1887
Volume 14, Issue 2
  • ISSN 1879-7865
  • E-ISSN: 1879-7873
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

This study examines the effect of gestures on second language (L2) pitch measures and fluency in storytelling tasks. It is well documented that L2 learners commonly use gestures in conversations and storytelling. Research has shown that gestures benefit L2 word learning and recall, but it is still unclear to what extent gestures affect speech fluency and pitch range. We compared a series of speech fluency and pitch measures across two storytelling tasks: task repetition and explicit instruction on the use of gestures. By analysing the use of gestures in these tasks, we provide insights on how gestures can help L2 fluency and intonation.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lia.22023.cav
2024-03-22
2024-10-12
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Ahmadian, M. J., & Tavakoli, M.
    (2011) The effects of simultaneous use of careful online planning and task repetition on accuracy, fluency, and complexity of EFL learners’ oral production. Language Teaching Research, 151, 35–59. 10.1177/1362168810383329
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168810383329 [Google Scholar]
  2. Alibali, M. W., Kita, S., & Young, A. J.
    (2000) Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(6), 593–613. 10.1080/016909600750040571
    https://doi.org/10.1080/016909600750040571 [Google Scholar]
  3. Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J.
    (2001) Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 441, 169–188. 10.1006/jmla.2000.2752
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2752 [Google Scholar]
  4. Andreeva, B., & Dimitrova, S.
    (2022) Prosodic characteristics of Bulgarian-accented German: Prosodic units and prenuclear pitch accents. Languages, 71, 1–18. 10.3390/languages7040263
    https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7040263 [Google Scholar]
  5. Andreeva, B., Demenko, G., Wolska, M., Möbius, B., Zimmerer, F., Jügler, J., & Trouvain, J.
    (2014) Comparison of pitch range and pitch variation in Slavic and German languages. InN. Campbell, D. Gibbon, & D. Hirst (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th international conference on speech prosody (pp. 776–780). Retrieved fromhttps://www.isca-speech.org/archive_v0/SpeechProsody_2014/
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Arevart, S., & Nation, P.
    (1993) Adjusting fluency and grammar through repetition. InJ. Oller (Ed.), Methods that work: Ideas for language teachers (pp. 298–308). Heinle & Heinle.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J.
    (2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 681, 255–278. 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 [Google Scholar]
  8. Blomgren, M., Chen, Y., Ng, M. L., & Gilbert, H. R.
    (1998) Acoustic, aerodynamic, physiologic, and perceptual properties of modal and vocal fry registers. Journal of Acoustic Society of America, 103(5), 2649–2658. 10.1121/1.422785
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.422785 [Google Scholar]
  9. Boersma, P., & Weenink, D.
    (2018) Praat: Doing phonetics by computer [Computer program]. Version 6.0.37. Retrieved on March14 2018fromwww.praat.org/
  10. Bosker, H., Pinget, A., Quené, H., Sanders, T., & de Jong, N.
    (2013) What makes speech sound fluent? The contributions of pauses, speed and repairs. Language Testing, 301, 159–175. 10.1177/0265532212455394
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0265532212455394 [Google Scholar]
  11. Bretz, F., Hothorn, T., & Westfall, P.
    (2010) Multiple comparisons using R. CRC Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Bruin, J.
    (2014) R library: Contrast coding systems for categorical variables. Retrieved fromhttps://stats.idre.ucla.edu/r/library/r-library-contrast-coding-systems-for-categorical-variables/
  13. Busà, M. G.
    (2007) New perspectives in teaching pronunciation. InA. Baldry, M. Pavesi, C. Taylor Torsello, & C. Taylor (Eds.), From DIDACTAS to eCoLingua: An ongoing research project on translation and corpus linguistics (pp. 171–188). EUT.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Busà, M. G., & Urbani, M.
    (2011) A cross linguistic analysis of pitch range in English L1 and L2. InW. S. Lee & E. Zee (Eds.), Proceedings of 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 380–383). Retrieved fromhttps://www.internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2011/index.htm
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Butterworth, B.
    (1975) Hesitation and semantic planning in speech. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 41, 75–87. 10.1007/BF01066991
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01066991 [Google Scholar]
  16. Bygate, M.
    (2001) Effects of task repetition on the structure and control of oral language. InM. Bygate, P. Skehan, & M. Swain (Eds.), Researching pedagogic tasks (pp. 23–48). Harlow Longman.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Calbris, G.
