1887
Volume 8, Issue 2
  • ISSN 1879-7865
  • E-ISSN: 1879-7873
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Certain grammatical morphemes are variably produced in the speech of children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Previous research tends to view this as a consequence of either a deficit in linguistic knowledge or a limitation in processing capacity; however, both approaches raise problems. For example, linguistic accounts are unable to explain why these children’s problems are mostly with production rather than comprehension. Processing accounts, on the other hand, have difficulty explaining why affected children have differing levels of problems with grammatical morphemes that are similar on the surface (e.g. English plural - vs. third person singular -). In this paper, a new, phonological account is proposed which avoids these problems, and better captures the wide array of data presented in the literature. It is proposed that children with SLI have problems with organizing segmental data into prosodic structures that are linguistically highly marked, in particular those that involve various forms of extraprosodicity.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lia.15029.ozc
2018-03-02
2025-04-19
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Alers-Valentín, H.
    (2000) The prosodic structure of the Spanish verb: Evolution and configuration. Unpublished PhD dissertation. University of Massachusetts.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Árnason, K.
    (1985) Icelandic word stress and metrical phonology. Studia Linguistica, 39, 93–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9582.1985.tb00747.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9582.1985.tb00747.x [Google Scholar]
  3. Bat-El, O.
    (1993) Parasitic metrification in the modern Hebrew stress system. The Linguistic Review, 10, 189–210. doi: 10.1515/tlir.1993.10.3.189
    https://doi.org/10.1515/tlir.1993.10.3.189 [Google Scholar]
  4. Becker, M.
    (2003) Hebrew stress: Can’t you hear those trochees?University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics, 9, 45–57.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bedore, L. , & Leonard, L.
    (2001) Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 905–924. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2001/072)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/072) [Google Scholar]
  6. (2005) Verb inflections and noun phrase morphology in the spontaneous speech of Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 26, 195–225. doi: 10.1017/S0142716405050149
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716405050149 [Google Scholar]
  7. Bortolini, U. , Caselli, M. C. , & Leonard, L.
    (1997) Grammatical deficits in Italian-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research40, 809–820. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4004.809
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4004.809 [Google Scholar]
  8. Bortolini, U. , Leonard, L. , & Caselli, M. C.
    (1998) Specific language impairment in Italian and English: Evaluating alternative accounts of grammatical deficits. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13, 1–20. doi: 10.1080/016909698386573
    https://doi.org/10.1080/016909698386573 [Google Scholar]
  9. Bishop, D. V.
    (2002) The role of genes in the etiology of specific language impairment. Journal of Communication Disorders, 35, 311–328. doi: 10.1016/S0021‑9924(02)00087‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(02)00087-4 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bloom, L. , & Lahey, M.
    (1978) Language development and language disorders. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Bortolini, U. , & Leonard, L.
    (1996) Phonology and grammatical morphology in specific language impairment: Accounting for individual variation in English and Italian. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 85–104. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400009474
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400009474 [Google Scholar]
  12. Bortolini, U. , Caselli, M. C. , & Leonard, L.
    (1997) Grammatical deficits in Italian-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 809–820. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4004.809
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4004.809 [Google Scholar]
  13. Charette, M.
    (1991) Conditions on phonological government. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511554339
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511554339 [Google Scholar]
  14. Cheung, H.
    (2009) Grammatical characteristics of Mandarin-speaking children with Specific Language Impairment. In S. Law , B. Weekes , & A. M-Y. Wong (Eds.), Language disorders in speakers of Chinese (pp.19–32). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Chiat, S.
    (2001) Mapping theories of developmental language impairment: Premises predictions and evidence. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 113–142. doi: 10.1080/01690960042000012
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960042000012 [Google Scholar]
  16. Chomsky, N.
    (1981) Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. (1995) The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Clahsen, H.
