1887
Volume 7, Issue 2
  • ISSN 1879-7865
  • E-ISSN: 1879-7873
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

In this study we analyzed the verbal and gestural behavior of 6-year-old French children during free dialogue explanation. We expected that children would alter their gestures according to content explained and interlocutor visibility. Thirty children explained two games to a peer: fifteen could not see their interlocutor whereas fifteen could. Results showed that the mean number of clauses per explanation of a child speaker interacting with a child addressee did not significantly change according to content explained or the visibility of the addressee. However, the mean number of gestures per clause is higher for a child speaker interacting with a child addressee both when explaining the spatial game and when face-to-face. Finally, children explaining how to play a spatial game produced more representational gestures and made more interactive gestures when the addressee was visible. These results are discussed in relation to previous studies on children’s language production and acquisition.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lia.7.2.02maz
2017-02-13
2025-02-14
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Aisenman, R. , & Berman, R
    (2000) Rethinking lexical analysis. Developing Literacy across Genres, Modalities, and Languages, 3, 187–196.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Alibali, M.W. , & Don, L.S
    (2001) Children’s gesture are meant to be seen. Gesture, 1(2), 113–127. doi: 10.1075/gest.1.2.02ali
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.1.2.02ali [Google Scholar]
  3. Alibali, M.W. , Kita, S. , & Young, A.J
    (2000) Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15(6), 593–613. doi: 10.1080/016909600750040571
    https://doi.org/10.1080/016909600750040571 [Google Scholar]
  4. Alibali, M.W. , Heath, D.C. , & Myers, H.J
    (2001) Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(6), 169–188. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2752
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2000.2752 [Google Scholar]
  5. Alamillo, R.A. , Colletta, J.-M. , & Guidetti, M
    (2013) Gesture and language in narratives and explanations: The effects of age and communicative activity on late multimodal discourse development. Journal of Child Language, 40(3), 511–538. doi: 10.1017/S0305000912000062
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000912000062 [Google Scholar]
  6. Antaki, C
    (1994) Explaining and arguing. The social organisation of accounts. London: Sage.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Argerich, N. , & Tolchinsky, L
    (2000) On a definition of lexical items in written and spoken texts. Developing Literacy Across Genres, Modalities and Languages, 3, 197–204.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Bavelas, J.B. , & Healing, S
    (2013) Reconciling the effects of mutual visibility on gesturing. Gesture, 13(1), 63–92. doi: 10.1075/gest.13.1.03bav
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.13.1.03bav [Google Scholar]
  9. Bavelas, J.B. , Chovil, N. , Lawrie, D.A. , & Wade, A
    (1992) Interactive gestures. Discourse Processes, 15(4), 469–489. doi: 10.1080/01638539209544823
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539209544823 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bavelas, J.B. , Gerwing, J. , Sutton, C. , & Prevost D
    (2008) Gesturing on the telephone: independent effects of dialogue and visibility. Journal of Memory and Language58, 495–520. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.004 [Google Scholar]
  11. Beattie, G. & Aboudan, R
    (1994) Gestures, pauses and speech: An experimental Investigation of the effects of changing social context on their precise temporal relationships. Semiotica, 99, 239–272. doi: 10.1515/semi.1994.99.3‑4.239
    https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1994.99.3-4.239 [Google Scholar]
  12. Berman, R.A
    (2000) Thematic perspectives on how children talk about interpersonal conflict. Developing Literacy across Genres, Modalities, and Languages, 3, 65–76.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. (2008) The psycholinguistics of text construction. Journal of Child Language, 35(4), 1–37. doi: 10.1017/S0305000908008787
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908008787 [Google Scholar]
  14. Berman, R.A. , & Katzenberger, I
    (2004) Form and function in introducing narrative and expository texts: A developmental perspective. Discourse Processes, 38(1), 57–94. doi: 10.1207/s15326950dp3801_3
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326950dp3801_3 [Google Scholar]
  15. Berman, R.A. , & Slobin, D.I
