1887
Volume 14, Issue 2
  • ISSN 1871-1340
  • E-ISSN: 1871-1375
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Hebrew verbs were analyzed in the peer talk produced by 36 Hebrew-speaking children in two age/schooling groups (4;0–5;0 and 5;0–6;0 years), and from two socio-economic backgrounds (SES), mid-high and low. Each of the four age/SES groups consisted of nine children in three triads, where each triad was recorded for 30 minutes while playing. The interface of lexical and morphological growth was demonstrated in the developing organization of verbs in terms of roots, conjugations and derivational families. SES was found the major source of variation in all measures, indicating a smaller and less specific verb lexicon in the low SES groups. Network analyses, a novel methodological approach, revealed the internal structure of the verb category in each age/SES cell, pointing to a scarce and less complex verb lexicon of the low SES groups. These measures also accounted for the growth potential of the network, increasing from the younger low SES group at one pole and peaking in the older mid-high SES at the other pole. These quantitative and qualitative differences in the morphological make-up of the verb lexicon and its usage patterns in preschool peer talk have implications for the impact of SES on verb learning in Hebrew.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ml.00006.lev
2020-01-15
2023-06-01
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Ackerman, F., and Malouf, R.
    (2013) Morphological organization: The low conditional entropy conjecture. Language, 89(3), 429–464. 10.1353/lan.2013.0054
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2013.0054 [Google Scholar]
  2. Aram, D., and Biron, S.
    (2004) Joint storybook reading and joint writing interventions among low SES preschoolers: Differential contributions to early literacy. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19, 588–610. 10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.10.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.10.003 [Google Scholar]
  3. Armon-Lotem, S., and Berman, R. A.
    (2003) The emergence of grammar: Early verbs and beyond. Journal of Child Language, 30(4), 845–877. 10.1017/S0305000903005750
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000903005750 [Google Scholar]
  4. Aronoff, M.
    (1976) Morphology in generative grammar. Cambridge, Mass: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Arriaga, R. I., Fenson, L., Cronan, T., and Pethick, S. J.
    (1998) Scores on the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory of children from low and middle-income families. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(2), 209–223. 10.1017/S0142716400010043
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400010043 [Google Scholar]
  6. Asaridou, S. S., Demir-Lira, Ö. E., Goldin-Meadow, S., and Small, S. L.
    (2017) The pace of vocabulary growth during preschool predicts cortical structure at school age. Neuropsychologia, 98, 13–23. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.05.018
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.05.018 [Google Scholar]
  7. Ashkenazi, O.
    (2015) Input-output relations in the early acquisition of the Hebrew verb. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv.
  8. Ashkenazi, O., Gillis, S., and Ravid, D.
    (In press). Input-output relations in Hebrew verb acquisition at the morpho-lexical interface. Journal of Child Language. doi:  10.1017/S0305000919000540
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000919000540 [Google Scholar]
  9. Ashkenazi, O., Ravid, D., and Gillis, S.
    (2016) Breaking into the Hebrew verb system: A learning problem. First Language, 36(5), 505–524. 10.1177/0142723716648865
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723716648865 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bat-El, O.
    (2003) Semitic verb structure within a universal perspective. Language Acquisition and Language Disorders, 28, 29–60. 10.1075/lald.28.02bat
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.28.02bat [Google Scholar]
  11. Beckage, N., Smith, L., and Hills, T.
    (2010) Semantic network connectivity is related to vocabulary growth rate in children. Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 32, 2769–2774.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. (2011) Small worlds and semantic network growth in typical and late talkers. PloS One, 6, e19348. 10.1371/journal.pone.0019348
    https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0019348 [Google Scholar]
  13. Bell, M., Perera, S., Piraveenan, M., Bliemer, M., Latty, T., and Reid, C.
    (2017) Network growth models: A behavioral basis for attachment proportional to fitness. Scientific Reports (Nature Publisher Group), 7, 42431.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Ben-Zvi, G., and Levie, R.