    (2008) From left to right…: Coverbal gestures and their symbolic use of space. InA. Cienki & C. Müller (Eds.), Metaphor and gesture (pp. 27–53). John Benjamins.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Cavicchio, F., & Poesio, M.
    (2009) Multimodal Corpora Annotation: Validation Methods to Assess Coding Scheme Reliability. InM. Kipp, J.-C. Martin, P. Paggio, and D. Heylen (Eds.), Multimodal CorporaLNAI 5509, Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 109–121.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Cienki, A., & Müller, C.
    (Eds.) (2008) Metaphor and gesture. John Benjamins.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Chu, M., Meyer, A., Foulkes, L., & Kita, S.
    (2014) Individual differences in frequency and saliency of speech-accompanying gestures: The role of cognitive abilities and empathy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 1431, 694–709. 10.1037/a0033861
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033861 [Google Scholar]
  21. Cravotta, A., Prieto, P., & Busà, M. G.
    (2021) Exploring the effects of encouraging the use of gestures on speech. Speech Communication, 1351, 25–36. 10.1016/j.specom.2021.09.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2021.09.005 [Google Scholar]
  22. Cruttenden, A.
    (1986) Intonation. Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. de Bot, K.
    (1992) A bilingual production model: Levelt’s speaking model adapted. Applied Linguistics, 131, 1–24.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. De Jong, N. H., Steinel, M. P., Florijn, A. F., Schoonen, R., & Hulstijn, J. H.
    (2012) Linguistic skills and speaking fluency in a L2. Applied Psycholinguistics, 331, 1–24.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Doughty, C. J.
    (2005) Instructed SLA: Constraints, compensation, and enhancement. InC. J. Doughty, & M. H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 1–39). John Wiley & Sons.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Dörnyei, Z., & Kormos, J.
    (1998) Problem-solving mechanisms in L2 communication: a psycholinguistic perspective. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 20(3), 349–385. 10.1017/S0272263198003039
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263198003039 [Google Scholar]
  27. Ellis, R.
    (2005) Measuring implicit and explicit knowledge of a second language: a psychometric Study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 271, 141–172. 10.1017/S0272263105050096
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263105050096 [Google Scholar]
  28. Ellis, R., & Barkhuizen, G.
    (2005) Analysing learner language. Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Esteve-Gibert, N., & Prieto, P.
    (2013) Prosodic structure shapes the temporal realisation of intonation and manual gesture movements. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 56(3), 850–864. 10.1044/1092‑4388(2012/12‑0049)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2012/12-0049) [Google Scholar]
  30. Ferrero, F., Magno Caldognetto, E., & Cosi, P.
    (1996) Sui piani formantici acustici e uditivi delle vocali di uomo, donna e bambino. InA. Perretti & P. Simonetti (Eds.), Atti del XXIV Convegno Nazionale dell’A.I.A. (pp. 169–178). Arti Grafiche Padovane.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Gao, M.
    (2021) Comparison of pitch variation and pitch range in L1 and L2 Mandarin Chinese. Poster presented at the1st International Conference on Tone and Intonation (TAI). Retrieved fromurn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:du-39054. 10.21437/TAI.2021‑12
    https://doi.org/10.21437/TAI.2021-12 [Google Scholar]
  32. Gillespie, M., James, A. N., Federmeier, K. D., & Watson, D. G.
    (2014) Verbal working memory predicts co-speech gesture: Evidence from individual differences. Cognition, 1321, 174–180. 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.012
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.03.012 [Google Scholar]
  33. Gluhareva, D., & Prieto, P.
    (2017) Training with rhythmic beat gestures favors L2 pronunciation in discourse-demanding situations. Language Teaching Research, 21(5), 609–631. 10.1177/1362168816651463
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168816651463 [Google Scholar]
  34. Graziano, M., Nicoladis, E., & Marentette, P.
    (2020) How referential gestures align with speech: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual speakers. Language Learning, 701, 266–304. 10.1111/lang.12376
    https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12376 [Google Scholar]
  35. Goodwin, M., & Goodwin, C.
    (1986) Gesture and coparticipation in the activity of searching for a word. Semiotica, 62(1–2), 51–76.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Gósy, M., Gyarmathy, D., & Beke, A.
