1887
Volume 15, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1879-7865
  • E-ISSN: 1879-7873
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Activity contexts are increasingly recognized as important for the variation observed in the type and quantity of speech heard by children. However, there is little data from children’s everyday experiences to properly study this role. We analyzed the quantity of target child-directed speech (TDS) and other-directed speech (ODS) according to children’s activity contexts. We used Daylong Audio Recordings of American English children aged 6–17 months old from the Bergelson Seedlings corpus. A higher quantity of ODS per minute was observed in Solitary playful and exploratory activity contexts, while a higher quantity of TDS per minute was observed in Social activity contexts in comparison to other activity contexts. However, the interindividual variation found needs to be further explored. These results highlight the link between the language environment and activity contexts, and their importance for understanding child language development.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lia.00021.gla
2024-11-14
2024-12-12
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Akhtar, N.
    (2005) The robustness of learning through overhearing. Developmental Science, 8(2), 199–209. 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2005.00406.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00406.x [Google Scholar]
  2. Akhtar, N., Jipson, J., & Callanan, M. A.
    (2001) Learning words through overhearing. Child Development, 72(2), 416–430. 10.1111/1467‑8624.00287
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00287 [Google Scholar]
  3. Aukrust, V. G.
    (1996) Learning to talk and keep silent about everyday routines: A study of verbal interaction between young children and their caregivers. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 40(4), 311–324. 10.1080/0031383960400403
    https://doi.org/10.1080/0031383960400403 [Google Scholar]
  4. Barnes, S., Gutfreund, M., Satterly, D., & Wells, G.
    (1983) Characteristics of adult speech which predict children’s language development. Journal of Child Language, 10(1), 65–84. 10.1017/S0305000900005146
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900005146 [Google Scholar]
  5. Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J.
    (2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 [Google Scholar]
  6. Barton, M. E., & Tomasello, M.
    (1991) Joint attention and conversation in mother-infant-sibling triads. Child Development, 62(3), 517. 10.2307/1131127
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1131127 [Google Scholar]
  7. Bergelson, E.
    (2016) Bergelson seedlings HomeBank corpus. 10.21415
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Bergelson, E., Casillas, M., Soderstrom, M., Seidl, A., Warlaumont, A. S., & Amatuni, A.
    (2019) What do North American babies hear? A large-scale cross-corpus analysis. Developmental Science, 221, e12724. 10.1111/desc.12724
    https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12724 [Google Scholar]
  9. Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, M. O., & Painter, K. M.
    (1998) Sources of child vocabulary competence: A multivariate model. Journal of Child Language, 25(02), 367–393. 10.1017/S0305000998003456
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000998003456 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bruner, J. S.
    (1981) The social context of language acquisition. Language & Communication, 1(2–3), 155–178. 10.1016/0271‑5309(81)90010‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0271-5309(81)90010-0 [Google Scholar]
  11. Casillas, M., Brown, P., & Levinson, S. C.
    (2020) Early language experience in a Tseltal Mayan village. Child Development, 91(5), 819–1835. 10.1111/cdev.13349
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13349 [Google Scholar]
  12. (2021) Early language experience in a Papuan community. Journal of Child Language, 48(4), 792–814. 10.1017/S0305000920000549
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000920000549 [Google Scholar]
  13. Choi, S.
    (2000) Caregiver input in English and Korean: Use of nouns and verbs in book-reading and toy-play contexts. Journal of Child Language, 27(01), 69–96. 10.1017/S0305000999004018
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000999004018 [Google Scholar]
  14. Crain-Thoreson, C., Dahlin, M. P., & Powell, T. A.
    (2001) Parent-child interaction in three conversational contexts: Variations in style and strategy. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2001(92), 23–38. 10.1002/cd.13
    https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.13 [Google Scholar]
  15. Cristia, A., Dupoux, E., Gurven, M., & Stieglitz, J.
    (2019) Child-directed speech is infrequent in a forager-farmer population: A time allocation study. Child Development, 901, 759–773. 10.1111/cdev.12974
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12974 [Google Scholar]
  16. Dunn, J., & Shatz, M.
    (1989) Becoming a conversationalist despite (or because of ) having an older sibling. Child Development, 60(2), 399–410. 10.2307/1130985
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1130985 [Google Scholar]
  17. Ely, R., Gleason, J. B., MacGibbon, A., & Zaretsky, E.
    (2001) Attention to language: Lessons learned at the dinner table. Social Development, 10(3), 355–373. 10.1111/1467‑9507.00170
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9507.00170 [Google Scholar]
  18. Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., & Weisleder, A.