    (2016) Development of Hebrew Derivational Morphology from Preschool to Adolescence. InR. A. Berman (Ed.), Acquisition and Development of Hebrew: From Infancy to Adolescence (pp.135–173). TILAR (Trends in Language Acquisition Research) series. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 10.1075/tilar.19.05ben
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.19.05ben [Google Scholar]
  15. Berman, R. A.
    (1978) Modern Hebrew structure. University Pub. Projects.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. (1980) Child language as evidence for grammatical description: Preschoolers’ construal of transitivity in Hebrew. Linguistics, 18, 677–701. 10.1515/ling.1980.18.7‑8.677
    https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1980.18.7-8.677 [Google Scholar]
  17. (1982) Verb-pattern alternation: the interface of morphology, syntax, and semantics in Hebrew child language. Journal of child language, 9(1), 169–191. 10.1017/S030500090000369X
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500090000369X [Google Scholar]
  18. (1993a) Developmental perspectives on transitivity: A confluence of cues. InY. Levy (Ed.), Other children, other languages: Issues in the theory of acquisition (pp.189–241). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. (1993b) Marking of verb transitivity by Hebrew-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 20, 641–669. 10.1017/S0305000900008527
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008527 [Google Scholar]
  20. (2003) Children’s lexical innovations: Developmental perspectives on Hebrew verb structure. InJ. Shimron (Ed.), Language processing and acquisition in languages of Semitic, root-based morphology (pp.243–291). Amsterdam: Benjamins. 10.1075/lald.28.13ber
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.28.13ber [Google Scholar]
  21. (2012) Revisiting roots in Hebrew: A multi-faceted view [in English]. InM. Muchnik & Z. Sadan, Studies on Modern Hebrew and Jewish Languages in Honor of Ora (Rodriguez) Schwarzwald (pp.132–158) [in Hebrew]. Jerusalem: Carmel Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Berman, R. A., Nayditz, R., and Ravid, D.
    (2011) Linguistic diagnostics of written texts in two school-age populations. Written Language and Literacy, 14, 161–187. 10.1075/wll.14.2.01ber
    https://doi.org/10.1075/wll.14.2.01ber [Google Scholar]
  23. Berman, R. A., and Nir-Sagiv, B.
    (2007) Comparing narrative and expository text construction across adolescence: A developmental paradox. Discourse processes, 43(2), 79–120. 10.1080/01638530709336894
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01638530709336894 [Google Scholar]
  24. Blachman, B. A., Tangel, D., Ball, E. W., Black, R., and Mcgraw, C. K.
    (1999) Developing phonological awareness and word recognition skills: A two-year intervention with low-income, inner-city children. Reading and Writing, 11, 239–273. 10.1023/A:1008050403932
    https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1008050403932 [Google Scholar]
  25. Black, E., Peppé, S., and Gibbon, F.
    (2008) The relationship between socio-economic status and lexical development. Clinical linguistics & phonetics, 22(4;0–5;0), 259–265. 10.1080/02699200801918887
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699200801918887 [Google Scholar]
  26. Bleses, D., Makransky, G., Dale, P. S., Højen, A., and Ari, B. A.
    (2016) Early productive vocabulary predicts academic achievement 10 years later. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(6), 1461–1476. 10.1017/S0142716416000060
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716416000060 [Google Scholar]
  27. Blum-Kulka, S., Hamo, M., and Habib, T.
    (2010) Explanations in naturally occurring peer talk: Conversational emergence and function, thematic scope, and contribution to the development of discursive skills. First Language, 30(3–4), 440–460. 10.1177/0142723710370528
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723710370528 [Google Scholar]
  28. Bolozky, S.
    (1978) Word formation strategies in the Hebrew verb system. Afroasiatic Linguistics, 5(3), 1–26.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. (1996) 501 Hebrew verbs: Fully conjugated in all the tenses in a new easy-to-follow format alphabetically arranged by root. Hauppauge, N.Y: Barron’s Educational Series.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. (1999) Measuring productivity in word formation: The case of Israeli Hebrew. Boston: Brill. 10.1163/9789004348431
    https://doi.org/10.1163/9789004348431 [Google Scholar]
  31. Bybee, J.
    (2007) Frequency of use and the organization of language. Oxford University Press on Demand. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195301571.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195301571.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  32. Chan, E.
    (2008) Structures and distributions in morphology learning. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania.