    (2017) Phonetic analysis of filled pauses based on a Hungarian-English learner corpus. International Journal of Learner Corpus Research, 3(2), 149–174. 10.1075/ijlcr.3.2.03gos
    https://doi.org/10.1075/ijlcr.3.2.03gos [Google Scholar]
  37. Götz, S.
    (2013) Fluency in native and non-native English speech. John Benjamins. 10.1075/scl.53
    https://doi.org/10.1075/scl.53 [Google Scholar]
  38. Gullberg, M.
    (1998) Gesture as a communication strategy in second language discourse: A study of learners of French and Swedish. Lund University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Hagoort, P., & Indefrey, P.
    (2014) The neurobiology of language beyond single words. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 371, 347–362. 10.1146/annurev‑neuro‑071013‑013847
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-013847 [Google Scholar]
  40. Herment, S., &, Tortel, A.
    (2022) The intonation contour of non-finality revisited Implications for EFL teaching. InA. Kirkova-Naskova, A. Henderson, & J. Fouz-González (Eds.), English pronunciation instruction: Research-based insights (pp. 176–195). John Benjamins.10.1075/aals.19.08her
    https://doi.org/10.1075/aals.19.08her [Google Scholar]
  41. Hincks, R.
    (2005) Measuring liveliness in presentation speech. InProceedings of Interspeech 2005 (pp. 765–768). Retrieved fromhttps://www.isca-archive.org/interspeech_2005/hincks05_interspeech.html. 10.21437/Interspeech.2005‑353
    https://doi.org/10.21437/Interspeech.2005-353 [Google Scholar]
  42. Hirata, Y., Kelly, S., Huang, J., & Manansala, M.
    (2014) Effect of hand gestures on auditory learning of second-language vowel length contrasts. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 571, 2090–2101. 10.1044/2014_JSLHR‑S‑14‑0049
    https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-S-14-0049 [Google Scholar]
  43. Hoetjes, M., & van Maastricht, L.
    (2020) Using gesture to facilitate L2 phoneme acquisition: The importance of gesture and phoneme complexity. Frontiers in Psychology, 111, 575032. 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.575032
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.575032 [Google Scholar]
  44. Hoetjes, M., Krahmer, E., & Swerts, M.
    (2014) Does our speech change when we cannot gesture?Speech Communication, 571. 257–267.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Holler, J., & Stevens, R.
    (2007) The effect of common ground on how speakers use gesture and speech to represent size information. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 26(1), 4–27. 10.1177/0261927X06296428
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0261927X06296428 [Google Scholar]
  46. Housen, A., & Pierrard, M.
    (2006) Investigating instructed second language acquisition. InA. Housen, & M. Pierrard (Eds.), Investigations in instructed second language acquisition (pp. 12–27). Mouton de Gruyter.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Indefrey, P.
    (2011) The spatial and temporal signatures of word production components: A critical update. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(255), 1–16. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00255
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00255 [Google Scholar]
  48. Kearney, E., & Guenther, F. H.
    (2019) Articulating: The neural mechanisms of speech production. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 34(9), 1214–1229. 10.1080/23273798.2019.1589541
    https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2019.1589541 [Google Scholar]
  49. Keating, P., & Kuo, G.
    (2012) Comparison of speaking fundamental frequency in English and Mandarin. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132(2), 1050–1060. 10.1121/1.4730893
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4730893 [Google Scholar]
  50. Kelly, S. D., McDevitt, T., & Esch, M.
    (2009) Brief training with co-speech gesture lends a hand to word learning in a foreign language. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(2), 313–334. 10.1080/01690960802365567
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960802365567 [Google Scholar]
  51. Kita, S.
    (2000) Hito wa naze jesuchaa o suru noka [why do people gesture?]. Ninchikagaku [Cognitive Studies], 7(1), 9–21.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Kita, S., & Özyürek, A.