    (1987) Five ways of learning to talk about events: A crosslinguistic study of children’s narratives (Berkeley Cognitive Science Report No. 46). Berkeley CA: Institute of Cognitive Studies, University of California.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Berman, R.A. , & Verhoeven, L
    (2002) Cross linguistic perspectives on the development of text-production abilities in speech and writing. Written Language and Literacy, 5(1), 1–43. doi: 10.1075/wll.5.1.02ber
    https://doi.org/10.1075/wll.5.1.02ber [Google Scholar]
  17. Cohen, A.A
    (1977) The communications functions of hand illustrators. Journal of Communication, 27(4), 54–63. doi: 10.1111/j.1460‑2466.1977.tb01856.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.1977.tb01856.x [Google Scholar]
  18. Cohen, A.A. , & Harrison, R.P
    (1973) Intentionality in the use of hand illustrators in face-to-face communication situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 28(2), 276–279. doi: 10.1037/h0035792
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0035792 [Google Scholar]
  19. Coirier, P. , Gaonac’h, D. , & Passerault, J.-M
    (1996) Psycholinguistique textuelle. Paris: Armand Colin Édition.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Colletta, J.-M
    (2004) Le développement de la parole chez l’enfant âgé de 6 et 11 ans. Corps, langage et cognition. Hayen, Belgique: Mardaga.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. (2009) Comparative analysis of children’s narratives at different ages: A multimodal approach. Gesture, 9(1), 61–97. doi: 10.1075/gest.9.1.03col
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.9.1.03col [Google Scholar]
  22. Colletta, J.-M. , Lachnitt, C. , & Simon, J.-P
    (2005) Les conduites explicatives orales à l’école maternelle. In J.-F. Halté & M. Rispail (Eds.), L’oral dans la classe: Compétences, enseignement, activités (pp.137–151). Paris: Editions L’Harmattan.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Colletta, J.-M. , & Pellenq, C
    (2005) Les explications entre 3 et 11 ans: Fenêtre sur le développement discursif et multimodal de la parole. Fifth IAIMTE Conference, Albi, 11-13 mai.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Colletta, J.-M. & Pellenq, C
    (2009) The development of multimodal explanations in French children. In M.A. Nippold & C.M. Scott (Eds.), Expository discourse in children, adolescents, and adults development and disorders (pp.63–100). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Colletta, J.-M. , Capirci, O. , Cristilli, C. , Goldin-Meadow, S. , Guidetti, M. , & Levine, S
    (2011) Multimodal data transcription and annotation with ELAN (Coding manual). ANR Multimodality research. Retrived fromlidilem.u-grenoble3.fr/IMG/pdf/anrmultimodalityresearch-codingmanual.pdf
  26. Cosnier, J
    (1996) Les gestes du dialogue, la communication verbale. Psychologie de la Motivation, 21, 129–138.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. (2008) Les gestes du dialogue. In P. Cabin & J.-F. Dortier (Eds.), La communication, état des savoirs (pp.119–128). Auxerre: Editions Sciences Humaines.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Doherty-Sneddon, G. , & Kent, G
    (1996) Visual signals and communication abilities of children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18(3), 595–608. doi: 10.1348/026151000165878
    https://doi.org/10.1348/026151000165878 [Google Scholar]
  29. Emmorey, K. , & Casey, S
    (2001) Gesture, thought and spatial language. Gesture, 1(1), 35–50. doi: 10.1075/gest.1.1.04emm
    https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.1.1.04emm [Google Scholar]
  30. Fantazi, D
    (2010) La gestualité cohésive dans les récits d’enfants âgés de 9 à 11 ans. Lidil, 42, 97–112.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Favart, M. , & Passerault, J.-M
    (1996) Functionnality of cohesion devices in the management of local and global coherence: two studies in children’s written production of narratives. In G. Rijlaarsdam , H. van den Berg , & M. Couzijn (Eds.), Theories, models and methodologies in writing (pp.349–365). Amsterdam: Amsterdam University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Gineste, M.-D. , & Le Ny, J.-F
    (2002) Psychologie cognitive du langage. Paris: Dunod.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Goldin-Meadow, S
    (2003) Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Cambridge, MA: The Bellknap Press of Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Graziano, M
    (2010) Le développement des gestes pragmatiques et leur relation avec le développement de la compétence textuelle chez l’enfant âgés de 4 à 10 ans. Lidil, 42, 113–138.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Grize, J.-B