  33. Chiu, M. M., and McBride-Chang, C.
    (2006) Gender, context, and reading: A comparison of students in 43 countries. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 331–362. 10.1207/s1532799xssr1004_1
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s1532799xssr1004_1 [Google Scholar]
  34. D’angiulli, A., Lipina, S. J., and Olesinska, A.
    (2012) Explicit and implicit issues in the developmental cognitive neuroscience of social inequality. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 254.
    [Google Scholar]
  35. Dattner, E.
    (2015) Enabling and allowing in hebrew: A usage-based construction grammar account. InB. Nolan, G. Rawoens & E. Diedrichsen (Eds.), Causation, permission, and transfer: Argument realization in GET, TAKE, PUT, GIVE and LET verbs (pp.271–293). Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. (2019) The Hebrew dative: Usage patterns as discourse profile constructions. Linguistics57(5). 1073–1110. doi:  10.1515/ling‑2019‑0022
    https://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2019-0022 [Google Scholar]
  37. Dattner, E., Kertes, L., Zwilling, R., and Ravid, D.
    (2019) Usage patterns in the development of Hebrew grammatical subjects. Glossa: a journal of general linguistics. doi:  10.5334/gjgl.928
    https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.928 [Google Scholar]
  38. Elman, J. L.
    (1993) Learning and development in neural networks: The importance of starting small. Cognition, 48, 71–99. 10.1016/0010‑0277(93)90058‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(93)90058-4 [Google Scholar]
  39. Engel, P. M. J., Santos, F. H., and Gathercole, S. E.
    (2008) Are working memory measures free of socioeconomic influence?Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51(6), 1580–1587. 10.1044/1092‑4388(2008/07‑0210)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2008/07-0210) [Google Scholar]
  40. Farkas, G., and Beron, K.
    (2004) The detailed age trajectory of oral vocabulary knowledge: Differences by class and race. Social Science Research, 33(3), 464–497. 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2003.08.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssresearch.2003.08.001 [Google Scholar]
  41. Fernald, A., Marchman, V. A., and Weisleder, A.
    (2013) SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Developmental Science, 16, 234–248. 10.1111/desc.12019
    https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12019 [Google Scholar]
  42. Fish, M., and Pinkerman, B.
    (2003) Language skills in low-SES rural Appalachian children: Normative development and individual differences, infancy to preschool. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 23(5), 539–565. 10.1016/S0193‑3973(02)00141‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0193-3973(02)00141-7 [Google Scholar]
  43. Forrester, M. A., and Cherrington, S.
    (2009) The development of other-related conversational skills: A case study of conversational repair during the early years. First Language, 29, 167–192. 10.1177/0142723708094452
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723708094452 [Google Scholar]
  44. Franklin, B., Warlaumont, A. S., Messinger, D., Bene, E., Nathani Iyer, S., Lee, C. C., … and Oller, D. K.
    (2014) Effects of parental interaction on infant vocalization rate, variability and vocal type. Language Learning and Development, 10(3), 279–296. 10.1080/15475441.2013.849176
    https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2013.849176 [Google Scholar]
  45. Frost, R., Forster, K. I., and Deutsch, A.
    (1997) What can we learn from the morphology of Hebrew? A masked-priming investigation of morphological representation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(4), 829–856.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. Ginsborg, J.
    (2006) The effects of socio-economic status on children’s language acquisition and use. InJ. Clegg & J. Ginsborg (Eds.), Language and social disadvantage: Theory into practice (pp.9–27). London: Wiley.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Goldin-Meadow, S., Levine, S. C., Hedges, L. V., Huttenlocher, J., Raudenbush, S. W., and Small, S. L.
    (2014) New evidence about language and cognitive development based on a longitudinal study: Hypotheses for intervention. American Psychologist, 69(6), 588–599. 10.1037/a0036886
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036886 [Google Scholar]
  48. Golinkoff, R. M., Hoff, E., Rowe, M. L., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., and Hirsh-Pasek, K.
    (2018) Language Matters: Denying the Existence of the 30-Million-Word Gap Has Serious Consequences. Child Development, 26(7), 1071–1083.
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Gosso, Y., e Morais, M. D. L. S., and Otta, E.