    (2013) SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16(2), 234–248. 10.1111/desc.12019
    https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12019 [Google Scholar]
  19. Floor, P., & Akhtar, N.
    (2006) Can 18-month-old infants learn words by listening in on conversations?Infancy, 9(3), 327–339. 10.1207/s15327078in0903_4
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327078in0903_4 [Google Scholar]
  20. Fouts, H. N., Roopnarine, J. L., Lamb, M. E., & Evans, M.
    (2012) Infant social interactions with multiple caregivers: The importance of ethnicity and socioeconomic status. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(2), 328–348. 10.1177/0022022110388564
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022110388564 [Google Scholar]
  21. Gampe, A., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M.
    (2012) Eighteen-month-olds learn novel words through overhearing. First Language, 32(3), 385–397. 10.1177/0142723711433584
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723711433584 [Google Scholar]
  22. Glas, L.
    (2019) Développement du lexique précoce chez des enfants français monolingues: Analyse des différences inter-individuelles via des approches complémentaires et une prise en compte des contextes de production [PhD Thesis, Lyon]. www.theses.fr/2019LYSE2098
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Glas, L., & Kern, S.
    (2015) Early vocabulary development in French monolingual children and activity types (poster). Workshop on Infant Language Development, Stockholm.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Glas, L., & Rossi, C.
    (2014) Activity contexts and early vocabulary development: A cross linguistic study (poster). Georgetown University Round Table on Languages and Linguistics, Washington D.C.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Glas, L., Rossi, C., Hamdi-Sultan, R., Batailler, C., & Bellemmouche, H.
    (2018) Activity types and child-directed speech: A comparison between French, Tunisian Arabic and English. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 63(4), 633–666. 10.1017/cnj.2018.20
    https://doi.org/10.1017/cnj.2018.20 [Google Scholar]
  26. Gleitman, L. R., Newport, E. L., & Gleitman, H.
    (1984) The current status of the motherese hypothesis. Journal of Child Language, 11(1), 43–79. 10.1017/S0305000900005584
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900005584 [Google Scholar]
  27. Greenwood, C. R., Thiemann-Bourque, K., Walker, D., Buzhardt, J., & Gilkerson, J.
    (2011) Assessing children’s home language environments using automatic speech recognition technology. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 32(2), 83–92. 10.1177/1525740110367826
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740110367826 [Google Scholar]
  28. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R.
    (1995) Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Paul H Brookes Publishing.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Hartig, F., & Lohse, L.
    (2022) DHARMa: Residual Diagnostics for Hierarchical (Multi-Level / Mixed) Regression Models (0.4.6) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=DHARMa
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Hebert-Myers, H., Guttentag, C. L., Swank, P. R., Smith, K. E., & Landry, S. H.
    (2006) The importance of language, social, and behavioral skills across early and later childhood as predictors of social competence with peers. Applied Developmental Science, 10(4), 174–187. 10.1207/s1532480xads1004_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532480xads1004_2 [Google Scholar]
  31. Hoff, E.
    (2003) The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech. Child Development, 74(5), 1368–1378. 10.1111/1467‑8624.00612
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00612 [Google Scholar]
  32. (2006) How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88. 10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2005.11.002 [Google Scholar]
  33. Hofferth, S. L.
    (1995) Caring for children at the poverty line. Children and Youth Services Review, 17(1), 61–90. 10.1016/0190‑7409(95)00004‑V
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0190-7409(95)00004-V [Google Scholar]
  34. Hoff-Ginsberg, E.
    (1991) Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Development, 62(4), 782–796. 10.2307/1131177
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1131177 [Google Scholar]
  35. Huttenlocher, J., Haight, W., Bryk, A., Seltzer, M., & Lyons, T.
    (1991) Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27(2), 236–248. 10.1037/0012‑1649.27.2.236
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.27.2.236 [Google Scholar]
  36. Johnson, C. J., Beitchman, J. H., & Brownlie, E. B.
    (2010) Twenty-year follow-up of children with and without speech-language impairments: Family, educational, occupational, and quality of life outcomes. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19(1), 51–65. 10.1044/1058‑0360(2009/08‑0083)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2009/08-0083) [Google Scholar]
  37. Kern, S.
    (2005) De l’universalité et des spécificités du développement langagier précoce. InJ. Hombert (Ed.), Aux origines des langues et du langage (pp.270–291). Fayard.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Knoeferle, P., Mishra, R. K., & Peña, M.
    (2022) Editorial: Socially situated? Effects of social and cultural context on language processing and learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 131. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.855733. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.855733
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.855733 [Google Scholar]
  39. Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B.
