1887
The locus of linguistic variation
  • ISSN 2211-6834
  • E-ISSN: 2211-6842
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

This paper examines the factors conditioning the production of linguistic variables in real time by individual speakers: what we term the . We propose a framework that recognizes three types of factors conditioning variation: sociostylistic, internal linguistic, and psychophysiological. We develop two main points against this background. The first is that sequences of variants produced by individuals display systematic patterns that can be understood in terms of sociostylistic conditioning and psychophysiological conditioning. The second is that psychophysiological conditioning and internal linguistic conditioning are distinct in their mental implementations; this claim has implications for understanding the locality of the factors conditioning alternations, the universality and language-specificity of variation, and the general question of whether grammar and language use are distinct. Questions about the dynamics of variation in individuals are set against community-centered perspectives to argue that findings in the two domains, though differing in explanatory focus, can ultimately be mutually informative.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/lv.16.2.06tam
2017-01-12
2024-12-08
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Abramowicz, Lukasz
    2007 Sociolinguistics meets Exemplar Theory: Frequency and recency effects in (ing). University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics13. 27–37.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Ackerman, Phillip L
    1988 Determinants of individual differences during skill acquisition: Cognitive abilities and information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology117(3). 288–318. doi: 10.1037/0096‑3445.117.3.288
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.117.3.288 [Google Scholar]
  3. Allum, Paul H. & Linda Wheeldon
    2007 Planning scope in spoken sentence production: The role of grammatical units. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition33(4). 791–810. doi: 10.1037/0278‑7393.33.4.791
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.33.4.791 [Google Scholar]
  4. Babel, Molly
    2012 Evidence for phonetic and social selectivity in spontaneous phonetic imitation. Journal of Phonetics40. 177–189. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2011.09.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.09.001 [Google Scholar]
  5. Baddeley, Alan
    1986Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Bayley, Robert
    2013 The quantitative paradigm. In J.K. Chambers & Natalie Schilling (eds.), The handbook of language variation and change, 85–107. Malden, MA: Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Bermudez-Otero, Ricardo
    2013 Amphichronic explanation and the life cycle of phonological processes. In Patrick Honeybone & Joseph Salmons (eds.), The Oxford handbook of historical phonology, Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Bock, Kathryn
    1986 Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology18. 355–387. doi: 10.1016/0010‑0285(86)90004‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(86)90004-6 [Google Scholar]
  9. Bresnan, Joan & Tatiana Nikitina
    2009 The gradience of the dative alternation. In Linda Uyechi & Lian-Hee Wee (eds.), Reality exploration and discovery: Pattern interaction in language and life, 161–184. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Brewer, Gene A. & Nash Unsworth
    2012 Individual differences in the effects of retrieval from long-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language66. 407–415. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2011.12.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.12.009 [Google Scholar]
  11. Cameron, Richard
    1992Pronominal and null subject variation in Spanish: Constraints, dialects, and functional compensation. University of Pennsylvania dissertation.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Cameron, Richard & Nydia Flores-Ferran
    2004 Perseveration of subject expression across regional dialects of Spanish. Spanish in Context1(1). 41–65. doi: 10.1075/sic.1.1.05cam
    https://doi.org/10.1075/sic.1.1.05cam [Google Scholar]
  13. Campbell-Kibler, Kathryn
    2010 New directions in sociolinguistic cognition. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics15(2). 31–39.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Cedergren, Henrietta & David Sankoff
    1974 Variable rules: Performance as a statistical reflection of competence. Language50(2). 333–355. doi: 10.2307/412441
    https://doi.org/10.2307/412441 [Google Scholar]
  15. Chomsky, Noam
    1965Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. 1986Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin, and use. Greenwood Publishing Group.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Clark, Herbert H. & Thomas Wasow
    1998 Repeating words in spontaneous speech. Cognitive Psychology37(3). 201–242. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0693
    https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1998.0693 [Google Scholar]
  18. Clark, Lynn & Graeme Trousdale