    (2007) Pretend play of Brazilian children: A window into different cultural worlds. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(5), 539–558. 10.1177/0022022107305237
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022107305237 [Google Scholar]
  50. Hart, B., and Risley, T. R.
    (2003) The early catastrophe: The 30 million word gap by age 3. American Educator, 27(1), 4–9.
    [Google Scholar]
  51. Heppt, B., Haag, N., Böhme, K., and Stanat, P.
    (2015) The role of academic-language features for reading comprehension of language-minority students and students from low-SES families. Reading Research Quarterly, 50(1), 61–82. 10.1002/rrq.83
    https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.83 [Google Scholar]
  52. Hills, T. T., Maouene, M., Maouene, J., Sheya, A., and Smith, L.
    (2009) Longitudinal analysis of early semantic networks: Preferential attachment or preferential acquisition?. Psychological science, 20(6), 729–739. 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.2009.02365.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02365.x [Google Scholar]
  53. Hoff, E.
    (2013) Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low-SES and language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental Psychology, 49(1), 4–14. 10.1037/a0027238
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027238 [Google Scholar]
  54. James, G., Witten, D., Hastie, T., and Tibshirani, R.
    (2013) An introduction to statistical learning. New York: springer. 10.1007/978‑1‑4614‑7138‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-7138-7 [Google Scholar]
  55. Jarrett, R. L., Bahar, O. S., and Kersh, R. T.
    (2016) “When we do sit down together” family meal times in low-income African American families with preschoolers. Journal of Family Issues, 37(11), 1483–1513. 10.1177/0192513X14547417
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0192513X14547417 [Google Scholar]
  56. Karmiloff-Smith, A.
    (1992) Beyond modularity: A developmental perspective of cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Kiparsky, P.
    (1982) Explanation in phonology. Walter de Gruyter. 10.1515/9783111666242
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783111666242 [Google Scholar]
  58. Kishiyama, M. M., Boyce, W. T., Jimenez, A. M., Perry, L. M., and Knight, R. T.
    (2009) Socioeconomic disparities affect prefrontal function in children. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 21(6), 1106–1115. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21101
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21101 [Google Scholar]
  59. Kolaczyk, E. D.
    (2009) Statistical Analysis of Network Data: Methods and models. Springer. 10.1007/978‑0‑387‑88146‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-0-387-88146-1 [Google Scholar]
  60. Kyratzis, A., and Tarım, Ş. D.
    (2010) Using directives to construct egalitarian or hierarchical social organization: Turkish middle-class preschool girls’ socialization about gender, affect, and context in peer group conversations. First Language, 30(3–4), 473–492. 10.1177/0142723710370547
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723710370547 [Google Scholar]
  61. Levie, R., Ashkenazi, O., Eitan Stanzas, S., Zwilling, R., Raz, E., Hershkovitz, L., and Ravid, D.
    (Under revision). The route to the Hebrew root-based verb family. Morphology.
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Levie, R., Ben-Zvi, G., and Ravid, D.
    (2017) Morpho-lexical development in language impaired and typically developing Hebrew-speaking children from two SES backgrounds. Reading and Writing, 30(5), 1035–1064. 10.1007/s11145‑016‑9711‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-016-9711-3 [Google Scholar]
  63. Levie, R., Dattner, E., Peleg, S., Peleg, O., and Ravid, D.
    (Submitted). Input to infants in the first year of life: A double case-study of two mothers from different socio-economic backgrounds. Journal of Child Language.
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Lieven, E.
    (2010) Input and first language acquisition: Evaluating the role of frequency. Lingua, 120(11), 2546–2556. 10.1016/j.lingua.2010.06.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.06.005 [Google Scholar]
  65. Lignos, C., and Yang, C.
    (2017) Morphology and language acquisition. InA. Hippsley and G. Stump (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Morphology (pp.765–791). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Lundberg, I., Larsman, P., and Strid, A.
    (2012) Development of phonological awareness during the preschool year: The influence of gender and socio-economic status. Reading and writing, 25(2), 305–320. 10.1007/s11145‑010‑9269‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-010-9269-4 [Google Scholar]
  67. Ministry of Education
    Ministry of Education (2006) National curriculum: Afoundation towards reading and writing in Israeli kindergarten. Jerusalem [in Hebrew].