    (1989) The grammatical characterization of developmental dysphasia. Linguistics, 27, 897–920. doi: 10.1515/ling.1989.27.5.897
    https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1989.27.5.897 [Google Scholar]
  19. Clahsen, H. , Bartke, S. , & Göllner, S.
    (1997) Formal features in impaired grammars: A comparison of English and German SLI children. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 10, 151–171. doi: 10.1016/S0911‑6044(97)00006‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0911-6044(97)00006-7 [Google Scholar]
  20. Clahsen, H. , & Dalalakis, J.
    (1999) Tense and agreement in Greek SLI: A case study. Essex Research Reports in Linguistics, 24, 1–25.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Clahsen, H. , & Rothweiler, M.
    (1993) Inflectional rules in children’s grammars: evidence from the development of participles in German. Yearbook of Morphology 1992, 1–34.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Demuth, K.
    (2001) Prosodic constraints on morphological development. In J. Weissenborn & B. Höhle (Eds.), Approaches to bootstrapping: Phonological, dyntactic and neurophysiological aspects of early language acquisition (pp.3–21). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/lald.24.02dem
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.24.02dem [Google Scholar]
  23. (2006) Crosslinguistic perspectives on the development of prosodic words. Special issue ofLanguage and Speech, 49(2), 129–297. doi: 10.1177/00238309060490020101
    https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309060490020101 [Google Scholar]
  24. (2007) Acquisition at the prosody-morphology interface. In A. Belikova et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of GALANA 2 (pp.84–91). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Domahs, U. , Lohmann, K. , Moritz, N. , & Kauschke, C.
    (2013) The acquisition of prosodic constraints on derivational morphology in typically developing children and children with SLI. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 27, 855–573. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2013.796405
    https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2013.796405 [Google Scholar]
  26. Dresher, E. , & Kaye, J.
    (1990) A computational learning model for metrical phonology. Cognition, 34, 137–195. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0277(90)90042‑I
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90042-I [Google Scholar]
  27. Dromi, E. , Leonard, L. , Adam, G. , & Zadunaisky-Ehrlich, S.
    (1999) Verb agreement morphology in Hebrew-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 1414–1431. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4206.1414
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4206.1414 [Google Scholar]
  28. Dromi, E. , Leonard, L. , & Shteiman, M.
    (1993) The grammatical morphology of Hebrew speaking children with specific language impairment: Some competing hypothesis. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 760–771. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3604.760
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3604.760 [Google Scholar]
  29. Eisenbeiss, S. , Bartke, S. , & Clahsen, H.
    (2006) Structural and lexical case in child German: Evidence from language-impaired and typically-developing children. Language Acquisition, 13, 3–32. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la1301_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la1301_2 [Google Scholar]
  30. Fletcher, P. , Leonard, L. , Wong, A. , & Stokes, S.
    (2005) The expression of aspect in Cantonese-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 48, 621–634. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2005/043)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/043) [Google Scholar]
  31. Frid, J.
    (2001) Swedish word stress in optimality theory. Lund University Department of Linguistics Working Papers48, 1–16.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Gathercole, S.
    (2006) Nonword repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 513–543.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Gathercole, S. , & Baddeley, A.
    (1990) Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: is there a causal connection?Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360. doi: 10.1016/0749‑596X(90)90004‑J
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)90004-J [Google Scholar]
  34. (1993) Working memory and language. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Gerken, L. A.
    (1996) Prosodic structure in young children’s language production. Language72, 683–712. doi: 10.2307/416099
    https://doi.org/10.2307/416099 [Google Scholar]
  36. Gerken, L. A. , & McGregor, K.
    (1998) An overview of prosody and its role in normal and disordered child language. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 7, 38–48. doi: 10.1044/1058‑0360.0702.38
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360.0702.38 [Google Scholar]
  37. Goad, H.
    (1998) Plurals in SLI: Prosodic deficit or morphological deficit?Language Acquisition, 7, 247–284. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0702‑4_6
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0702-4_6 [Google Scholar]
  38. Goad, H. , & Buckley, M.
    (2006) Prosodic structure in child French: Evidence for the foot. Catalan Journal of Linguistics, 5, 109–142.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Goad, H. , & Rebellati, C.
    (1994) Pluralization in familial language impairment. McGill Working Papers in Linguistics, 10, 24–40.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Goad, H. , & White, L.
