1887
Volume 13, Issue 6
  • ISSN 1879-9264
  • E-ISSN: 1879-9272
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

This paper investigates how late L2 learners resolve filler-gap dependencies (FGD) in real-time and how proficiency and working memory (WM) modulate their brain responses in an event-related potential (ERP) experiment. A group of intermediate to highly proficient Mandarin Chinese learners of English listened to sentences such as “The zebra that the hippo kissed *the camel on the nose ran far away,” in which the extra noun phrase “the camel” created a ‘filled-gap’ effect. The results show that although L2 behavioral responses are comparable to native speakers and are positively correlated with proficiency and WM span, the brain responses to the filled gap are qualitatively different. Importantly, L2 processing patterns did not become more nativelike with higher proficiency levels or greater WM capacity. Specifically, while the native speakers exhibited a P600 typically observed for syntactic violations and repair, the L2 group produced a prefrontal-central positivity. Similar ERPs have previously been reported to reflect domain-general attentional and non-structural-based processes, suggesting that the L2 group has a reduced sensitivity to structural requirements for gap positing in the online resolution of FGDs. Our findings are discussed in light of various proposals accounting for L1-L2 processing differences, including the Shallow Structure Hypothesis.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lab.20058.don
2022-03-02
2024-12-09
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Andersson, A., Sayehli, S., & Gullberg, M.
    (2019) Language background affects online word order processing in a second language but not offline. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(4), 802–825. 10.1017/S1366728918000573
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728918000573 [Google Scholar]
  2. Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B.
    (2012) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999999-0.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Bernstein, J. & De Jong, J. H. A. L.
    (2001) An experiment in predicting proficiency within the Common Europe Framework Level Descriptors. InY. N. Leung (Eds.), Selected Papers from the Tenth International Symposium on English Teaching (pp. 8–14). Crane Publishing.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Bernstein, J. & Cheng, J.
    (2007) Logic and validation of fully automatic spoken English test. InM. Holland & F. P. Fisher. (Eds.), The path of speech technologies in computer assisted language learning: From research toward practice (pp. 174–194). Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bowden, H. W., Steinhauer, K., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M. T.
    (2013) Native-like brain processing of syntax can be attained by university foreign language learners. Neuropsychologia, 51(13), 2492–2511. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.004 [Google Scholar]
  6. Caffarra, S., Mendoza, M., & Davidson, D.
    (2019) Is the LAN effect in morphosyntactic processing an ERP artifact?Brain and Language, 1911, 9–16. 10.1016/j.bandl.2019.01.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2019.01.003 [Google Scholar]
  7. Chomsky, N.
    (1986) Knowledge of language: Its nature, origins, and use. Greenwood Publishing Group.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Christensen, R. H. B.
    (2019) Regression Models for Ordinal Data [R package ordinal version 2019.12-10].
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Cunnings, I.
    (2012) An overview of mixed-effects statistical models for second language researchers. Second Language Research, 28(3), 369–382. 10.1177/0267658312443651
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658312443651 [Google Scholar]
  10. (2017) Interference in Native and Non-Native Sentence Processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(04), 712–721. 10.1017/S1366728916001243
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916001243 [Google Scholar]
  11. Clahsen, H., & Felser, C.
    (2006) Grammatical processing in language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(1), 3–42. 10.1017/S0142716406060024
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716406060024 [Google Scholar]
  12. (2018) Some notes on the shallow structure hypothesis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 40(3), 693–706. 10.1017/S0272263117000250
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263117000250 [Google Scholar]
  13. Clifton, C., & Frazier, L.
    (1989) Comprehending sentences with long distance dependencies. InG. M. Carlson & M. K. Tanenhaus. (Eds.), Linguistic structure in language processing (pp. 273–317). Kluwer. 10.1007/978‑94‑009‑2729‑2_8
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-2729-2_8 [Google Scholar]
  14. Dallas, A., DeDe, G., & Nicol, J.
    (2013) An Event-Related Potential (ERP) Investigation of Filler-Gap Processing in Native and Second Language Speakers. Language Learning, 63(4), 766–799. 10.1111/lang.12026
    https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12026 [Google Scholar]
  15. Daneman, M. & Carpenter, P. A.
    (1980) Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior19(4), 450–466. 10.1016/S0022‑5371(80)90312‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(80)90312-6 [Google Scholar]