    (1990) Logique et langage. Paris: Ophrys.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Guidetti, M. , Fibigerova, K. , & Colletta, J.-M
    (2014) Gestures and multimodal development. Some keys issues for language acquisition. In M. Seyfeddinipur & M. Gulberg (Eds.), From Gesture in conversation to visible action as utterance: Essays in honor of Adam Kendon (pp.351–370). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/z.188.16gui
    https://doi.org/10.1075/z.188.16gui [Google Scholar]
  37. Halliday, M.A.K
    (1989) Spoken and written language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Hempel, C.G. & Oppenheim, P
    (1948) Studies in the logic of explanation. Philosophy of Science, 15(2), 567–579.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Hickmann, M. , Kail, M. , & Roland, F
    (1995) L’organisation référentielle dans les récits d’enfants en fonction des contraintes contextuelles. Enfance, 2, 215–226. doi: 10.3406/enfan.1995.2131
    https://doi.org/10.3406/enfan.1995.2131 [Google Scholar]
  40. Jisa, H
    (2004) Growing into academic French. In R.A. Berman (Ed.), Later language development: Typological and psycholinguistic perspectives (Trends in Language Acquisition Research 3), (pp.135–162). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/tilar.3.10jis
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.3.10jis [Google Scholar]
  41. Johansson, V
    (1999) Word frequencies in speech and Writing: A study of expository discourse. Genre and Modality in Developing Discourse Abilities, 1, 182–198.
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Katzenberger, I
    (2004) The development of clause packaging in spoken and written texts. Journal of Pragmatics, 36, 1921–1948. doi: 10.1016/j.pragma.2004.01.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2004.01.010 [Google Scholar]
  43. Kendon, A
    (1987) On gesture: Its complementary relationship with speech. In A.W. Siegman & S. Feldstein (Eds.), Nonverbal behavior and communication (pp.65–97). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. (2000) Language and gesture: Unity or duality. In D. McNeill (Eds.), Language and gesture: Window into thought and action (pp.47–63). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. (2004) Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511807572
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807572 [Google Scholar]
  46. Kintsch, W
    (1974) The representation of meaning in memory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Kita, S
    (2000) How representational gestures help speaking. In D. McNeil (Eds.), Language and gesture (pp. 162–185). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511620850.011
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620850.011 [Google Scholar]
  48. Kita, S. , & Özyürek, A
    (2003) What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(1), 16–32. doi: 10.1016/S0749‑596X(02)00505‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00505-3 [Google Scholar]
  49. Kita, S. , Özyürek, A. , Allen, S. , Brown, A. , Furman, R. , & Ishizuka, T
    (2007) Relations between syntactic encoding and co-speech gestures: Implications for a model of speech and gesture production. Language and Cognitive Processes, 22(8), 1212–1236. doi: 10.1080/01690960701461426
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960701461426 [Google Scholar]
  50. Krauss, R.M
    (1998) Why do we gesture when we speak?Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 54–59. doi: 10.1111/1467‑8721.ep13175642
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep13175642 [Google Scholar]
  51. Krauss, R.M. , Dushay, R.A. , Chen, Y. , & Rausher, F
    (1995) The communicative value of conversational hand gestures. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 31(6), 533–552. doi: 10.1006/jesp.1995.1024
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jesp.1995.1024 [Google Scholar]
  52. Le Ny, J.-F
    (1979) La sémantique psychologique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. doi: 10.3917/puf.lenyj.1979.01
    https://doi.org/10.3917/puf.lenyj.1979.01 [Google Scholar]
  53. Levelt, W
    (1989) Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  54. Lickiss, K.P. , & Wellens, A.R
    (1978) Effects of visual accessibility and hand restraint on fluency of gesticulator and effectiveness of message. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 46, 925–926. doi: 10.2466/pms.1978.46.3.925
    https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1978.46.3.925 [Google Scholar]
  55. Lund, K
    (2003) Analyse de l’activité explicative en interaction: Étude de dialogues d’enseignants de physique en formation interprétant les interactions entre élèves. Unpublished PhD dissertation. University of Lyon.