    [Google Scholar]
  68. Morgan, P. L., Farkas, G., Hillemeier, M. M., Hammer, C. S., and Maczuga, S.
    (2015) 24-month-old children with larger oral vocabularies display greater academic and behavioral functioning at kindergarten entry. Child Development, 86(5), 1351–1370. 10.1111/cdev.12398
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12398 [Google Scholar]
  69. National Statistical Report of Israel
    National Statistical Report of Israel (2012) Society in Israel: Socio-Economic Gaps. Report No. 5. Jerusalem, Israeli Central Bureau of Statistics [in Hebrew].
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Neuman, S. B., and Celano, D.
    (2001) Access to print in low-income and middle-income communities: An ecological study of four neighborhoods. Reading Research Quarterly, 36(1), 8–26. 10.1598/RRQ.36.1.1
    https://doi.org/10.1598/RRQ.36.1.1 [Google Scholar]
  71. Newman, R. S., Rowe, M. L., and Ratner, N. B.
    (2016) Input and uptake at 7 months predict toddler vocabulary: The role of child-directed speech and infant processing skills in language development. Journal of Child Language, 43(5), 1158–1173. 10.1017/S0305000915000446
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000915000446 [Google Scholar]
  72. Nittrouer, S.
    (1996) The relation between speech perception and phonemic awareness: Evidence from low-SES children and children with chronic OM. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 1059–1071. 10.1044/jshr.3905.1059
    https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3905.1059 [Google Scholar]
  73. Noble, K. G., Farah, M. J., and McCandliss, B. M.
    (2006) Socioeconomic background modulates cognition-achievement relationships in reading. Cognitive Development, 21(3), 349–368. 10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.01.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.01.007 [Google Scholar]
  74. Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Kan, E., and Sowell, E. R.
    (2012) Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Developmental Science, 15(4), 516–527. 10.1111/j.1467‑7687.2012.01147.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01147.x [Google Scholar]
  75. Oller, D. K., Eilers, R. E., Steffens, M. L., Lynch, M. P., and Urbano, R.
    (1994) Speech-like vocalizations in infancy: an evaluation of potential risk factors. Journal of Child Language, 21(1), 33–58. 10.1017/S0305000900008667
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008667 [Google Scholar]
  76. Qi, C. H., Kaiser, A. P., Milan, S., and Hancock, T.
    (2006) Language performance of low-Income African American and European American preschool children on the PPVT-III. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 37, 5–16. 10.1044/0161‑1461(2006/002)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2006/002) [Google Scholar]
  77. Ragnarsdóttir, H., Simonsen, H. G., and Plunkett, K.
    (1999) The acquisition of past tense morphology in Icelandic and Norwegian children: An experimental study. Journal of child language, 26(3), 577–618. 10.1017/S0305000999003918
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000999003918 [Google Scholar]
  78. Ravid, D. D.
    (1995) Language change in child and adult Hebrew: A psycholinguistic perspective. Oxford University Press on Demand.
    [Google Scholar]
  79. Ravid, D.
    (2003) A developmental perspective on root perception in Hebrew and Palestinian Arabic. Language Acquisition and Language Disorders, 28, 293–320. 10.1075/lald.28.14rav
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.28.14rav [Google Scholar]
  80. (2012) Spelling Morphology: The psycholinguistics of Hebrew spelling. New York: Springer. 10.1007/978‑1‑4419‑0588‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0588-8 [Google Scholar]
  81. (In press). Derivational morphology. InR. A. Berman Ed. Usage-Based Studies in Modern Hebrew: Background, Morpho-lexicon and Syntax. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
    [Google Scholar]
  82. Ravid, D., Ashkenazi, O., Levie, R., Ben Zadok, G., Grunwald, T., Bratslavsky, R. and Gillis, S.
    (2016) Foundations of the early root category: Analyses of linguistic input to Hebrew-speaking children. InR. A. Berman (Ed.), Acquisition and development of Hebrew: From infancy to adolescence (pp.95–134). Trends in Language Acquisition Research series. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 10.1075/tilar.19.04rav
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.19.04rav [Google Scholar]
  83. Ravid, D., Keuleers, E., and Dressler, W. U.
    (in press) Emergence and early development of lexicon and morphology. InW. U. Dressler, I. Plag and V. Pirrelli (Eds.), A cross-disciplinary guide to the (mental) lexicon: structure, meaning, acquisition and processing. Berlin: De Gruyter.