  16. DeLong, K. A., & Kutas, M.
    (2020) Comprehending surprising sentences: sensitivity of post-N400 positivities to contextual congruity and semantic relatedness. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 1–20. 10.1080/23273798.2019.1708960
    https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1708960 [Google Scholar]
  17. Delorme, A., & Makeig, S.
    (2004) EEGLAB: an open-source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics, Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 1341, 9–21. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009 [Google Scholar]
  18. Dien, J.
    (2010) The ERP PCA Toolkit: An open source program for advanced statistical analysis of event-related potential data. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 187(1), 138–145. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.12.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.12.009 [Google Scholar]
  19. (2012) Applying principal components analysis to event-related potentials: a tutorial. Developmental Neuropsychology, 37(6), 497–517. 10.1080/87565641.2012.697503
    https://doi.org/10.1080/87565641.2012.697503 [Google Scholar]
  20. Dong, Z., Rhodes, R., & Hestvik, A.
    (2021) Active Gap Filling and Island Constraint in Processing the Mandarin ‘Gap-Type’ Topic Structure. Frontiers in Communication61: 650659. 10.3389/fcomm.2021.650659
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2021.650659 [Google Scholar]
  21. Dowens, M. G., Guo, T., Guo, J., Barber, H., & Carreiras, M.
    (2011) Gender and number processing in Chinese learners of Spanish–Evidence from event related potentials. Neuropsychologia, 49(7), 1651–1659. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.034
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.034 [Google Scholar]
  22. Dussias, P. E., & Piñar, P.
    (2010) Effects of reading span and plausibility in the reanalysis of wh-gaps by Chinese-English second language speakers. Second Language Research, 26(4), 443–472. 10.1177/0267658310373326
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658310373326 [Google Scholar]
  23. Felser, C.
    (2019) Structure-sensitive constraints in non-native sentence processing. Journal of the European Second Language Association, 3(1), 12–22. 10.22599/jesla.52
    https://doi.org/10.22599/jesla.52 [Google Scholar]
  24. Felser, C., & Roberts, L.
    (2007) Processing wh-dependencies in a second language: A cross-modal priming study. Second Language Research, 23(1), 9–36. 10.1177/0267658307071600
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658307071600 [Google Scholar]
  25. Foucart, A., Martin, C. D., Moreno, E. M., & Costa, A.
    (2014) Can bilinguals see it coming? Word anticipation in L2 sentence reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 40(5), 1461–1469.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Friederici, A. D.
    (2002) Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(2), 78–84. 10.1016/S1364‑6613(00)01839‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01839-8 [Google Scholar]
  27. Friederici, A. D., Hahne, A., & Saddy, D.
    (2002) Distinct neurophysiological patterns reflecting aspects of syntactic complexity and syntactic repair. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 311, 45–63. 10.1023/A:1014376204525
    https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014376204525 [Google Scholar]
  28. Frisch, S., Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D.
    (2004) Word category and verb–argument structure information in the dynamics of parsing. Cognition, 91(3), 191–219. 10.1016/j.cognition.2003.09.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.09.009 [Google Scholar]
  29. Gouvea, A. C., Phillips, C., Kazanina, N., & Poeppel, D.
    (2010) The linguistic processes underlying the P600. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25(2), 149–188. 10.1080/01690960902965951
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960902965951 [Google Scholar]
  30. Gregg, K.
    (2003) SLA theory construction and assessment. InC. Doughty & M. Long. (Eds.). Handbook of Second Language Acquisition (pp. 831–865). Oxford: Blackwell. 10.1002/9780470756492.ch23
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470756492.ch23 [Google Scholar]
  31. Hagoort, P., Brown, C. M., & Groothusen, J.
    (1993) The syntactic positive shift as an ERP measure of syntactic processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8(4), 439–483. 10.1080/01690969308407585
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690969308407585 [Google Scholar]
  32. Hahne, A., & Friederici, A. D.
    (1999) Electrophysiological evidence for two steps in syntactic analysis: Early automatic and late controlled processes. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 194–205. 10.1162/089892999563328
    https://doi.org/10.1162/089892999563328 [Google Scholar]
  33. Harrington, M., & Sawyer, M.
    (1992) L2 working memory capacity and L2 reading skill. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 14(1), 25–38. 10.1017/S0272263100010457
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100010457 [Google Scholar]
  34. Hawkins, R., & Chan, C. Y.
    (1997) The partial availability of universal grammar in second language acquisition: The “Failed functional features hypothesis.” Second Language Research, 13(3), 187–226. 10.1191/026765897671476153
    https://doi.org/10.1191/026765897671476153 [Google Scholar]
