1887
Volume 8, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1879-7865
  • E-ISSN: 1879-7873
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Stefanini, Bello, Caselli, Iverson, & Volterra (2009) reported that Italian 24–36 month old children use a high proportion of representational gestures to accompany their spoken responses when labelling pictures. The two studies reported here used the same naming task with (1) typically developing 24–46-month-old hearing children acquiring English and (2) 24–63-month-old deaf children of deaf and hearing parents acquiring British Sign Language (BSL) and spoken English. In Study 1 children scored within the range of correct spoken responses previously reported, but produced very few representational gestures. However, when they did gesture, they expressed the same action meanings as reported in previous research. The action bias was also observed in deaf children of hearing parents in Study 2, who labelled pictures with signs, spoken words and gestures. The deaf group with deaf parents used BSL almost exclusively with few additional gestures. The function of representational gestures in spoken and signed vocabulary development is considered in relation to differences between native and non-native sign language acquisition.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lia.8.1.04tho
2017-10-02
2025-02-16
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Barsalou, L.
    (2008) Grounded Cognition. Annual Review of Psychology59(1), 617–645. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639 [Google Scholar]
  2. Bates, E. , & Dick, F.
    (2002) Language, gesture, and the developing brain. Developmental Psychobiology40, 293–310. doi: 10.1002/dev.10034
    https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.10034 [Google Scholar]
  3. Bates, E. , Benigni, L. , Bretherton, I. , Camaioni, L. , & Volterra, V.
    (1979) The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Bello, A. , Caselli, M. C. , Pettenati, P. , & Stefanini, S.
    (2010) PinG – Parole in Gioco. Una prova di comprensione e produzione lessicale per la prima infanzia. Firenze, Italy: Giunti O.S. Organizzazioni Speciali.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bello, A. , Giannantoni, P. , Pettenati, P. , Stefanini, S. , & Caselli, M. C.
    (2012) Assessing lexicon: Validation and developmental data of the Picture Naming Game (PiNG), a new picture naming task for toddlers. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders47, 589–602. doi: 10.1111/j.1460‑6984.2012.00168.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00168.x [Google Scholar]
  6. Butcher, C. , & Goldin-Meadow, S.
    (2000) Gesture and the transition from one- to two-word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp.235–257). New York: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511620850.015
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511620850.015 [Google Scholar]
  7. Capirci, O. , Pizzuto, E. , & Volterra, V.
    (1996) Gesture and words during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language23, 645–673. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900008989
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008989 [Google Scholar]
  8. Clark, E. V.
    (2009) First Language Acquisition (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511806698
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511806698 [Google Scholar]
  9. Cormier, K. , Schembri, A. , Vinson, D. , & Orfanidou, E.
    (2012) First language acquisition differs from second language acquisition in prelingually deaf signers: Evidence from grammatical processing of British Sign Language. Cognition124, 50–65. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.04.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.04.003 [Google Scholar]
  10. Goldin-Meadow, S.
    (2003) The resilience of language: What gesture creation in deaf children can tell us about how all children learn language. New York: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. (2007) Pointing sets the stage for learning language – and creating language. Child Development78, 741–745. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2007.01029.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01029.x [Google Scholar]
  12. Iverson, J. M. , Capirci, O. , Volterra, V. , & Goldin-Meadow, S.
    (2008) Learning to talk in a gesture rich world: Early communication of Italian vs. American children. First Language28(2), 164–181. doi: 10.1177/0142723707087736
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723707087736 [Google Scholar]
  13. Iverson, J. M. , & Goldin-Meadow, S.
    (Eds.) (1998) The nature and functions of gesture in children's communications. San Francisco: Jossey- Bass.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Kendon, A.
    (2004) Gesture: Visible Action as Utterance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511807572
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511807572 [Google Scholar]
  15. Klima, E. S. , & Bellugi, U.
    (1979) The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Lu, J. , Jones, A. , & Morgan, G.
    (2016) The impact of input quality on early sign development in native and non-native language learners. Journal of Child Language43, 537–552. doi: 10.1017/S0305000915000835
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000915000835 [Google Scholar]
  17. Mann, W. , Sheng, L. , & Morgan, G.
    (2016) Lexical-semantic organization in bilingually developing deaf children with ASL-dominant language exposure: Evidence from a repeated meaning association task. Language Learning66(4), 872–899. doi: 10.1111/lang.12169
    https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12169 [Google Scholar]
  18. Mayberry, R. I. , & Eichen, E. B.
    (1991) The long-lasting advantage of learning sign language in childhood: Another look at the critical period for language acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language30, 486–512. doi: 10.1016/0749‑596X(91)90018‑F
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90018-F [Google Scholar]
  19. McNeill, D.
    (1992) Hand and Mind: What Gestures Reveal About Thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Mitchell, R. E. , & Karchmer, M. A.