    (2003) What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 16–32. 10.1016/S0749‑596X(02)00505‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00505-3 [Google Scholar]
  53. Kormos, J.
    (1999) Monitoring and self-repair in L2. Language Learning, 491, 303–342. 10.1111/0023‑8333.00090
    https://doi.org/10.1111/0023-8333.00090 [Google Scholar]
  54. (2006) Speech production and L2 acquisition. Lawrence Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  55. (2011) Speech production and the cognition hypothesis. InP. Robinson (Ed.), L2 task complexity: Researching the cognition hypothesis of language learning and performance (pp. 39–60). John Benjamins. 10.1075/tblt.2.06ch2
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tblt.2.06ch2 [Google Scholar]
  56. Krauss, R. M.
    (1998) Why do we gesture when we speak?Current Directions in Psychological Science, 71, 54–60. 10.1111/1467‑8721.ep13175642
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep13175642 [Google Scholar]
  57. Krauss, R. M., Chen, Y., & Gottesman, R. F.
    (2000) Lexical gestures and lexical access: A process model. InD. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 261–283). Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511620850.017
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620850.017 [Google Scholar]
  58. Kushch, O., & Prieto, P.
    (2016) The effects of pitch accentuation and beat gestures on information recall in contrastive discourse. InJ. Barnes, A. Brugos, S. Shattuck-Hufnagel, & N. Veilleux. Proceedings of the Speech Prosody (pp. 922–925). International Speech Communication Association. 10.21437/SpeechProsody.2016‑189
    https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2016-189 [Google Scholar]
  59. Kushch, O., Igualada, A., & Prieto, P.
    (2018) Prominence in speech and gesture favour second language novel word learning. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33(8), 992–1004. 10.1080/23273798.2018.1435894
    https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2018.1435894 [Google Scholar]
  60. Lazaraton, A.
    (2004) Gesture and speech in the vocabulary explanations of one ESL teacher: A microanalytic inquiry. Language Learning, 54(1), 79–117. 10.1111/j.1467‑9922.2004.00249.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2004.00249.x [Google Scholar]
  61. Levelt, W. J. M.
    (1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  62. (1999) Producing spoken language: A blueprint of the speaker. InC. Brown & P. Hagoort (Eds.), Neurocognition of language (pp. 83–122). Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Lichtman, K.
    (2021) What about fluency? Implicit vs. explicit training affects artificial mini-language production. Applied Linguistics, 42(4), 668–691. 10.1093/applin/amaa054
    https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amaa054 [Google Scholar]
  64. Lin, Y. L.
    (2021) Gestures as scaffolding for L2 narrative recall: The role of gesture type, task complexity, and working memory. Language Teaching Research. 10.1177/13621688211044584
    https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688211044584 [Google Scholar]
  65. Liu, D., & Reed, M.
    (2021) Exploring the complexity of the L2 intonation system: An acoustic and eye-tracking study. Frontiers in Communication, 61, 627316. 10.3389/fcomm.2021.627316
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2021.627316 [Google Scholar]
  66. Loehr, D. P.
    (2012) Temporal, structural, and pragmatic synchrony between intonation and gesture. Laboratory Phonology, 3(1), 71–89. 10.1515/lp‑2012‑0006
    https://doi.org/10.1515/lp-2012-0006 [Google Scholar]
  67. Lynch, T., & Maclean, J.
    (2000) Exploring the benefits of task repetition and recycling for classroom language learning. Language Teaching Research, 41, 221–250. 10.1177/136216880000400303
    https://doi.org/10.1177/136216880000400303 [Google Scholar]
  68. Ma, S., & Jin, G.
    (2022) The relationship between different types of co-speech gestures and L2 speech performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 131, 941114. 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.941114
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.941114 [Google Scholar]
  69. Macedonia, M.
    (2014) Bringing back the body into the mind: Gestures enhance word learning in foreign language. Frontiers in Psychology, 51, 1467. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01467
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01467 [Google Scholar]
  70. Macedonia, M., Müller, K., & Friederici, A. D.
    (2011) The impact of iconic gestures on foreign language word learning and its neural substrate. Human Brain Mapping, 321, 982–998. 10.1002/hbm.21084
    https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21084 [Google Scholar]
  71. Macedonia, M., & von Kriegstein, K.
    (2012) Gestures enhance foreign language learning. Biolinguistics, 61, 393–416. 10.5964/bioling.8931
    https://doi.org/10.5964/bioling.8931 [Google Scholar]
  72. McCafferty, S. G.
    (2002) Gesture and creating zones of proximal development for second language learning. The Modern Language Journal, 86(2), 192–203. 10.1111/1540‑4781.00144
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1540-4781.00144 [Google Scholar]
  73. McClave, E.
    (1998) Pitch and manual gestures. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 271, 69–89. 10.1023/A:1023274823974
    https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023274823974 [Google Scholar]
  74. McNeill, D.
    (1992) Hand and mind. University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  75. (2005) Gesture and thought. University of Chicago Press. 10.7208/chicago/9780226514642.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226514642.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  76. Mennen, I.