  35. Hestvik, A., Maxfield, N., Schwartz, R. G., & Shafer, V. L.
    (2007) Brain responses to filled gaps. Brain and Language, 100(3), 301–316. 10.1016/j.bandl.2006.07.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2006.07.007 [Google Scholar]
  36. Hestvik, A., Bradley, E., & Bradley, C.
    (2012) Working Memory Effects of Gap-Predictions in Normal Adults: An Event-Related Potentials Study. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 41(6), 425–438. 10.1007/s10936‑011‑9197‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-011-9197-8 [Google Scholar]
  37. Hopp, H.
    (2017) Individual differences in L2 parsing and lexical representations. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 20(4), 689–690. 10.1017/S1366728916000821
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728916000821 [Google Scholar]
  38. Horn, J. L.
    (1965) A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 30(2), 179–185. 10.1007/BF02289447
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02289447 [Google Scholar]
  39. Hsu, C. -C. N.
    (2008) Revisit relative clause islands in Chinese, Language and Linguistics, 9(1), 23–48.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Huang, J., Li, Y. A., & Li, Y.
    (2009) The Syntax of Chinese. Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9781139166935
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139166935 [Google Scholar]
  41. Jessen, A., Festman, J., Boxell, O., & Felser, C.
    (2017) Native and non-native speakers’ brain responses to filled indirect object gaps. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 46(5), 1319–1338. 10.1007/s10936‑017‑9496‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-017-9496-9 [Google Scholar]
  42. Johnson, A., Fiorentino, R., & Gabriele, A.
    (2016) Syntactic constraints and individual differences in native and non-native processing of wh-movement. Frontiers in psychology, 71, 549. 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00549
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00549 [Google Scholar]
  43. Juffs, A.
    (2006) Grammar and parsing and a transition theory. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27(1), 69–71. 10.1017/S0142716406060115
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716406060115 [Google Scholar]
  44. Kaan, E.
    (2014) Predictive sentence processing in L2 and L1: What is different?. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4(2), 257–282. 10.1075/lab.4.2.05kaa
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.4.2.05kaa [Google Scholar]
  45. Kaan, E., & Swaab, T. Y.
    (2003) Repair, revision, and complexity in syntactic analysis: An electrophysiological differentiation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(1), 98–110. 10.1162/089892903321107855
    https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903321107855 [Google Scholar]
  46. Kim, A., & Osterhout, L.
    (2005) The independence of combinatory semantic processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Memory and Language, 52(2), 205–225. 10.1016/j.jml.2004.10.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.10.002 [Google Scholar]
  47. Kizach, J.
    (2014) Analyzing Likert-scale data with mixed-effects linear models: a simulation study. Poster Presented at Linguistic Evidence. Tübingen, Germany.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Kuperberg, G. R., Kreher, D. A., Sitnikova, T., Caplan, D. N., & Holcomb, P. J.
    (2007) The role of animacy and thematic relationships in processing active English sentences: Evidence from event-related potentials. Brain and Language, 100(3), 223–237. 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2005.12.006 [Google Scholar]
  49. Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D.
    (2011) Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 621, 621–647. 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123 [Google Scholar]
  50. Liang, L., & Chen, B.
    (2014) Processing morphologically complex words in second-language learners: The effect of proficiency. Acta Psychologica, 1501, 69–79. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.04.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.04.009 [Google Scholar]
  51. Lin, Y., & Garnsey, S. M.
    (2010) Animacy and the resolution of temporary ambiguity in relative clause comprehension in Mandarin. InProcessing and producing head-final structures (pp. 241–275). Springer, Dordrecht. 10.1007/978‑90‑481‑9213‑7_12
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-9213-7_12 [Google Scholar]
  52. Luck, S. J., & Gaspelin, N.
    (2017) How to get statistically significant effects in any ERP experiment (and why you shouldn’t). Psychophysiology, 54(1), 146–157. 10.1111/psyp.12639
    https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.12639 [Google Scholar]
  53. Marinis, T., Roberts, L., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H.
    (2005) Gaps in second language sentence processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 27(1), 53–78. 10.1017/S0272263105050035
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263105050035 [Google Scholar]
  54. Martin, K. I., & Ellis, N. C.
    (2012) The roles of phonological short-term memory and working memory in L2 grammar and vocabulary learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(3), 379–413. 10.1017/S0272263112000125
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263112000125 [Google Scholar]