    [Google Scholar]
  56. Mazur-Palandre, A
    (2009) Les connexions syntaxiques: Considérations contextuelles et développementales. In M. Sandré (Ed.), Analyses du discours et contextes: Quelles relations? (pp.125–136). Limoges: Lambert-Lucas.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. (2010) Formes et positions syntaxiques du syntagme nominal: Une étude développementale du français scolaire en ZEP. Sciences Croisées, 6(Numéro thématique), 75–112.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. (2011) Le flux de l’information: Aspects syntaxiques et discursifs. Une étude fonctionnaliste et développementale. Berlin: Éditions universitaires européennes.
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Mazur-Palandre, A. , & Jisa, H
    (2012) La lexicalité des productions orales et écrites: Une étude développementale. Enfance, 4, 359–371. doi: 10.4074/S0013754512004016
    https://doi.org/10.4074/S0013754512004016 [Google Scholar]
  60. McNeill, D
    (1992) Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  61. (2000) Language and gesture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511620850
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620850 [Google Scholar]
  62. McNeill, D. , & Duncan, S.D
    (2000) Growth points in thinking-for-speaking. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 141–161). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511620850.010
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620850.010 [Google Scholar]
  63. Özcaliskan, S. , & Goldin-Meadow, S
    (2004) When mothers do not lead their children by the hand. In A. Brugos , L. Micciulla , & C. Smith (Eds.), Proceedings of the 28th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 424–435). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Rabahi, T. , Fargier, P. , Rifai-Sarraj, A. , Clouzeau, C. & Massarelli, R
    (2012) Motor performance may be improved by kinesthetic imagery, specific action verb production, and mental calculation. NeuroReport, 23(2), 78–81. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834e7dce
    https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834e7dce [Google Scholar]
  65. Ravid, D
    (2006) Semantic development in textual contexts during the school years: Noun scale analyses. Journal of Child Language, 33(4), 791–821. doi: 10.1017/S0305000906007586
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000906007586 [Google Scholar]
  66. Ravid, D. , & Berman, R.A
    (2010) Developing noun phrase complexity across adolescence: A text-embedded analysis. First Language, 30(1), 1–29.
    [Google Scholar]
  67. Rimé, B
    (1982) The elimination of visible behavior from social interactions: Effects on verbal, non verbal ans interpersonal variables. European Journal of Social Psychology, 12(2), 113–129. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420120201
    https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420120201 [Google Scholar]
  68. Schneuwly, B
    (1988) Le langage écrit chez l’enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé.
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Traxler, M. , & Gernsbacher, M.A
    (1995) Improving coherence in written communication. In M.A. Gernsbacher & T. Givón (Eds.), Coherence in spontaneous text (pp.215–238). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/tsl.31.08tra
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tsl.31.08tra [Google Scholar]
  70. Veneziano, E. , & Hudelot, C
    (2002) Développement des compétences pragmatiques et théories de l’esprit chez l’enfant: Le cas de l’explication. In J. Bernicot , A. Trognon , M. Guidetti , & M. Musiol (Eds.), Pragmatique et psychologie (pp. 215–236). Nancy: Presses Universitaires de Nancy.
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Veneziano, E. , & Sinclair, H
    (1995) Functional changes in early child language: The appearance of references to the past and of explanations. Journal of Child Language, 22(3), 557–581. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900009958
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900009958 [Google Scholar]
  72. Warren, A.R. & Tate, C.S
    (1992) Egocentrism in children’s telephone conversations. In R.M. Diaz & L.E. Berk (Eds.), Private speech: From social interaction to self-regulation (pp.245–264). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lia.7.2.02maz
Loading

Most Cited

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error