    (2007) Phonological and phonetic influences in non-native intonation. InJ. Trouvain, & U. Gut (Eds.), Non-native prosody. Phonetic description and teaching practice (pp. 53–76). Mouton de Gruyter. 10.1515/9783110198751.1.53
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110198751.1.53 [Google Scholar]
  77. Mennen, I., Schaeffler, F., & Docherty, G.
    (2008) A methodological study into the linguistic dimensions of pitch range differences between German and English. InP. L. Barbosa, S. Madureira, & C. Reis. Proceedings of the IV Conference on Speech Prosody (pp. 527–530). ISCA Archivewww.isca-speech.org/archive/sp2008
    [Google Scholar]
  78. Mennen, I., Schaeffler, F., Watt, N., & Miller, N.
    (2008) An autosegmental-metrical investigation of intonation in people with Parkinson’s Disease. Asia-Pacific Journal of Speech Language and Hearing, 11(4), 205–219. 10.1179/136132808805335545
    https://doi.org/10.1179/136132808805335545 [Google Scholar]
  79. Mennen, I., Schaeffler, F., Docherty, G.
    (2012) Cross-language differences in fundamental frequency range: A comparison of English and German. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 131(3), 2249–2260. 10.1121/1.3681950
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.3681950 [Google Scholar]
  80. Mittelberg, I., & Waugh, L. R.
    (2009) Metonymy first, metaphor second: A cognitive-semiotic approach to multimodal figures of thought in co-speech gesture. InC. Forceville & E. Urios-Aparisi (Eds.), Multimodal metaphor (pp. 329–356). Mouton de Gruyter.
    [Google Scholar]
  81. Morett, L. M., & Chang, L. Y.
    (2014) Emphasising sound and meaning: Pitch gestures enhance Mandarin lexical tone acquisition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 98(3), 773–794.
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Morett, L. M., & Fraundorf, S. H.
    (2019) Listeners consider alternative speaker productions in discourse comprehension and memory: Evidence from beat gesture and pitch accenting. Memory and Cognition, 471, 1515–1530. 10.3758/s13421‑019‑00945‑1
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-00945-1 [Google Scholar]
  83. Mori, J., & Hayashi, M.
    (2006) The achievement of intersubjectivity through embodied completions: A study of interactions between first and second language speakers. Applied Linguistics, 271, 195–219. 10.1093/applin/aml014
    https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/aml014 [Google Scholar]
  84. Morris, D., Collett, P., Marsh, P., & O’Shaughnessy, M.
    (1980) Gestures: Their origins and distribution. New York.
  85. Morsella, E., & Krauss, R. M.
    (2004) The role of gestures in spatial working memory and speech. American Journal of Psychology, 1171, 411–424. 10.2307/4149008
    https://doi.org/10.2307/4149008 [Google Scholar]
  86. Nergis, A.
    (2021) Can explicit instruction of formulaic sequences enhance L2 oral fluency?Lingua, 2551, 1–21. 10.1016/j.lingua.2021.103072
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2021.103072 [Google Scholar]
  87. Nguyen, A. T., & Dao, D. M.
    (2018) The acquisition of question intonation by Vietnamese learners of English. Asian Journal of Second Foreign Language Education, 3(3), 1–13. 10.1186/s40862‑018‑0044‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1186/s40862-018-0044-4 [Google Scholar]
  88. Núñez, R.
    (2008) A fresh look at the foundations of mathematics: Gesture and the psychological reality of conceptual metaphor. InA. Cienki & C. Müller (Eds.), Metaphor and gesture (pp. 93–114). John Benjamins.
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Pang, F., & Skehan, P.
    (2014) Self-reported planning behaviour and L2 performance in narrative retelling. InP. Skehan (Ed.), Processing perspectives on task performance (pp. 95–128). John Benjamins. 10.1075/tblt.5.04pan
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tblt.5.04pan [Google Scholar]
  90. Patterson, D.
    (2000) A linguistic approach to pitch range modelling. University of Edinburgh.