  55. McDonald, J. L.
    (2006) Beyond the critical period: Processing-based explanations for poor grammaticality judgment performance by late second language learners. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(3), 381–401. 10.1016/j.jml.2006.06.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.06.006 [Google Scholar]
  56. Miller, A. K.
    (2015) Intermediate Traces and Intermediate Learners: Evidence for the Use of Intermediate Structure during Sentence Processing in Second Language French. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37(3), 487–516. 10.1017/S0272263114000588
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263114000588 [Google Scholar]
  57. Morgan-Short, K., Steinhauer, K., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M. T.
    (2012) Explicit and implicit second language training differentially affect the achievement of native-like brain activation patterns. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(4), 933–947. 10.1162/jocn_a_00119
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00119 [Google Scholar]
  58. Nakano, Y., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H.
    (2002) Antecedent priming at trace positions in Japanese long-distance scrambling. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(5), 531–571. 10.1023/A:1021260920232
    https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021260920232 [Google Scholar]
  59. Nicenboim, B., Vasishth, S., Gattei, C., Sigman, M., & Kliegl, R.
    (2015) Working memory differences in long-distance dependency resolution. Frontiers in Psychology, 61, Article 312. 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00312
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00312 [Google Scholar]
  60. Ojima, S., Nakata, H., & Kakigi, R.
    (2005) An ERP study of second language learning after childhood: Effects of proficiency. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(8), 1212–1228. 10.1162/0898929055002436
    https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929055002436 [Google Scholar]
  61. Omaki, A., & Schulz, B.
    (2011) Filler-gap dependencies and island constraints in second language sentence processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(4), 563–588. 10.1017/S0272263111000313
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263111000313 [Google Scholar]
  62. Pakulak, E., & Neville, H. J.
    (2011) Maturational constraints on the recruitment of early processes for syntactic processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(10), 2752–2765. 10.1162/jocn.2010.21586
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21586 [Google Scholar]
  63. Phillips, C., & Lewis, S.
    (2013) Derivational order in syntax: Evidence and architectural consequences. Studies in Linguistics, 61, 11–47.
    [Google Scholar]
  64. R Core Team
    R Core Team (2019) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Sabourin, L., & Stowe, L. A.
    (2008) Second language processing: When are first and second languages processed similarly?Second Language Research, 24(3), 397–430. 10.1177/0267658308090186
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658308090186 [Google Scholar]
  66. Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A.
    (2002) E-Prime Reference Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools, Inc.
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Shyu, S. I.
    (1995) The syntax of focus and topic in Mandarian Chinese. (Doctoral dissertation). University of Southern California.
  68. Sprouse, J., & Almeida, D.
    (2012) Assessing the reliability of textbook data in syntax: Adger’s Core Syntax. Journal of Linguistics, 48(3), 609–652. 10.1017/S0022226712000011
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226712000011 [Google Scholar]
  69. Steinhauer, K., & Drury, J. E.
    (2012) On the early left-anterior negativity (ELAN) in syntax studies. Brain and Language, 120(2), 135–162. 10.1016/j.bandl.2011.07.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2011.07.001 [Google Scholar]
  70. Stowe, L. A.
    (1986) Parsing WH-constructions: Evidence for on-line gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1(3), 227–245. 10.1080/01690968608407062
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690968608407062 [Google Scholar]
  71. Thornhill, D. E., & Van Petten, C.
    (2012) Lexical versus conceptual anticipation during sentence processing: frontal positivity and N400 ERP components. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 83(3), 382–392. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.12.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.12.007 [Google Scholar]
  72. Tolentino, L. C., & Tokowicz, N.
    (2011) Across Language, space and time. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33(1), 91–125. 10.1017/S0272263110000549
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263110000549 [Google Scholar]
  73. Van Hell, J. G., & Tokowicz, N.
    (2010) Event-related brain potentials and second language learning: Syntactic processing in late L2 learners at different L2 proficiency levels. Second Language Research, 26(1), 43–74. 10.1177/0267658309337637
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658309337637 [Google Scholar]
  74. Weber, A., & Cutler, A.
    (2004) Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(1), 1–25. 10.1016/S0749‑596X(03)00105‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00105-0 [Google Scholar]
  75. Williams, J., Möbius, P., & Kim, C.
    (2001) Native and non-native processing of English wh-questions: Parsing strategies and plausibility constraints. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22(4), 509–540. 10.1017/S0142716401004027
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716401004027 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.20058.don
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/lab.20058.don
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error