    (2004) Ultimate attainment of L2 inflection: Effects of L1 prosodic structure. In S. Foster-Cohen , M. Sharwood Smith , A. Sorace & M. Ota (eds.), EUROSLA Yearbook (Vol.4; pp.119–145). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Goad, H. & White, L.
    (2006) Ultimate attainment in interlanguage grammars: A prosodic approach. Second Language Research, 22, 243–268. doi: 10.1191/0267658306sr268oa
    https://doi.org/10.1191/0267658306sr268oa [Google Scholar]
  42. Goad, H. , & White, L.
    (2008) Prosodic structure and the representation of L2 functional morphology: A nativist approach. Lingua, 118, 577–594. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2007.01.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2007.01.008 [Google Scholar]
  43. (2009) Prosodic transfer and the representation of determiners in Turkish- English interlanguage. In N. Snape , Y.-k. I. Leung & M. Sharwood-Smith (Eds.), Representational deficits in SLA: Studies in honor of Roger Hawkins (pp.1–26). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/lald.47.04goa
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.47.04goa [Google Scholar]
  44. (2009) Articles in Turkish/English interlanguage revisited: Implications of vowel harmony. In P. García-Mayo & R. Hawkins (Eds.), Second language acquisition of articles: Empirical findings and theoretical implications (pp.201–232). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/lald.49.13goa
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.49.13goa [Google Scholar]
  45. Goad, H. , White, L. , & Steele, J.
    (2003) Missing inflection in L2 acquisition: Defective syntax or L1- constrained prosodic representations?Canadian Journal of Linguistics48, 243–263. doi: 10.1353/cjl.2004.0027
    https://doi.org/10.1353/cjl.2004.0027 [Google Scholar]
  46. Gopnik, M.
    (1990) Genetic basis of grammar defect. Nature, 347, 26.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Gopnik, M. , & Crago, M.
    (1991) Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition39, 1–50. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0277(91)90058‑C
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(91)90058-C [Google Scholar]
  48. Grijzenhout, J. , & Penke, M.
    (2005) On the interaction of phonology and morphology in language acquisition and German and Dutch Broca’s Aphasia: A case of inflected verbs. Yearbook of Morphology, 49–81. doi: 10.1007/1‑4020‑4066‑0_3
    https://doi.org/10.1007/1-4020-4066-0_3 [Google Scholar]
  49. Grinstead, J. , Baron, A. , Vega-Mendoza, M. , De la Mora, J. , Cantú-Sánchez, M. , & Flores, B.
    (2013) Tense marking and spontaneous speech measures in Spanish SLI: A discriminant function analysis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 352–363. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2012/11‑0289)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0289) [Google Scholar]
  50. Halle, M. , & Vergnaud, J. R.
    (1980) Three dimensional phonology. Journal of Linguistic Research1, 83–105.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Hamann, C.
    (2004) Comparing the development of the nominal and the verbal functional domain in French language impairment. In P. Prévost & J. Paradis , (eds.), The acquisition of French in different contexts: Focus on functional categories (pp.109–144). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/lald.32.07ham
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.32.07ham [Google Scholar]
  52. Hansson, K. , Nettelbladt, U. , & Leonard, L.
    (2003) Indefinite articles and definite forms in Swedish children with specific language impairment. First Language, 23, 343–362. doi: 10.1177/01427237030233005
    https://doi.org/10.1177/01427237030233005 [Google Scholar]
  53. Harris, J.
    (1983) Syllable structure and stress in Spanish: A nonlinear analysis (Linguistic Inquiry Monograph 8). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  54. (1989) The stress erasure convention and cliticization in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry, 20, 339–363.
    [Google Scholar]
  55. Hayes, B.
    (1995) Metrical stress theory: Principles and case studies. Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Hsieh, L. , Leonard, L. B. , & Swanson, L. A.
    (1999) Some differences between English noun inflections and third singular verb inflections in the input: The contribution of fluency, sentence position, and duration. Journal of Child Language, 26, 531–543. doi: 10.1017/S030500099900392X
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500099900392X [Google Scholar]
  57. Hualde, J.
    (2005) The sounds of Spanish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Huang, C. -T. J.