    [Google Scholar]
  84. Ravid, D., and Schiff, R.
    (2006) Morphological abilities in Hebrew-speaking gradeschoolers from two socioeconomic backgrounds: An analogy task. First Language, 26(4), 381–402. 10.1177/0142723706064828
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723706064828 [Google Scholar]
  85. Ravid, D., Shalom, T., Dattner, E., Katzenberger, I., and Sha’ashua, G.
    (2015) Top-down measures in 7th grade writing: the effects of genre and SES. InJ. Perera Parramon, M. Aparici, E. Rosado, & N. Salas (Eds.) Written and spoken language development across the lifespan: Essays in honor of Liliana Tolchinsky (pp.345–359). New York: Springer.
    [Google Scholar]
  86. Ravid, D., and Vered, L.
    (2017) Hebrew verbal passives in Later Language Development: the interface of register and verb morphology. Journal of child language, 44(6), 1309–1336. 10.1017/S0305000916000544
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000916000544 [Google Scholar]
  87. Ravid, D. and Zimmerman, A.
    (2017) Maternal input at 1;6: A comparison of two mothers from different SES backgrounds. InN. Ketrez, A. C. Küntay, Ş. Özçalışkan & A. Özyürek (Eds.), Social environment and cognition in language development: Studies in honor of Ayhan Aksu-Koç (pp.35–52). Trends in Language Acquisition Research series. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 10.1075/tilar.21.03rav
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.21.03rav [Google Scholar]
  88. Reynolds, E., Stagnitti, K., and Kidd, E.
    (2011) Play, language and social skills of children attending a play-based curriculum school and a traditionally structured classroom curriculum school in low socioeconomic areas. Australasian journal of early childhood, 36(4), 120–130. 10.1177/183693911103600416
    https://doi.org/10.1177/183693911103600416 [Google Scholar]
  89. Romeo, R. R., Leonard, J. A., Robinson, S. T., West, M. R., Mackey, A. P., Rowe, M. L., and Gabrieli, J. D. E.
    (2018) Beyond the “30 million word gap:” Children’s conversational exposure is associated with language-related brain function. Psychological Science, 29(5), 700–710. 10.1177/0956797617742725
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617742725 [Google Scholar]
  90. Rowe, M. L.
    (2012) A longitudinal investigation of the role of quantity and quality of child-directed speech in vocabulary development. Child Development, 83(5), 1762–1774. 10.1111/j.1467‑8624.2012.01805.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01805.x [Google Scholar]
  91. Rowe, M. L., and Goldin-Meadow, S.
    (2009) Differences in early gesture explain SES disparities in child vocabulary size at school entry. Science, 323(5916), 951–953. 10.1126/science.1167025
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1167025 [Google Scholar]
  92. Saiegh-Haddad, E., Hadieh, A., and Ravid, D.
    (2012) Acquiring noun plurals in Palestinian Arabic: Morphology, familiarity, and pattern frequency. Language Learning, 62, 1024–1051. 10.1111/j.1467‑9922.2012.00727.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2012.00727.x [Google Scholar]
  93. Schiff, R., and Ravid, D.
    (2012) Linguistic processing in Hebrew-speaking children from low and high SES backgrounds. Reading and Writing, 25(6), 1427–1448. 10.1007/s11145‑011‑9326‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9326-7 [Google Scholar]
  94. Schuele, C. M.
    (2010) The many things language sample analysis has taught me. Perspectives on Language Learning and Education, 17(1), 32–37. 10.1044/lle17.1.32
    https://doi.org/10.1044/lle17.1.32 [Google Scholar]
  95. Schwarzwald, O. R.
    (2001) Modern Hebrew. Munich: Lincom Europa.
    [Google Scholar]
  96. (2002) Modern Hebrew morphology. Tel Aviv: The Open University [in Hebrew].