    2009 Exploring the role of token frequency in phonological change: Evidence from TH-fronting in east-central Scotland. English Language and Linguistics13(1). 33–55. doi: 10.1017/S1360674308002852
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1360674308002852 [Google Scholar]
  19. Clark, Lynn & Liam Walsh
    2014 Recency effects on word-medial /t/ in New Zealand English: initial observations. Poster presented at the 15th Australasian International Speech Science and Technology Conference , Christchurch, New Zealand.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Coetzee, Andries & Joe Pater
    2011 The place of variation in phonological theory. In John A. Goldsmith , Jason Riggle , & Alan C.L. Yu (eds.), The handbook of phonological theory, 2nd edn, 401–434. Malden, MA: Blackwell. doi: 10.1002/9781444343069.ch13
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444343069.ch13 [Google Scholar]
  21. Daneman, Meredyth
    1991 Working memory as a predictor of verbal fluency. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research20(6). 445–464. doi: 10.1007/BF01067637
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067637 [Google Scholar]
  22. Drager, Katie & Jennifer Hay
    2012 Exploiting random intercepts: Two case studies in sociophonetics. Language Variation and Change24. 59–78. doi: 10.1017/S0954394512000014
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394512000014 [Google Scholar]
  23. Eckert, Penelope
    2012 Three waves of variation study: The emergence of meaning in the study of sociolinguistic variation. Annual Review of Anthropology41. 87–100. doi: 10.1146/annurev‑anthro‑092611‑145828
    https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-anthro-092611-145828 [Google Scholar]
  24. Ellis, Lucy & William J. Hardcastle
    2002 Categorical and gradient properties of assimilation in alveolar to velar sequences: Evidence from EPG and EMA data. Journal of Phonetics30(3). 373–396. doi: 10.1006/jpho.2001.0162
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.2001.0162 [Google Scholar]
  25. Embick, David
    2008 Variation and morphosyntactic theory: Competition fractionated. Language and Linguistics Compass2(1). 59–78. doi: 10.1111/j.1749‑818X.2007.00038.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2007.00038.x [Google Scholar]
  26. 2010aLocalism versus globalism in morphology and phonology. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/9780262014229.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262014229.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  27. 2010bStem alternations and stem distributions. Ms., University of Pennsylvania; www.ling.upenn.edu/˜embick/stem-ms-10.pdf.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. 2013 Morphemes and morphophonological loci. In Alec Marantz & Ora Matushansky (eds.), Distributed morphology today: Morphemes for Morris Halle, 151–166. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. doi: 10.7551/mitpress/9780262019675.003.0009
    https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262019675.003.0009 [Google Scholar]
  29. Embick, David & David Poeppel
    2015 Towards a computational(ist) neurobiology of language: Correlational, integrated, and explanatory neurolinguistics. Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience30(4). 357–366. doi: 10.1080/23273798.2014.980750
    https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.980750 [Google Scholar]
  30. Ernestus, Mirjam
    2014 Acoustic reduction and the roles of abstractions and exemplars in speech processing. Lingua142. 27–41. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2012.12.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2012.12.006 [Google Scholar]
  31. Farnetani, Edda & Daniel Recasens
    2010 Coarticulation and connected speech processes. In William J. Hardcastle , John Laver , & Fiona E. Gibbon (eds.), The handbook of phonetic sciences, 316–352. Malden, MA: Blackwell. doi: 10.1002/9781444317251.ch9
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444317251.ch9 [Google Scholar]
  32. Farrar, Kimberly & Mari C. Jones
    2002 Introduction. In Mari C. Jones & Edith Esch (eds.), Language change, 1–18. Mouton de Gruyter. doi: 10.1515/9783110892598.1
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110892598.1 [Google Scholar]
  33. Fasold, Ralph W
    1991 The quiet demise of variable rules. American Speech66(1). 3–21. doi: 10.2307/455431
    https://doi.org/10.2307/455431 [Google Scholar]
  34. Ferreira, Fernanda
    1991 Effects of length and syntactic complexity on initiation times for prepared utterances. Journal of Memory and Language30(2). 210- 233. doi: 10.1016/0749‑596X(91)90004‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(91)90004-4 [Google Scholar]
  35. Fruehwald, Josef
    2012 Redevelopment of a morphological class. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics18(1). 77–86.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. German, J.S. , K. Carlson & J.B. Pierrehumbert
    2013 Reassignment of consonant allophones in rapid dialect acquisition. Journal of Phonetics41(3). 228–248. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2013.03.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2013.03.001 [Google Scholar]