    (1982) Logical relations in Chinese and the theory of grammar. Unpublished PhD dissertation. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Inkelas, Sharon
    (1989) Prosodic constituency in the lexicon. Unpublished PhD dissertation. Stanford University.
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Jakubowicz, C. , & Nash, L.
    (2001) Functional categories and syntactic operations in (Ab)normal language acquisition. Brain and Language, 77, 321–339. doi: 10.1006/brln.2000.2405
    https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2000.2405 [Google Scholar]
  61. Joanisse, M. , & Seidenberg, M.
    (1998) Specific language impairment: A deficit in grammar or processing?Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 240–247. doi: 10.1016/S1364‑6613(98)01186‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01186-3 [Google Scholar]
  62. Kager, R.
    (1999) Optimality theory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511812408
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511812408 [Google Scholar]
  63. Kail, R.
    (1994) A method for studying the generalized slowing hypothesis in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 418–421. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3702.418
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3702.418 [Google Scholar]
  64. Kauschke, C. , Kurth, A. , & Domahs, U.
    (2011) Acquisition of German noun plurals in typically developing children and children with specific language impairment. Child Development Research2011, 1–7. doi: 10.1155/2011/718925
    https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/718925 [Google Scholar]
  65. Kauschke, C. , Renner, L. , & Domahs, U.
    (2013) Prosodic constraints on inflected words: An area of difficulty for German-speaking children with specific language impairment?Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 27, 574–593. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2013.798033
    https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2013.798033 [Google Scholar]
  66. Kornfilt, J.
    (1997) Turkish. London & New York: Routledge.
  67. Lahey, M. , Liebergott, J. , Chesnick, M. , Menyuk, P. , & Adams, J.
    (1992) Variability in children’s use of grammatical morphemes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13, 373–398. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400005683
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400005683 [Google Scholar]
  68. Lai, C. S. , Fisher, S. E. , Hurst, J. A. , Vargha-Khadem, F. , & Monaco, A. P.
    (2001) A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature, 419, 519–523. doi: 10.1038/35097076
    https://doi.org/10.1038/35097076 [Google Scholar]
  69. Leonard, L.
    (1989) Language learnability and specific language impairment in children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 10, 179–202. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400008511
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400008511 [Google Scholar]
  70. (1995) Functional categories in the grammars of children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 1270–1283. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3806.1270
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3806.1270 [Google Scholar]
  71. (2000a/2014a) Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  72. (2000b) Specific language impairment across languages. In D. Bishop & L. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp.115–129). Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  73. (2014b) Specific language impairment across languages. Child Development Perspectives, 8, 1–5. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12053
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12053 [Google Scholar]
  74. Leonard, L. , & Bortolini, U.
    (1998) Grammatical morphology and the role of weak syllables in the speech of Italian-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 1363–1374. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4106.1363
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4106.1363 [Google Scholar]
  75. Leonard, L. , Bortolini, U. , Caselli, M. C. , & McGregor, K. K.
    (1992) Morphological deficits in children with specific language impairment: The status of features in the underlying grammar. Language Acquisition, 2, 151–179. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0202_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0202_2 [Google Scholar]
  76. Leonard, L. , Deevy, P. , Fey, M. , & Bredin-Oja, S.
    (2013) Sentence comprehension in specific language impairment: A task designed to distinguish between cognitive capacity and syntactic complexity. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 577–589. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2012/11‑0254)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0254) [Google Scholar]
  77. Leonard, L. B. , & Dromi, E.
    (1994) The use of Hebrew verb morphology by children with specific language impairment and children developing language normally. First Language, 14, 283–304.
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Leonard, L. , & Eyer, J.
    (1996) Deficits of grammatical morphology in children with specific language impairment and their implications for notions of bootstrapping. In J. Morgan & K. Demuth (Eds.), Signal to Syntax (pp.233–248). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Leonard, L. , Eyer, J. M. , Bedore, L. M. , & Grela, B. G.
