Nederlandse versie door Liesbeth Schlichting [Dutch version by Liesbeth Schlichting]. Harcourt Assessment B.V.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Ebert, K. D., Pham, G., Kohnert, K.
    (2014) Lexical profiles of bilingual children with primary language impairment. Bilingualism, 171, 766–783.   10.1017/S1366728913000825
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000825 [Google Scholar]
  22. Engel de Abreu, P. M. J.
    (2011) Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect?Memory, 191, 529–537.   10.1080/09658211.2011.590504
    https://doi.org/10.1080/09658211.2011.590504 [Google Scholar]
  23. Engel de Abreu, P. M., Baldassi, M., Puglisi, M. L., & Befi-Lopes, D. M.
    (2013) Cross-linguistic and cross-cultural effects on verbal working memory and vocabulary: Testing language- minority children with an immigrant background. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 561, 630–642.   10.1044/1092‑4388(2012/12‑0079)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/12-0079) [Google Scholar]
  24. Fikkert, P.
    (1994) On the acquisition of prosodic structure. Doctoral dissertation, University of Leiden.
  25. Gathercole, S. E.
    (1995) Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or long-term knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory and Cognition, 231, 83–94.   10.3758/BF03210559
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03210559 [Google Scholar]
  26. (2006) Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 271, 513–543.   10.1017/S0142716406060383
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716406060383 [Google Scholar]
  27. Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D.
    (1989) Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 281, 200–213.   10.1016/0749‑596X(89)90044‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90044-2 [Google Scholar]
  28. Gathercole, S., & Baddeley, A.
    (1990) Phonological memory deficits in language disordered s children: Is there a causal connection?Journal of Memory and Language, 291, 336–360.   10.1016/0749‑596X(90)90004‑J
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)90004-J [Google Scholar]
  29. Gibson, T., Summers, C., Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., Gillam, R. B., & Bohman, T.
    (2015) The role of language structure and experience in bilingual children’s nonword repetition performance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. 181, 551–560.   10.1017/S1366728914000248
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728914000248 [Google Scholar]
  30. Graf-Estes, K., Evans, J. L., Else-Quest, N. M.
    (2007) Differences in the nonword repetition performance of children with and without specific language impairment: a meta-analysis. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 501, 177–195.   10.1044/1092‑4388(2007/015)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/015) [Google Scholar]
  31. Gray, S.
    (2003) Diagnostic accuracy and test-retest reliability of nonword repetition and digit span tasks administered to preschool children with specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 361, 129–151.   10.1016/S0021‑9924(03)00003‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00003-0 [Google Scholar]
  32. Grimm, A., & Schulz, P.
    (2014) Specific language impairment and early second language acquisition: The risk of over- and underdiagnosis. Child Indicators Research, 71, 821–84.   10.1007/s12187‑013‑9230‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s12187-013-9230-6 [Google Scholar]
  33. Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. F., & Simon-Cerejido, G.
    (2010) Using nonword repetition tasks for the identification of language impairment in Spanish-English-speaking children: Does the language of assessment matter?Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 251, 48–58.   10.1111/j.1540‑5826.2009.00300.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2009.00300.x [Google Scholar]
  34. Haspelmath, M.
    (2006) Against markedness (and what to replace it with). Journal of Linguistics, 421, 25–70.   10.1017/S0022226705003683
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226705003683 [Google Scholar]
  35. Hoff, E., Core, C., & Bridges, K.
    (2008) Nonword repetition assesses phonological memory and is related to vocabulary development in one-year-olds. Journal of Child Language, 351, 903–916.   10.1017/S0305000908008751
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008751 [Google Scholar]
  36. Hoff, E., Core, C., Place, S., Rumiche, R., Senor, M., & Parra, M.
    (2012) Dual language exposure and early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 391, 1–27.   10.1017/S0305000910000759
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000910000759 [Google Scholar]
  37. Jones, G.
    (2016) The influence of children’s language exposure to language from two to six years: The case of nonword repetition. Cognition, 1531, 79–88.   10.1016/j.cognition.2016.04.017
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.04.017 [Google Scholar]
  38. Kaushanskaya, M.
    (2012) Cognitive mechanisms of word learning in bilingual and monolingual adults: The role of phonological memory. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 151, 470–489.   10.1017/S1366728911000472
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000472 [Google Scholar]
  39. Keydeniers, D., Aalberse, S., Andringa, S., & Kuiken, F.
    (2021) Bilingual daycares in the Netherlands: an analysis of the implementation of bilingual input and underlying ideologies. Current Issues in Language Planning.   10.1080/14664208.2021.1939988
    https://doi.org/10.1080/14664208.2021.1939988 [Google Scholar]
  40. Kohnert, K., Windsor, J., & Yim, D.
    (2006) Do language-based processing tasks separate children with language impairment from typical bilinguals?Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 211, 19–29.   10.1111/j.1540‑5826.2006.00204.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2006.00204.x [Google Scholar]