    [Google Scholar]
  91. Peters, J., Frank, M., & Rohloff, M.
    (2020) Pitch range variation in High German (L1) and Low German (L2). InProceedings of the 10th International Conference on Speech Prosody (pp. 650–654). 10.21437/SpeechProsody.2020‑133
    https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2020-133 [Google Scholar]
  92. Pika, S., Nicoladis, E., & Marentette, P. F.
    (2006) A cross-cultural study on the use of gestures: Evidence for cross-linguistic transfer?Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 91, 319–327. 10.1017/S1366728906002665
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728906002665 [Google Scholar]
  93. Pine, K. J., Bird, H., & Kirk, E.
    (2007) The effects of prohibiting gestures on children’s lexical retrieval ability. Developmental Science, 101, 747–754. 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2007.00610.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00610.x [Google Scholar]
  94. Pinheiro, J., Bates, D., DebRoy, S., Sarkar, D., & R Core Team
    (2020) nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1–149. Retrieved fromhttps://CRAN.R-project.org/package=nlme
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Poesio, M., & Artstein, R.
    (2008) Introduction to the special issue on ambiguity and semantic judgments. Research on Language and Computation, 61, 241–245. 10.1007/s11168‑008‑9057‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11168-008-9057-3 [Google Scholar]
  96. Quinn-Allen, L. Q.
    (1995) The effects of emblematic gestures on the development and access of mental representations of French expressions. The Modern Language Journal, 791, 521–529. 10.1111/j.1540‑4781.1995.tb05454.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.1995.tb05454.x [Google Scholar]
  97. Rauscher, F. H., Krauss, R. M. & Chen, Y.
    (1996) Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production. Psychological Science, 7(4), 226–231. 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.1996.tb00364.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00364.x [Google Scholar]
  98. Robinson, P.
    (1996) Learning simple and complex second language rules under implicit, incidental, rule-search, and instructed conditions. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18(1), 27–67. 10.1017/S0272263100014674
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100014674 [Google Scholar]
  99. Rohrer, D., & Pashler, H.
    (2007) Increasing retention without increasing study time. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(4), 183–186. 10.1111/j.1467‑8721.2007.00500.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00500.x [Google Scholar]
  100. Rousse-Malpat, A., Koote, L., Steinkrauss, R., & Verspoor, M.
    (2021) Parlez-vous francais? Effects of structure-based versus dynamic-usage-based approaches on oral proficiency. Language Teaching Research. 10.1177/13621688211040298
    https://doi.org/10.1177/13621688211040298 [Google Scholar]
  101. Santiago, F., & Delais-Roussarie, E.
    (2014) What explains the distribution and form of non-final rising contours in French as an L2?InConcordia Working Papers in Applied Linguistics Vol. 5 (pp. 561–579). Retrieved fromdoe.concordia.ca/copal/volumes/
    [Google Scholar]
  102. Schmidt, R.
    (2000) Attention. InP. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 3–32). Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Schwab, S., & Goldman, J. P.
    (2016) Do speakers show different F0 when they speak in different languages? The case of English, French and German. InProceedings of Speech Prosody 2016. Retrieved fromhttps://www.isca-speech.org/archive/speechprosody_2016/. 10.21437/SpeechProsody.2016‑2
    https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2016-2 [Google Scholar]
  104. Segalowitz, N.
    (2010) Cognitive bases of L2 fluency. Routledge. 10.4324/9780203851357
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203851357 [Google Scholar]
  105. (2016) Second language fluency and its underlying cognitive and social determinants. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 54(2), 79–95. 10.1515/iral‑2016‑9991
    https://doi.org/10.1515/iral-2016-9991 [Google Scholar]
  106. Seyfeddinipur, M.
    (2006) Disfluency: Interrupting speech and gesture. Doctoral thesis. Radboud University.
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., Yasinnik, Y., Veilleux, N., & Renwick, M.
    (2007) A method for studying the time alignment of gestures and prosody in American English: ‘Hits’ and pitch accents in academic-lecture-style speech. InA. Esposito, M. Bratanic, E. Keller & M. Marinaro (Eds.), Fundamentals of verbal and nonverbal communication and the biometric issue (pp. 34–44). IOS Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Skehan, P.