    (2016) lmerTest: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Lenth, R. V., Buerkner, P., Giné-Vázquez, I., Herve, M., Jung, M., Love, J., Miguez, F., Riebl, H., & Singmann, H.
    (2022) emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means (1.8.3) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Loukatou, G., Scaff, C., Demuth, K., Cristia, A., & Havron, N.
    (2022) Child-directed and overheard input from different speakers in two distinct cultures. Journal of Child Language, 49(6), 1173–1192. 10.1017/S0305000921000623
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000921000623 [Google Scholar]
  42. MacWhinney, B.
    (2009) The CHILDES project: Tools for analyzing talk (Version 3, 3rd ed.). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Masson, C.
    (2022) Parler à et avec les enfants en crèche: Conduites langagières et dialogiques des adultes et des enfants en situation de lecture partagée et de jeu libre. SHS Web of Conferences, 1381, 09008. 10.1051/shsconf/202213809008
    https://doi.org/10.1051/shsconf/202213809008 [Google Scholar]
  44. Masur, E. F.
    (1987) Imitative interchanges in a social context: Mother-infant matching behavior at the beginning of the second year. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33(4), 453–472.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. (1989) Individual and dyadic patterns of imitation: Cognitive and social aspects. InG. E. Speidel & K. E. Nelson (Eds.), The many faces of imitation in language learning (pp.53–71). Springer Science+Business Media. 10.1007/978‑1‑4612‑1011‑5_3
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-1011-5_3 [Google Scholar]
  46. Masur, E. F., & Rodemaker, J. E.
    (1999) Mothers’ and infants’ spontaneous vocal, verbal, and action imitation during the second year. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45(3), 392–412.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Montag, J. L.
    (2020) New insights from daylong audio transcripts of children’s language environments (poster). Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 421, 3004–3010.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. (2021) Temporal dynamics of words in daylong audio recordings. Reorienting the study of language learning using naturalistic data. SRCD Biennial Meeting, Online.
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Montag, J. L., Jones, M. N., & Smith, L. B.
    (2018) Quantity and diversity: Simulating early word learning environments. Cognitive Science, 42(S2), 375–412. 10.1111/cogs.12592
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12592 [Google Scholar]
  50. Newland, L. A., Roggman, L. A., & Boyce, L. K.
    (2001) The development of social toy play and language in infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 24(1), 1–25. 10.1016/S0163‑6383(01)00067‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00067-4 [Google Scholar]
  51. Ochs, E.
    (1982) Ergativity and word order in Samoan child language. Language, 58(3), 646–671. 10.2307/413852
    https://doi.org/10.2307/413852 [Google Scholar]
  52. Oller, D. K.
    (2010) All-day recordings to investigate vocabulary development: A case study of a trilingual toddler. Communication Disorders Quarterly, 31(4), 213–222. 10.1177/1525740109358628
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1525740109358628 [Google Scholar]
  53. R Core Team
    R Core Team (2018) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.r-project.org/
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Raikes, H., Alexander Pan, B., Luze, G., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Brooks-Gunn, J., Constantine, J., Banks Tarullo, L., Abigail Raikes, H., & Rodriguez, E. T.
    (2006) Mother–child bookreading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development, 77(4), 924–953. 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2006.00911.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00911.x [Google Scholar]
  55. Reese, E., & Cox, A.
    (1999) Quality of adult book reading affects children’s emergent literacy. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 20–28. 10.1037/0012‑1649.35.1.20
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.35.1.20 [Google Scholar]
  56. Rosemberg, C. R., Alam, F., Audisio, C. P., Ramirez, M. L., Garber, L., & Migdalek, M. J.
    (2020) Nouns and verbs in the linguistic environment of Argentinian toddlers: Socioeconomic and context-related differences. First Language, 40(2), 192–217. 10.1177/0142723719901226
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723719901226 [Google Scholar]
  57. Rosemberg, C. R., Alam, F., Stein, A., Migdalek, M., Menti, A., & Ojea, G.
    (2015) El entorno lingüístico de niños pequeños argentinos [Language environments of young Argentinian children]. CONICET.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Rowe, M. L.
    (2008) Child-directed speech: Relation to socioeconomic status, knowledge of child development and child vocabulary skill. Journal of Child Language, 35(1), 185–205. 10.1017/S0305000907008343
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000907008343 [Google Scholar]
  59. Roy, B. C.
    (2013) The birth of a word [Thesis (PhD)]. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Roy, B., Frank, M., & Roy, D.
    (2012) Relating activity contexts to early word learning in dense longitudinal data. 34th Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society, 935–940.