  41. Masoura, E. V. & Gathercole, S. E.
    (1999) Phonological short-term memory and foreign language learning. International Journal of Psychology, 341, 383–388.   10.1080/002075999399738
    https://doi.org/10.1080/002075999399738 [Google Scholar]
  42. Meir, N., & Armon-Lotem, S.
    (2017) Independent and combined effects of socioeconomic status (SES) and bilingualism on children’s vocabulary and verbal short-term memory. Frontiers in Psychology, 81: 1442.   10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01442
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01442 [Google Scholar]
  43. Messer, M. H., Leseman, P. P. M., Boom, J., & Mayo, A. Y.
    (2010) Phonotactic probability effect in nonword recall and its relation with vocabulary in monolingual and bilingual preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1051, 306–323.   10.1016/j.jecp.2009.12.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.12.006 [Google Scholar]
  44. Metsala, J. L., & Chisholm, G. M.
    (2010) The influence of lexical status and neighborhood density on children’s nonword repetition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 311, 489–506. 10.1017/S0142716410000081
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716410000081 [Google Scholar]
  45. Munson, B., Kurtz, B. A., & Windsor, J.
    (2005) The influence of vocabulary size, phonotactic probability, and wordlikeness on nonword repetitions of children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 481, 1033–1047.   10.1044/1092‑4388(2005/072)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/072) [Google Scholar]
  46. Parra, M., Hoff, E., & Core, C.
    (2011) Relations among language exposure, phonological memory, and language development in Spanish-English bilingually developing 2-year- olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1081, 113–125.   10.1016/j.jecp.2010.07.011
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2010.07.011 [Google Scholar]
  47. Powell, M. J. D.
    (2009) The BOBYQA algorithm for bound constrained optimization without derivatives. Seminar scholar. https://pdfs.semanticscholar.org/0d2e/dc46f81f9a0b0b62937507ad977b46729f64.pdf?_ga=2.268128462.1175451878.1541705909-955068333.1521386318
    [Google Scholar]
  48. R Core Team
    R Core Team (2019) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. www.R-project.org/
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Rispens, J., & Baker, A.
    (2012) Nonword repetition: the relative contributions of phonological short-term memory and phonological representations in children with language and hearing impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 551, 683–694.   10.1044/1092‑4388(2011/10‑0263)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2011/10-0263) [Google Scholar]
  50. Roy, P., & Chiat, S.
    (2004) A prosodically controlled word and nonword repetition task for 2-to-4-year-olds: Evidence from typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 471, 223–234.   10.1044/1092‑4388(2004/019)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/019) [Google Scholar]
  51. Schad, D. J., Vasishth, S., Hohenstein, S., & Kliegl, R.
    (2020) How to capitalize on a priori contrasts in linear (mixed) models: A tutorial. Journal of Memory and Language, 1101, 104038.   10.1016/j.jml.2019.104038
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2019.104038 [Google Scholar]
  52. Thorn, A., & Gathercole, S. E.
    (1999) Language-specific knowledge and short-term memory in bilingual and non-bilingual children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 521, 303–324.   10.1080/713755823
    https://doi.org/10.1080/713755823 [Google Scholar]
  53. Thorn, A., Gathercole, S. E., & Frankish, C. R.
    (2005) Redintegration and the benefits of long- term knowledge in verbal short-term memory: An evaluation of Schweickert’s (1993) multinomial processing tree model. Cognitive Psychology, 501, 133–158.   10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.07.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.07.001 [Google Scholar]
  54. Toombs, M., Singh, S., & Hayden, M.
    (1981) Markedness of features in the articulatory substitutions of children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 461, 184–191. 10.1044/jshd.4602.184
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.4602.184 [Google Scholar]
  55. Unsworth, S.
    (2015) Amount of exposure as a proxy for dominance in bilingual language acquisition. InC. Silva-Corvalan & J. Treffers-Daller (Eds.), Language dominance in bilinguals: Issues of measurement and operationalization (pp. 156–173). Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9781107375345.008
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107375345.008 [Google Scholar]
  56. Verhagen, J. & Leseman, P. P. M.
    (2016) How do verbal short-term memory and working memory relate to the acquisition of vocabulary and grammar? A comparison between first and second language learners. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1411, 65–82.   10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.015
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.015 [Google Scholar]
  57. Verhagen, J., & Boom, J., Mulder, H., de Bree, E. H., & Leseman, P. P. M.
    (2019) Reciprocal relationships between nonword repetition and vocabulary during the preschool years. Developmental Psychology, 551, 1125–1137.   10.1037/dev0000702
    https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000702 [Google Scholar]
  58. White, M. J.
    (2019) The development of English proficiency and working memory in 5–6 year old ELLs in their first year of formal education, International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism.   10.1080/13670050.2019.1571009
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13670050.2019.1571009 [Google Scholar]
  59. Windsor, J., Kohnert, K., Lobitz, K. F., & Pham, G. T.
    (2010) Cross-language nonword repetition by bilingual and monolingual children. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 191, 298–310.   10.1044/1058‑0360(2010/09‑0064)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2010/09-0064) [Google Scholar]
  60. Zipf, G. K.
    (1935) (reprinted 1965). The psycho-biology of language. MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.20050.ver
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.20050.ver
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error