  37. Ginsberg, Daniel
    2012 A computational approach to conversational style. Paper presented at NWAV 41 .
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Gries, Stefan Th
    2005 Syntactic priming: A corpus-based approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research34(4). 365–399. doi: 10.1007/s10936‑005‑6139‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-005-6139-3 [Google Scholar]
  39. Guy, Gregory R
    1980 Variation in the group and the individual: The case of final stop deletion. In William Labov (ed.), Locating language in time and space, chap. 1, 1–36. New York: Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. 1991a Contextual conditioning in variable lexical phonology. Language Variation and Change3. 223–239. doi: 10.1017/S0954394500000533
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394500000533 [Google Scholar]
  41. 1991b Explanation in variable phonology: An exponential model of morphological constraints. Language Variation and Change3. 1–22. doi: 10.1017/S0954394500000429
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394500000429 [Google Scholar]
  42. Guy, Gregory R. & Charles Boberg
    1997 Inherent variability and the obligatory contour principle. Language Variation and Change9. 149–164. doi: 10.1017/S095439450000185X
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S095439450000185X [Google Scholar]
  43. Hartsuiker, Robert J. & Pashiera N. Barkhuysen
    2006 Language production and working memory: The case of subject-verb agreement. Language and Cognitive Processes21(1–3). 181–204. doi: 10.1080/01690960400002117
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960400002117 [Google Scholar]
  44. Hay, Jennifer & Joan Bresnan
    2006 Spoken syntax: The phonetics of giving a hand in New Zealand English. The Linguistic Review23(3). 321–349.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Hoit, Jeannette D. , Nancy Pearl Solomon & Thomas J. Hixon
    1993 Effect of lung volume on voice onset time (VOT). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research36(3). 516–520. doi: 10.1044/jshr.3603.516
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3603.516 [Google Scholar]
  46. Hoole, Philip , Noel Nguyen-Trong & William Hardcastle
    1993 A comparative investigation of coarticulation in fricatives: Electropalatographic, electromagnetic, and acoustic data. Language and Speech36(2–3). 235–260.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Houston, Ann
    1985Continuity and change in English morphology: The variable (ING). University of Pennsylvania dissertation.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Jaeger, T. Florian & Neal E. Snider
    2013 Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime’s prediction error given both prior and recent experience. Cognition127. 57–83. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.10.013
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.10.013 [Google Scholar]
  49. Kemper, Susan , Ruth E. Herman & Cindy H.T. Lian
    2003 The costs of doing two things at once for young and older adults: Talking while walking, finger tapping, and ignoring speech of noise. Psychology and Aging18(2). 181–192. doi: 10.1037/0882‑7974.18.2.181
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.181 [Google Scholar]
  50. Kemper, Susan & Aaron Sumner
    2001 The structure of verbal abilities in young and older adults. Psychology and Aging16(2). 312–322. doi: 10.1037/0882‑7974.16.2.312
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.16.2.312 [Google Scholar]
  51. King, Ruth , France Martineau & Raymond Mougeon
    2011 The interplay of internal and external factors in grammatical change: First person plural pronouns in French. Language87(3). 470–509. doi: 10.1353/lan.2011.0072
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2011.0072 [Google Scholar]
  52. Kiparsky, Paul
    1972 Explanation in phonology. In Stanley Peters (ed.), Goals of linguistic theory, 189–227. Cinnaminson, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Kutik, Elanah J. , William E. Cooper & Suzanne Boyce
    1983 Declination of fundamental frequency in speakers production of parenthetical and main clauses. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America73(5). 1731–1738. doi: 10.1121/1.389397
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.389397 [Google Scholar]
  54. Labov, William
    1969 Contraction, deletion, and inherent variability of the English copula. Language45(4). 715–762. doi: 10.2307/412333
    https://doi.org/10.2307/412333 [Google Scholar]
  55. 1979 Locating the frontier between social and psychological factors in linguistic variation. In Charles J. Fillmore , Daniel Kempler , & William S.-Y. Yang (eds.), Individual differences in language ability and language behavior, chap. 18, 327–340. Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  56. 1989 The child as linguistic historian. Language Variation and Change1. 85–97. doi: 10.1017/S0954394500000120
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394500000120 [Google Scholar]
  57. 2001 The anatomy of style-shifting. In Penelope Eckert & John R. Rickford (eds.), Style and sociolinguistic variation, chap. 5, 85–108. Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. 2006[1966]The social stratification of English in New York City. Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  59. 2012 What is to be learned: The community as the focus of social cognition. Review of Cognitive Linguistics10(2). 265–293. doi: 10.1075/rcl.10.2.02lab
    https://doi.org/10.1075/rcl.10.2.02lab [Google Scholar]
  60. 2014 The sociophonetic orientation of the language learner. In Chiara Celata & Silvia Calamai (eds.), Advances in sociophonetics, 17–29. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins Publishing Company. doi: 10.1075/silv.15.01lab
    https://doi.org/10.1075/silv.15.01lab [Google Scholar]
  61. Labov, William , Sharon Ash & Charles Boberg
    2006The atlas of North American English: Phonetics, phonology and sound change. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Labov, William , Sharon Ash , Maya Ravindranath , Tracey Weldon , Maciej Baranowski & Naomi Nagy
    2011 Properties of the sociolinguistic monitor. Journal of Sociolinguistics15(4). 431–463. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9841.2011.00504.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9841.2011.00504.x [Google Scholar]
  63. Labov, William , Paul Cohen , Clarence Robins & John Lewis
    1968 A study of the non-standard English of Negro and Puerto Rican speakers in New York City. Final report, Cooperative Research Project 3288, vols. I and II.