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Shneidman, L. A., Arroyo, M. E., Levine, S. C., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
    (2013) What counts as effective input for word learning?Journal of Child Language, 40(3), 672–686. 10.1017/S0305000912000141
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000912000141 [Google Scholar]
  62. Shneidman, L. A., Buresh, J. S., Shimpi, P. M., Knight-Schwarz, J., & Woodward, A. L.
    (2009) Social experience, social attention and word learning in an overhearing paradigm. Language Learning and Development, 5(4), 266-281. 10.1080/15475440903001115
    https://doi.org/10.1080/15475440903001115 [Google Scholar]
  63. Shneidman, L., & Goldin-Meadow, S.
    (2012) Language input and acquisition in a Mayan village: How important is directed speech?Developmental Science, 15(5), 659–673. 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2012.01168.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01168.x [Google Scholar]
  64. Snow, C. E., & Beals, D. E.
    (2006) Mealtime talk that supports literacy development. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 2006(111), 51–66. 10.1002/cd.155
    https://doi.org/10.1002/cd.155 [Google Scholar]
  65. Soderstrom, M., Casillas, M., Bergelson, E., Rosemberg, C., Alam, F., Warlaumont, A. S., & Bunce, J.
    (2021) Developing a cross-cultural annotation system and metacorpus for studying infants’ real world language experience. Collabra: Psychology, 7(1), 1–13. 10.1525/collabra.23445
    https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.23445 [Google Scholar]
  66. Soderstrom, M., Grauer, E., Dufault, B., & McDivitt, K.
    (2018) Influences of number of adults and adult: Child ratios on the quantity of adult language input across childcare settings. First Language, 38(6), 563–581. 10.1177/0142723718785013
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723718785013 [Google Scholar]
  67. Soderstrom, M., & Wittebolle, K.
    (2013) When do caregivers talk? The influences of activity and time of day on caregiver speech and child vocalizations in two childcare environments. PLoS ONE, 8(11), e80646. 10.1371/journal.pone.0080646
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080646 [Google Scholar]
  68. Stewart, R. B., & Marvin, R. S.
    (1984) Sibling relations: The role of conceptual perspective-taking in the ontogeny of sibling caregiving. Child Development, 55(4), 1322–1332. 10.2307/1130002
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1130002 [Google Scholar]
  69. Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Custode, S., Kuchirko, Y., Escobar, K., & Lo, T.
    (2019) Routine language: Speech directed to infants during home activities. Child Development, 901, 2135–2152. 10.1111/cdev.13089
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13089 [Google Scholar]
  70. Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Song, L.
    (2012) Parent–infant communicative interactions in cultural context. InR. M. Lerner, E. Easterbrooks, & J. Mistry (Eds.), Handbook of psychology (Second Edition, Second Edition, Vol.61, pp.143–170). John Wiley & Sons. 10.1002/9781118133880.hop206006
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118133880.hop206006 [Google Scholar]
  71. Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., Luo, R., Escobar, K., & Bornstein, M. H.
    (2017) Power in methods: Language to infants in structured and naturalistic contexts. Developmental Science, 201, e12456. 10.1111/desc.12456
    https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12456 [Google Scholar]
  72. Tomasello, M.
    (2006) First steps toward a usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cognitive Linguistics, 11(1–2), 61–82. 10.1515/cogl.2001.012
    https://doi.org/10.1515/cogl.2001.012 [Google Scholar]
  73. Tulviste, T.
    (2003) Contextual variability in interactions between mothers and 2-year-olds. First Language, 23(3), 311–325. 10.1177/01427237030233004
    https://doi.org/10.1177/01427237030233004 [Google Scholar]
  74. Vanormelingen, L., & Gillis, S.
    (2016) The influence of socio-economic status on mothers’ volubility and responsiveness in a monolingual Dutch-speaking sample. First Language, 36(2), 140–156. 10.1177/0142723716639502
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723716639502 [Google Scholar]
  75. Weisleder, A., & Fernald, A.
    (2013) Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–2152. 10.1177/0956797613488145
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613488145 [Google Scholar]
  76. Weizman, Z. O., & Snow, C. E.
    (2001) Lexical input as related to children’s vocabulary acquisition: Effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology, 37(2), 265–279. 10.1037/0012‑1649.37.2.265
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.2.265 [Google Scholar]
  77. Wittenburg, P., Brugman, H., Russel, A., Klassmann, A., & Sloetjes, H.
    (2006) ELAN: A professional framework for multimodality research, 1556–1559. https://pure.mpg.de/pub​man/faces/ViewItemOverviewPage.jsp?itemId=item_60436
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lia.00021.gla
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/lia.00021.gla
Loading

Data & Media loading...

Most Cited

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error