    (1997) Three accounts of the grammatical morpheme difficulties of English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 40, 741–753. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4004.741
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4004.741 [Google Scholar]
  80. Leonard, L. , McGregor, K. , & Allen, G.
    (1992) Grammatical morphology and speech perception in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 1076–1085. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3505.1076
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3505.1076 [Google Scholar]
  81. Li, Y. -H. A.
    (1985) Abstract case in Chinese. Unpublished PhD dissertation. University of Southern California.
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Lukács, Á. , Leonard, L. , & Kas, B.
    (2010) The use of noun morphology by children with language impairment: The case of Hungarian. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 45, 145–161. doi: 10.3109/13682820902781060
    https://doi.org/10.3109/13682820902781060 [Google Scholar]
  83. Lukács, Á. , Leonard, L. , Kas, B. , & Pléh, C.
    (2009) The use of tense and agreement by Hungarian-speaking children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 52, 98–117. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2008/07‑0183)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0183) [Google Scholar]
  84. Maillart, C. , & Schelstraete, M. A.
    (2003) Sentence processing strategies in French-speaking children with SLI: A study of morphosyntactic cues. Journal of Multilingual Communication Disorders, 1, 201–209. doi: 10.1080/1476967031000090999
    https://doi.org/10.1080/1476967031000090999 [Google Scholar]
  85. Marchman, V. , Wulfeck, B. , & Ellis Weismer, S.
    (1999) Productivity of past tense in children with normal language and specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 42, 206–219. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4201.206
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4201.206 [Google Scholar]
  86. Marinis, T. , & van der Lely, H.
    (2007) Processing of wh-questions in children with G-SLI and typically developing children. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 42, 557–582. doi: 10.1080/13682820601058190
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820601058190 [Google Scholar]
  87. Marshall, C. R. , & van der Lely, H. K. J.
    (2006) A challenge to current models of past tense inflection: The impact of phonotactics. Cognition, 100, 302–320. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.06.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.06.001 [Google Scholar]
  88. (2007) The impact of phonological complexity on past tense inflection in children with Grammatical-SLI. Advances in Speech-Language Pathology, 9, 191–203. doi: 10.1080/14417040701261509
    https://doi.org/10.1080/14417040701261509 [Google Scholar]
  89. (2012) Irregular past tense forms in English: How data from children with specific language impairment contribute to models of morphology. Morphology, 22, 121–141. doi: 10.1007/s11525‑011‑9195‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-011-9195-4 [Google Scholar]
  90. McCarthy, J. , & Prince, A.
    (1986) Prosodic Morphology. Ms. University of Massachusetts and Brandeis University.
    [Google Scholar]
  91. (1998) Prosodic morphology. In A. Spencer & A. M. Zwicky (Eds.), The handbook of morphology (pp.283–305). Oxford: Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  92. (2001) Prosodic morphology: Constraint interaction and satisfaction. Ms. [ROA #482], Rutgers University.
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Menyuk, P.
    (1964) Comparisons of grammar of children with functionally deviant and normally developing speech. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 7, 109–121. doi: 10.1044/jshr.0702.109
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.0702.109 [Google Scholar]
  94. Morales, A.
    (2014) Production and comprehension of verb agreement morphology in Spanish and English child L2 learners: Evidence for the effects of morphological structure. Unpublished PhD dissertation. University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign.
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Nespor, M.
    (1993) Fonologia. Bologna: Il Mulino.
    [Google Scholar]
  96. Nespor, M. , & Vogel, I.
    (1986) Prosodic phonology. Dordrecht: Foris.
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Núñez Cedeño, R. A. , & Morales-Front, A.
    (1999) Fonología generativa contemporánea de la lengua española. Washington DC: Georgetown University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  98. Oetting, J.
    (1992) Language-impaired and normally developing children’s acquisition of English plurals. Unpublished PhD dissertation. University of Kansas, Lawrence, Kansas.
    [Google Scholar]
  99. Oetting, J. , & Horohov, J.
    (1997) Past tense marking by children with and without specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 62–74. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4001.62
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4001.62 [Google Scholar]
  100. Oetting, J. , & Rice, M.
    (1993) Plural acquisition in children with specific language impairments. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1236–1248. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3606.1236
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3606.1236 [Google Scholar]
  101. Özçelik, Ö.