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Levelt, William J.M
    1989Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Lewis, Shevaun & Colin Phillips
    2015 Aligning grammatical theories and language processing models. Journal of Psycholinguistic Reasearch44. 27–46. doi: 10.1007/s10936‑014‑9329‑z
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-014-9329-z [Google Scholar]
  66. Liberman, Alvin M. , Franklin S. Cooper , Donald P. Shankweiler & Michael Studdert-Kennedy
    1967 Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review74(6). 431–461. doi: 10.1037/h0020279
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0020279 [Google Scholar]
  67. MacKenzie, Laurel
    2012Locating variation above the phonology. University of Pennsylvania dissertation.
    [Google Scholar]
  68. 2013 Variation in English auxiliary realization: A new take on contraction. Language Variation and Change25(1). 17–41. doi: 10.1017/S0954394512000257
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394512000257 [Google Scholar]
  69. 2015a Opacity over time: Charting the paths of fricative voicing in English plurals. Paper presented at The Second Edinburgh Symposium on Historical Phonology , University of Edinburgh, December 4.
    [Google Scholar]
  70. 2015b Production planning effects on variable contraction in English. Paper presented at NWAV 44 , University of Toronto, October 23.
    [Google Scholar]
  71. Manuel, Sharon Y
    1990 The role of contrast in limiting vowel-to-vowel coarticulation in different languages. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America88(3). 1286–1298. doi: 10.1121/1.399705
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.399705 [Google Scholar]
  72. Marr, David
    1982Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  73. McFarland, David H
    2001 Respiratory markers of conversational interaction. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research44(1). 128–143. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2001/012)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2001/012) [Google Scholar]
  74. Miller, G.A. & N. Chomsky
    1963 Finitary models of language users. Handbook of mathematical psychology2. 419–491.
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Miller, George A
    1956 The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. The Psychological Review63(2). 81–97. doi: 10.1037/h0043158
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043158 [Google Scholar]
  76. Namy, Laura L. , Lynne C. Nygaard & Denise Sauerteig
    2002 Gender differences in vocal accommodation: The role of perception. Journal of Language and Social Psychology21(4). 422–432. doi: 10.1177/026192702237958
    https://doi.org/10.1177/026192702237958 [Google Scholar]
  77. Oushiro, Livia
    2016 Social and structural constraints in lectal cohesion. Lingua172–173. 116–130. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2015.10.015
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2015.10.015 [Google Scholar]
  78. Oushiro, Livia & Gregory R. Guy
    2015 The effect of salience on co-variation in Brazilian Portuguese. University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics21(2).
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Pardo, Jennifer S
    2006 On phonetic convergence during conversational interaction. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America119(4). 2382–2393. doi: 10.1121/1.2178720
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2178720 [Google Scholar]
  80. Pardo, Jennifer S. , Kelly Jordan , Rolliene Mallari , Caitlin Scanlon & Eva Lewandowski
    2013 Phonetic convergence in shadowed speech: The relation between acoustic and perceptual measures. Journal of Memory and Language69(3). 183–195. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.002 [Google Scholar]
  81. Pardo, J.S. , R. Gibbons , A. Suppes & R.M. Krauss
    2012 Phonetic convergence in college roommates. Journal of Phonetics40(1). 190–197. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2011.10.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.10.001 [Google Scholar]
  82. Paster, Mary
    2006Phonological conditions on affixation. University of California at Berkeley dissertation.