    [Google Scholar]
  97. Serratrice, L.
    (2005) The role of discourse pragmatics in the acquisition of subjects in Italian. Applied psycholinguistics, 26(3), 437–462. 10.1017/S0142716405050241
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716405050241 [Google Scholar]
  98. Sirin, S. R.
    (2005) Socioeconomic status and academic achievement: A meta-analytic review of research. Review of Educational Research, 75, 417–453. 10.3102/00346543075003417
    https://doi.org/10.3102/00346543075003417 [Google Scholar]
  99. Sivan, R.
    (1976) Contemporary Hebrew. Jerusalem: Rubinstein.
    [Google Scholar]
  100. Song, L., Spier, E. T., and Tamis-Lemonda, C. S.
    (2014) Reciprocal influences between maternal language and children’s language and cognitive development in low-income families. Journal of Child Language, 41(2), 305–326. 10.1017/S0305000912000700
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000912000700 [Google Scholar]
  101. Steyvers, M., and Tenenbaum, J. B.
    (2004) The large-scale structure of semantic networks: statistical analyses and a model of semantic growth. ‏Cognitive Science, 29(1), 41–78. 10.1207/s15516709cog2901_3
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2901_3 [Google Scholar]
  102. Strauss, S., & Stavy, R.
    (1982) U-shaped behavioral growth. Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  103. Sverdlov, A., Aram, D., and Levin, I.
    (2014) Kindergarten Teachers’ Literacy Beliefs and Self-Reported Practices: On the heels of a new national literacy curriculum. Teaching and Teacher Education, 39, 44–55. 10.1016/j.tate.2013.12.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tate.2013.12.004 [Google Scholar]
  104. Tomasello, M.
    (2003) Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  105. Tremblay-Leveau, H., Leclerc, S., and Nadel, J.
    (1999) Linguistic skills of 16-and 23-month-old twins and singletons in a triadic context. First Language, 19(56), 233–254. 10.1177/014272379901905605
    https://doi.org/10.1177/014272379901905605 [Google Scholar]
  106. Uziel-Karl, S.
    (2003) Motion verbs in the developing lexicon of Hebrew-speaking children. InE. V. Clark (Ed.), Proceedings of the 31st Stanford Child Language Research Forum (pp.31–39). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
    [Google Scholar]
  107. Uziel-Karl, S., Kanaan, F., Abugov, N., Yifat, R., Meir, I., and Ravid, D.
    (2014) Hebrew and Palestinian Arabic in Israel: Linguistic frameworks and SLP services. Special issue on Global issues in language disorders: Processes, frameworks and policies, Topics in Language Disorders, 34, 133–154.
    [Google Scholar]
  108. Van Steensel, R.
    (2006) Relations between socio-cultural factors, the home literacy environment and children’s literacy development in the first years of primary education. Journal of Research in Reading, 29(4), 367–382. 10.1111/j.1467‑9817.2006.00301.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00301.x [Google Scholar]
  109. Veneziano, E.
    (2010) Conversation in language development and use: An Introduction. First Language, 30(3–4), 241–249. 10.1177/0142723710380531
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723710380531 [Google Scholar]
  110. (2018) Learning conversational skills and learning from conversation. InA. Bar-On & D. Ravid (Eds.), Handbook of Communication Disorders: Theoretical, Empirical, and Applied Linguistic Perspectives (pp.311–328). Berlin, Boston: De Gruyter Mouton. 10.1515/9781614514909‑016
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9781614514909-016 [Google Scholar]
  111. Vitevitch, M. S., and Goldstein, R.
    (2014) Keywords in the mental lexicon. Journal of Memory and Language, 73, 131–147. 10.1016/j.jml.2014.03.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2014.03.005 [Google Scholar]
  112. Walker, D., Greenwood, C., Hart, B., and Carta, J.
    (1994) Prediction of school outcomes based on early language production and socioeconomic factors. Child Development, 65, 606–621. 10.2307/1131404
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1131404 [Google Scholar]
  113. Weissblay, I., and Weininger, A.
    (2015) The Education System in Israel: Selected Issues in the Field of the Education, Culture and Sport Committee of the Knesset. Knesset, Research and Information Center: Jerusalem [in Hebrew].
    [Google Scholar]
http://instance.metastore.ingenta.com/content/journals/10.1075/ml.00006.lev
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.00006.lev
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error