    (2014) Prosodic faithfulness to foot edges: The case of Turkish stress. Phonology, 31, 229–269. doi: 10.1017/S0952675714000128
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675714000128 [Google Scholar]
  102. (2017) The foot is not an obligatory constituent of the prosodic hierarchy: “Stress” in Turkish, French and child English. The Linguistic Review, 34, 157–213.
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Paradis, J. , Crago, M. , & Genesee, F.
    (2003) Object clitics as a clinical marker of SLI in French: Evidence from French-English bilingual children. In B. Beachley et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp.638–649). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  104. (2005) Domain-specific versus domain-general theories of the deficit in SLI: Object pronoun acquisition by French–English bilingual children. Language Acquisition13, 33–62.
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Peppé, S.
    (2009) Why is prosody in speech-language pathology so difficult?International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 258–271. doi: 10.1080/17549500902906339
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17549500902906339 [Google Scholar]
  106. Peppé, S. , & McCann, J.
    (2003) Assessing intonation and prosody in children with atypical language development: The PEPS-C test and the revised version. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 17, 345–354. doi: 10.1080/0269920031000079994
    https://doi.org/10.1080/0269920031000079994 [Google Scholar]
  107. Piggott, G. , & Kessler Robb, M.
    (1999) Prosodic features of familial language impairment: constraints on stress assignment. Folia Phoniatrica Logopaedica, 51: 55–69. doi: 10.1159/000021481
    https://doi.org/10.1159/000021481 [Google Scholar]
  108. Prince, A. , & Smolensky, P.
    (1993 [2002/2004]) Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Oxford: Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Rice, M. , & Oetting, J.
    (1993) Morphological deficits on children with SLI: Evaluation of number marking and agreement. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1249–1257. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3606.1249
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3606.1249 [Google Scholar]
  110. Rice, M. , & Wexler, K.
    (1996) Toward tense as a clinical marker for specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1239–1257. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3906.1239
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3906.1239 [Google Scholar]
  111. Rice, M. , Wexler, K. , & Cleave, P.
    (1995) Specific language impairment as a period of extended optional infinitive. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 38, 850–863. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3804.850
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3804.850 [Google Scholar]
  112. Rice, M. , Wexler, K. , & Redmond, S. M.
    (1999) Grammaticality judgments of an extended optional infinitive grammar: Evidence from English-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 42, 943–961. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4204.943
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4204.943 [Google Scholar]
  113. Schütze, C. , & Wexler, K.
    (1996) Subject case licensing and English root infinitives. In A. Stringfellow , D. Cahana-Amitay , E. Hughes , & A. Zukowski (eds.), Proceedings of the 20th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp.670–681). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  114. Scullen, M. E.
    (1997) French prosodic morphology: A unified account. Bloomington, IN: IULC.
    [Google Scholar]
  115. Selkirk, E.
    (1980) The role of prosodic categories in English word stress. Linguistic Inquiry, 11, 563–605.
    [Google Scholar]
  116. (1984) Phonology and syntax: The relation between sound and structure. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  117. (1986) On derived domains in sentence phonology. Phonology, 3, 371–405. doi: 10.1017/S0952675700000695
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675700000695 [Google Scholar]
  118. (1995) The prosodic structure of function words. In J. Beckman , L. W. Dickey & S. Urbanczyk (Eds.), Papers in optimality theory (pp.439–70). Amherst, MA: GLSA.
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Slobin, D.
    (1985) Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In D. Slobin (ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition. Vol. 2: Theoretical Issues (pp.1157–1256). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Setter, J. E. , & Stojanovic, V.