    (2004) Chasing the mythical ten percent: Parental hearing status of deaf and hard of hearing students in the United States. Sign Language Studies4(2), 138–163. doi: 10.1353/sls.2004.0005
    https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.2004.0005 [Google Scholar]
  21. Mumford, K. , & Kita, S.
    (2014) Children use gesture to interpret novel verb meanings. Child Development85, 1181–1189. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12188
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12188 [Google Scholar]
  22. Newport, E. L.
    (1990) Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science14, 11–28. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1401_2 [Google Scholar]
  23. Newport, E. , & Meier, R.
    (1985) The acquisition of American Sign Language. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition: The data (pp.881–938). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. O'Reilly, A. W.
    (1995) Using representations: Comprehension and production of actions with imagined objects. Child Development66(4), 999–1010. doi: 10.2307/1131794
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1131794 [Google Scholar]
  25. Orlansky, M. D. , & Bonvillian, J. D.
    (1984) The role of iconicity in early sign language acquisition. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders49, 287–292. doi: 10.1044/jshd.4903.287
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.4903.287 [Google Scholar]
  26. Özçalişkan, S. & Goldin-Meadow, S.
    (2005) Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition96 (B101-113).
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Özçalişkan, S. , Gentner, D. , & Goldin-Meadow, S.
    (2014) Do iconic gestures pave the way for children's early verbs?Applied Psycholinguistics35(6), 1143–1162. doi: 10.1017/S0142716412000720
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000720 [Google Scholar]
  28. Perez, M. , Valsameda, M. , & Morgan, G.
    (2015) Bilingual sign education in Madrid, Spain. In. G. Tang , H. Knoors & M. Marschark . Bilingualism and Bilingual Deaf Education (pp.602–645). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Perniss, P. , Özyürek, A. , & Morgan, G.
    (2015) The influence of the visual modality on language structure and language conventionalization: Insights from sign language and gesture. Topics in Cognitive Science7(1), 2–11. doi: 10.1111/tops.12127
    https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12127 [Google Scholar]
  30. Pettenati, P. , Stefanini, S. , & Volterra, V.
    (2010) Motoric characteristics of representational gestures produced by young children in a naming task. Journal of Child Language37, 887–911. doi: 10.1017/S0305000909990092
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000909990092 [Google Scholar]
  31. Pettenati, P. , Sekine K. , Congestrì, E. , & Volterra, V.
    (2012) A comparative study on representational gestures in Italian and Japanese children. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior36(2), 149–164. doi: 10.1007/s10919‑011‑0127‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-011-0127-0 [Google Scholar]
  32. Stefanini S. , Bello A. , Caselli M. C. , Iverson J. M. , & Volterra, V.
    (2009) Co-speech gestures in a naming task: developmental data. Language and Cognitive Processes24, 168–89. doi: 10.1080/01690960802187755
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960802187755 [Google Scholar]
  33. Tomasello, M.
    (2008) Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Thompson, R. L. , Vinson, D. P. , Woll, B. , & Vigliocco, G.
    (2012) The road to language learning is iconic: evidence from British Sign Language. Psychological Science23(12), 1443–1448. doi: 10.1177/0956797612459763
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612459763 [Google Scholar]
  35. Volterra, V. , & Erting. C. J.
    (Eds.) (1994) From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children. Washington DC: Gallaudet University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Werner, H. , & Kaplan, B.
    (1963) Symbol formation: An organismic developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. New York: John Wiley.
    [Google Scholar]
  37. Woolfe, T. , Herman, R. , Roy, P. , & Woll, B.
    (2010) Early vocabulary development in deaf native signers: A British Sign Language adaptation of the communicative development inventories. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry51, 322–331. doi: 10.1111/j.1469‑7610.2009.02151.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02151.x [Google Scholar]
  38. Woll, B.
    (2013) Sign language and spoken language development in young children: Measuring vocabulary by means of the CDI. In L. Meurant , A. Sinte , M. van Herreweghe & M. Vermeerbergen (Eds.), Sign language research, uses and practices: Crossing views on theoretical and applied sign language linguistics (pp.15–34). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter & Ishara Press. doi: 10.1515/9781614511472.15
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9781614511472.15 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lia.8.1.04tho
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/lia.8.1.04tho
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): deaf; gesture; language development; sign language

Most Cited

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error