    [Google Scholar]
  83. Patrick, Peter L
    1991 Creoles at the intersection of variable processes: (td)-deletion and past-marking in the Jamaican mesolect. Language Variation and Change3(2). 171–189. doi: 10.1017/S095439450000051X
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S095439450000051X [Google Scholar]
  84. Pickering, Martin J. & Victor S. Ferreira
    2008 Structural priming: A critical review. Psycholinguistic Bulletin134(3). 427–459. doi: 10.1037/0033‑2909.134.3.427
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.427 [Google Scholar]
  85. Pickering, Martin J. & Simon Garrod
    2004 Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences27. 169–226.
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Pierrehumbert, Janet B
    2002 Word-specific phonetics. InLaboratory phonologyVII, 101–139. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.
    [Google Scholar]
  87. Podesva, Robert J
    2007 Three sources of stylistic meaning. In Proceedings of SALSA 2007 .
    [Google Scholar]
  88. Poeppel, David & David Embick
    2005 Defining the relation between linguistics and neuroscience. In Ann Cutler (ed.), Twenty-first century psycholinguistics: Four cornerstones, Lawrence Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  89. Poplack, Shana
    1980 Deletion and disambiguation in Puerto Rican Spanish. Language56(2). 371–385. doi: 10.1353/lan.1980.0033
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1980.0033 [Google Scholar]
  90. 1984 Variable concord and sentential plural marking in Puerto Rican Spanish. Hispanic Review52(2). 205–222. doi: 10.2307/473375
    https://doi.org/10.2307/473375 [Google Scholar]
  91. Power, M.J
    1985 Sentence production and working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology37(3). 367–385. doi: 10.1080/14640748508400940
    https://doi.org/10.1080/14640748508400940 [Google Scholar]
  92. Preston, Dennis R
    2004 Three kinds of sociolinguistics: A psycholinguistic perspective. In Carmen Fought (ed.), Sociolinguistic variation: Critical reflections, 140–158. Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  93. Saito, Satoru & Alan D. Baddeley
    2004 Irrelevant sound disrupts speech production: Exploring the relationship between short-term memory and experimentally induced slips of the tongue. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A57(7). 1309–1340. doi: 10.1080/02724980343000783
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980343000783 [Google Scholar]
  94. Sankoff, D. & S. Laberge
    1978 Statistical dependence among successive occurrences of a variable in discourse. In David Sankoff (ed.), Linguistic variation: Models and methods, chap. 8, 119–126. Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  95. Scarborough, Rebecca
    2013 Neighborhood-conditioned patterns in phonetic detail: Relating coarticulation and hyperarticulation. Journal of Phonetics41(6). 491–508. doi: 10.1016/j.wocn.2013.09.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2013.09.004 [Google Scholar]
  96. Scherre, M.M.P
    2001 Phrase-level parallelism effect on noun phrase number agreement. Language Variation and Change13(01). 91–107. doi: 10.1017/S0954394501131042
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394501131042 [Google Scholar]
  97. Scherre, M.M.P. & A.J. Naro
    1991 Marking in discourse: “Birds of a feather”. Language Variation and Change3. 23–32. doi: 10.1017/S0954394500000430
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394500000430 [Google Scholar]
  98. Selkirk, Elisabeth O
    1986 On derived domains in sentence phonology. Phonology Yearbook3. 371–405. doi: 10.1017/S0952675700000695
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675700000695 [Google Scholar]
  99. Sharma, Devyani & Ben Rampton
    2011 Lectal focusing in interaction: A new methodology for the study of superdiverse speech. Queen Mary’s Occasional Papers Advancing Linguistics2. 1–22.
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Slevc, L. Robert
    2011 Saying what’s on your mind: Working memory effects on sentence production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition37(6). 1503–1514. doi: 10.1037/a0024350
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024350 [Google Scholar]
  101. Squires, Lauren
    2013 It don’t go both ways: Limited bidirectionality in sociolinguistic perception. Journal of Sociolinguistics17(2). 200–237. doi: 10.1111/josl.12025
    https://doi.org/10.1111/josl.12025 [Google Scholar]
  102. Stuart-Smith, Jane , Gwilym Pryce , Claire Timmins & Barrie Gunter
    2013 Television can also be a factor in language change: Evidence from an urban dialect. Language89(3). 501–536. doi: 10.1353/lan.2013.0041
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2013.0041 [Google Scholar]
  103. Szmrecsanyi, Benedikt
    2006Morphosyntactic persistence in spoken English: A corpus study at the intersection of variationist sociolinguistics, psycholinguistics, and discourse analysis. Mouton de Gruyter. doi: 10.1515/9783110197808
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110197808 [Google Scholar]
  104. Tagliamonte, Sali
    2004 Someth[in]’s go[ing] on!: Variable ing at ground zero. In G. Eklund S. Fidell , L.H. Hansen , A. Karstadt , B. Nordberg , E. Sundergren , M. Thelander , B.-L. Gunnarsson , & L. Bergström (ed.), Language variation in Europe: Papers from the Second International Conference on Language Variation in Europe, ICLAVE 2, 390–403. Department of Scandinavian Languages, Uppsala University.