    (2013) Prosody in typical and atypical populations. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 27, 853–854. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2013.800581
    https://doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2013.800581 [Google Scholar]
  121. Stojanovic, V. , & Setter, J. E.
    (2009) Conditions in which prosodic impairments occur. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 11, 293–297. doi: 10.1080/17549500902943647
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17549500902943647 [Google Scholar]
  122. Tallal, P. , & Ross, R. , & Curtiss, S.
    (1989) Familial aggregation in specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 167–173. doi: 10.1044/jshd.5402.167
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.5402.167 [Google Scholar]
  123. Tallal, P. , & Stark, R.
    (1981) Speech acoustic-cue discrimination abilities of normally developing and language-impaired children. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 69, 568–574. doi: 10.1121/1.385431
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.385431 [Google Scholar]
  124. Tang, C. -C. J.
    (1990) Chinese phrase structure and the extended X’-theory. Unpublished PhD dissertation. Cornell University.
    [Google Scholar]
  125. Thordardottir, E.
    (2008) Language specific effects of task demands on the manifestation of specific language impairment: A comparison of English and Icelandic. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 51, 922–937. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2008/068)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/068) [Google Scholar]
  126. Thordardottir, E. , & Namazi, M.
    (2007) Specific language impairment in French-speaking children: Beyond grammatical morphology. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 50, 698–715. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2007/049)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2007/049) [Google Scholar]
  127. Tremblay, A. , & Demuth, K.
    (2007) Prosodic licensing of determiners in children’s early French. In A. Belikova et al. (Eds.), Proceedings of GALANA 2 (pp.426–436). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  128. Van der Lely, H.
    (1994) Canonical linking rules: Forward vs. reverse linking in normally developing and specifically language impaired children. Cognition, 51, 29–72. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0277(94)90008‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90008-6 [Google Scholar]
  129. (1998) SLI in children: Movement, economy, and deficits in the computational-syntactic system. Language Acquisition, 7, 161–192. doi: 10.1207/s15327817la0702‑4_4
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la0702-4_4 [Google Scholar]
  130. (2005) Domain-specific cognitive systems: Insight from Grammatical SLI. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 53–59. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  131. Van der Lely, H. , & Battell, J.
    (2003) Wh-movement in children with Grammatical SLI: A test of the RDDR hypothesis. Language, 79, 153–181. doi: 10.1353/lan.2003.0089
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2003.0089 [Google Scholar]
  132. Van der Lely, H. , & Stollwerck, L.
    (1997) Binding theory and grammatical specific language impairment in children. Cognition, 62, 245–290. doi: 10.1016/S0010‑0277(96)00783‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(96)00783-4 [Google Scholar]
  133. Van der Lely, H. , & Ullman, M.
    (2001) Past tense morphology in specifically language impaired children and normally developing children. Language and Cognitive Processes, 16, 177–217. doi: 10.1080/01690960042000076
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960042000076 [Google Scholar]
  134. Van der Meulen, S. , Janssen, P. , & Den Os, E.
    (1997) Prosodic abilities in children with specific language impairment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 30, 155–170. doi: 10.1016/S0021‑9924(96)00059‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(96)00059-7 [Google Scholar]
  135. Yarbay Duman, T. , & Topbaş, S.
    (2016) Epistemic uncertainty: Turkish children with specific language impairment and their comprehension of tense and aspect. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 51, 732–744.
    [Google Scholar]
  136. Wexler, K.
    (1994) Optional infinitives, head movement and the economy of derivations in child grammar. In D. Lightfoot & N. Hornstein (Eds.) Verb movement (pp.305–350). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511627705.016
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511627705.016 [Google Scholar]
  137. (1998) Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage. Lingua, 106, 23–79. doi: 10.1016/S0024‑3841(98)00029‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00029-1 [Google Scholar]
  138. (2004) The unique checking constraint as the explanation of clitic omission in SLI and normal development. Presented at a2002 Conference on SLI organized by C. Jakubowicz. Ms., MIT. Presented with new comments as Wexler2014.
    [Google Scholar]
  139. (2014) The unique checking constraint as the explanation of clitic omission in SLI and normal development. In K. K. Grohmann & T. Neokleous (Eds.) Developments in the acquisition of clitics (pp.292–344), Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lia.15029.ozc
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/lia.15029.ozc
Loading

Data & Media loading...

Most Cited

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error