    (1998) A cognitive approach to language learning. Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  109. (2009) Modelling second language performance: Integrating complexity, accuracy, fluency, and lexis. Applied Linguistics, 30(4), 510–532. 10.1093/applin/amp047
    https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amp047 [Google Scholar]
  110. (2014) Processing perspectives on task performance. John Benjamins. 10.1075/tblt.5
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tblt.5 [Google Scholar]
  111. Skehan, P., & Foster, P.
    (2001) Cognition and tasks. InP. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 183–205). Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9781139524780.009
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139524780.009 [Google Scholar]
  112. Sloetjes, H., & Wittenburg, P.
    (2008) Annotation by category – ELAN and ISO DCR. InN. Calzolari, K. Choukri, M. Bente, J. Mariani, J. Odijk, S. Piperidis, & D. Tapias (Eds.), Proceedings of the 6th International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation. Retrieved fromhttps://aclanthology.org/volumes/L08-1/
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Smotrova, T., & Lantolf, J. P.
    (2013) The function of gesture in lexically focused L2 instructional conversations. Modern Language Journal, 971, 397–416. 10.1111/j.1540‑4781.2013.12008.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2013.12008.x [Google Scholar]
  114. Sueyoshi, A., & Hardison, D. M.
    (2005) The role of gestures and facial cues in second language listening comprehension. Language Learning, 55(4), 661–699. 10.1111/j.0023‑8333.2005.00320.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0023-8333.2005.00320.x [Google Scholar]
  115. Taguchi, N.
    (2008) Building language blocks in L2 Japanese: Chunk learning and the development of complexity and fluency in spoken production. Foreign Language Annals, 41(1), 132–156. 10.1111/j.1944‑9720.2008.tb03283.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1944-9720.2008.tb03283.x [Google Scholar]
  116. Tavakoli, P., & Skehan, P.
    (2005) Strategic planning, task structure, and performance testing. InR. Ellis (Ed.), Planning and task performance in a second language. John Benjamins. 10.1075/lllt.11.15tav
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lllt.11.15tav [Google Scholar]
  117. Tellier, M.
    (2008) The effect of gestures on second language memorisation by young children. Gesture, 8(2), 219–235. 10.1075/gest.8.2.06tel
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.8.2.06tel [Google Scholar]
  118. Trouvain, J., Zimmerer, F., Möbius, B., Gósy, M., & Bonneau, A.
    (2017) Segmental, prosodic and fluency features in phonetic learner corpora: Introduction to the special issue. International Journal of Learner Corpus Research, 3(2), 105–117. 10.1075/ijlcr.3.2.01tro
    https://doi.org/10.1075/ijlcr.3.2.01tro [Google Scholar]
  119. Ullakanoja, R.
    (2007) Comparison of pitch range in Finnish (L1) and Russian (L2). InJ. Trouvain, & W. J. Barry (Eds), Proceedings of the 16th International congress of phonetic sciences (pp. 1701–1704). Retrieved fromwww.icphs2007.de/
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Volin, J., Poesova, K., & Weingartova, L.
    (2015) Speech melody properties in English, Czech and Czech English: Reference and interference. Research in Language, 13(1), 107–123. 10.1515/rela‑2015‑0018
    https://doi.org/10.1515/rela-2015-0018 [Google Scholar]
  121. Wang, L., & Chu, M.
    (2013) The role of beat gesture and pitch accent in semantic processing: An ERP study. Neuropsychologia, 51(13), 2847–2855. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.027
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.027 [Google Scholar]
  122. Wang, Z.
    (2014) Online time pressure manipulations: L2 speaking performance under five types of planning and repetition conditions. InP. Skehan (Ed.), Task-based language teaching: Processing perspectives on task performance (p.27–61). John Benjamins. 10.1075/tblt.5.02wan
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tblt.5.02wan [Google Scholar]
  123. Yasinnik, Y., Renwick, M., Shattuck-Hufnagel, S.
    (2004) The timing of speech-accompanying gestures with respect to gestures. InProceedings of from sound to sense (pp. 97–102). Retrieved fromhttps://www.rle.mit.edu/soundtosense/conference/pages/contributed.htm
    [Google Scholar]
  124. Zimmerer, F., Jügler, J., Andreeva, B., Möbius, B., & Trouvain, J.
    (2014) Too cautious to vary more? A comparison of pitch variation in native and non-native productions of French and German speakers. InN. Campbell, D. Gibbon, & D. Hirst (Eds.), Proceedings of the 7th international conference on speech prosody (pp. 1037–1041). Retrieved fromhttps://www.isca-speech.org/archive_v0/SpeechProsody_2014/. 10.21437/SpeechProsody.2014‑197
    https://doi.org/10.21437/SpeechProsody.2014-197 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lia.22023.cav
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/lia.22023.cav
Loading

Data & Media loading...

Most Cited

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error