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Tagliamonte, Sali & Rosalind Temple
    2005 New perspectives on an ol’ variable: (t,d) in British English. Language Variation and Change17. 281–302.
    [Google Scholar]
  106. Tamminga, Meredith
    2014Persistence in the production of linguistic variation. University of Pennsylvania dissertation.
    [Google Scholar]
  107. 2015 Modulation of the following segment effect on coronal stop deletion. Paper presented at NWAV 44 , University of Toronto, October 23.
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Tamminga, Meredith & Josef Fruehwald
    2013 Deconstructing TD deletion. Paper presented at NWAV 42 , University of Pittsburgh/Carnegie Mellon University, October 17–20.
    [Google Scholar]
  109. Tamminga, Meredith & Laurel MacKenzie
    2014 Elaborating extragrammatical effects on variation. Paper presented at the Linguistics Society of America annual meeting , Minneapolis.
    [Google Scholar]
  110. Tamminga, Meredith , Christopher Ahern & Aaron Ecay
    2016 Generalized additive mixed models for intraspeaker variation. Linguistics Vanguard2(s1). 33–41. doi: 10.1515/lingvan‑2016‑0030
    https://doi.org/10.1515/lingvan-2016-0030 [Google Scholar]
  111. Tanner, James , Morgan Sonderegger & Michael Wagner
    2015 Production planning and coronal stop deletion in spontaneous speech. Paper presented at the International Congress of Phonetic Sciences , Glasgow, UK, August 9.
    [Google Scholar]
  112. Tilsen, Sam
    2012 Utterance preparation and stress clash: Planning prosodic alternations. In Suzanne Fuchs , Melanie Weirich , Daniel Pape , & Pascal Perrier (eds.), Speech production and perception vol. 1: Speech planning and dynamics, 119–156. Frankfurt: Peter Lang.
    [Google Scholar]
  113. Torgerson, Eivind & Paul Kerswill
    2004 Internal and external motivation in phonetic change: Dialect levelling outcomes for an English vowel shift. Journal of Sociolinguistics8(1). 23–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9841.2004.00250.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9841.2004.00250.x [Google Scholar]
  114. Travis, Catherine E
    2007 Genre effects on subject expression in Spanish: Priming in narrative and conversation. Language Variation and Change19. 101–135. doi: 10.1017/S0954394507070081
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394507070081 [Google Scholar]
  115. Wagner, Michael
    2011 Production planning constraints on allomorphy. Canadian Acoustics39(3).
    [Google Scholar]
  116. Wagner, Suzanne Evans
    2012 Real-time evidence for age-grad(ing) in late adolescence. Language Variation and Change24. 179–202. doi: 10.1017/S0954394512000099
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394512000099 [Google Scholar]
  117. Weiner, E. Judith & William Labov
    1983 Constraints on the agentless passive. Journal of Linguistics19(1). 29–58. doi: 10.1017/S0022226700007441
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226700007441 [Google Scholar]
  118. Weinreich, Uriel , William Labov & Marvin I. Herzog
    1968 Empirical foundations for a theory of language change. In W.P. Lehmann & Yakov Malkiel (eds.), Directions for historical linguistics: A symposium, 97–195. Austin and London: University of Texas Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  119. Zellner, Brigitte
    1994 Pauses and the temporal structure of speech. In É ric Keller (ed.), Fundamentals of speech synthesis and speech recognition, 41–62. Chichester: Wiley-Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  120. Zellou, Georgia , Delphine Dahan & David Embick
    2016Imitation of coarticulatory vowel nasality across words and time. Ms., U.C. Davis and University of Pennsylvania.
    [Google Scholar]
  121. Zsiga, Elizabeth C
    2000 Phonetic alignment constraints: Consonant overlap and palatalization in English and Russian. Journal of Phonetics28(1). 69–102. doi: 10.1006/jpho.2000.0109
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.2000.0109 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/lv.16.2.06tam
Loading
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error