1887
New Questions for the Next Decade
  • ISSN 1871-1340
  • E-ISSN: 1871-1375
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Objective frequency does not always provide reliable information about lexical distributions across individuals’ development. We propose the subjective ranking by experts of lexical items’ register in the sense of ‘levels of linguistic usage’, which has been independently linked to AoA, as an alternative. This proposal was tested in Hebrew, a language showing marked distinctions between the everyday colloquial style and more formal, historically-related types of expression. A list of over 3,500 Hebrew adjectives in 19 morphological categories was compiled from dictionary sources. All adjectives on the list were ranked on a 1–5 linguistic register scale by 329 language expert judges. A Model Based Latent Class Analysis yielded five high-agreement groups of adjectives with mean register scores from 1.44 to 4.51, taken to represent five developmentally consecutive adjective lexicons. Semantic and morphological analyses indicated a rise in the abstractness and specificity of adjectives in the five lexicons, with concurrent changes in their morphological makeup. Two morphological categories emerged as the major components of the Modern Hebrew adjective lexicon: Resultative patterns, expressing states, and i-suffixed denominals, expressing nominal attributes. The study showed that subjective register classification may constitute a yardstick in development, with implications for other languages where register judgements can apply.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ml.11.3.04rav
2016-12-16
2025-02-09
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Alderson, J.C
    (2007) Judging the frequency of English words. Applied Linguistics, 28(3), 383–409. doi: 10.1093/applin/amm024
    https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/amm024 [Google Scholar]
  2. Andersen, E.S
    (1992) Speaking with style: The sociolinguistic skills of children. London: Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. Auer, E.T. , Bernstein, L.E. , & Tucker, P.E
    (2000) Is subjective word familiarity a meter of ambient language? A natural experiment on effects of perceptual experience. Memory & Cognition, 28, 789–797. doi: 10.3758/BF03198414
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198414 [Google Scholar]
  4. Avneyon, E
    (2007) The Sapir Hebrew-Hebrew dictionary. Or Yehuda: Hed Artzi. [in Hebrew]
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Baayen, R.H. , Feldman, L.B. , & Schreuder, R
    (2006) Morphological influences on the recognition of monosyllabic monomorphemic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 290–313. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.008 [Google Scholar]
  6. Balota, D.A. , Pilotti, M. , & Cortese, M.J
    (2001) Subjective frequency estimates for 2,938 monosyllabic words. Memory & Cognition, 29, 639–647. doi: 10.3758/BF03200465
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200465 [Google Scholar]
  7. Balota, D. , Cortese, M. , Sergent-Marshall, S. , Spieler, D. , & Yap, M
    (2004) Visual word recognition for single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133(2), 283–316. doi: 10.1037/0096‑3445.133.2.283
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.283 [Google Scholar]
  8. Bar-Ilan, L. & Berman, R.A
    (2007) Developing register differentiation: The Latinate-Germanic divide in English. Linguistics, 45, 1–36. doi: 10.1515/LING.2007.001
    https://doi.org/10.1515/LING.2007.001 [Google Scholar]
  9. Bar-On, A. , & Ravid, D
    (2011) Morphological decoding in Hebrew pseudowords: A developmental study. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 553–581. doi: 10.1017/S014271641100021X
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S014271641100021X [Google Scholar]
  10. Bar-On, A. , Dattner, E. , & Ravid, D
    . (Submitted). Resolving homography in reading Hebrew garden-path sentences: A developmental perspective.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Benelli, N. , & Grinboim, Y
    (2015) Between familiarity and frequency: A psycholinguistic study of Hebrew adjectives. Tel Aviv University ms. [in Hebrew]
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Berman, R.A
    (1978) Modern Hebrew structure. Tel Aviv: University Publishing Projects.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. (1985) Acquisition of Hebrew. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. (1993) Marking of verb transitivity by Hebrew-speaking children. Journal of Child Language, 20, 641–669. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900008527
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008527 [Google Scholar]
  15. (1994) Formal, lexical, and semantic factors in acquisition of Hebrew resultative participles. Berkeley Linguistic Society, 20, 82–92. doi: 10.3765/bls.v20i1.1452
    https://doi.org/10.3765/bls.v20i1.1452 [Google Scholar]
  16. (2004) Between emergence and mastery: The long developmental route of language acquisition. In R.A. Berman (Ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence: Psycholinguistic and crosslinguistic perspectives (pp.9–34). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/tilar.3.05ber
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.3.05ber [Google Scholar]
  17. Berman, R.A. , & Ravid, D
    (2009) Becoming a literate language user: Oral and written text construction across adolescence. In D.R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of literacy (pp.92–111). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511609664.007
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511609664.007 [Google Scholar]
  18. Biber, D. , & Conrad, S
    (2009) Register, genre, and style. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511814358
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511814358 [Google Scholar]
  19. Blackwell, A.A
    (2005) Acquiring the English adjective lexicon: Relationships with input properties and semantic typology. Journal of Child Language, 32, 535–562. doi: 10.1017/S0305000905006938
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000905006938 [Google Scholar]
  20. Blodgett, E.G. , & Cooper, E.B
    (1988) Talking about it and doing it: Metalinguistic capacity and prosodic control in three to seven year olds. Journal of Fluency Disorders, 13, 283–290. doi: 10.1016/0094‑730X(88)90019‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730X(88)90019-8 [Google Scholar]
  21. Brysbaert, M. , & Cortese, M
    (2011) Do the effects of subjective frequency and age of acquisition survive better word frequency norms?The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64(3), 545–559. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.503374
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2010.503374 [Google Scholar]
  22. Brysbaert, M. , & Ghyselinck, M
    (2006) The effect of age of acquisition: Partly frequency related, partly frequency independent. Visual Cognition, 13, 992–1011. doi: 10.1080/13506280544000165
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280544000165 [Google Scholar]
  23. Carroll, J.B
    (1970) An alternative to Juilland’s usage coefficient for lexical frequencies. ETS Research Bulletin Series, 1970(2), i–15.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Caselli, M.C. , Bates E. , Casadio, P. , Fenson, J. , Fenson, L. , Sanderl, L. , & Weir, J
    (1995) A cross-linguistic study of early lexical development. Cognitive Development, 10, 159–199. doi: 10.1016/0885‑2014(95)90008‑X
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(95)90008-X [Google Scholar]
  25. Clark, E.V
    (2003) First language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  26. Diessel, H
    (2007) Frequency effects in language acquisition, language use, and diachronic change. New Ideas in Psychology, 25, 108–127. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.02.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.02.002 [Google Scholar]
  27. Dixon, R.M.W. , & Aikhenvald, A.Y
    (2014) Adjective classes: A cross-linguistic typology. Oxford: Oxford University Press. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198701316.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198701316.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  28. Dockrell, J.E. , & Messer, D
    (2004) Later vocabulary acquisition. In R.A. Berman (Ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence: Psycholinguistic and crosslinguistic perspectives (pp.35–52). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/tilar.3.06doc
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.3.06doc [Google Scholar]
  29. Douglas, F.M
    (2003) The Scottish corpus of texts and speech: Problems of corpus design. Literary and linguistic computing, 18(1), 23–37. doi: 10.1093/llc/18.1.23
    https://doi.org/10.1093/llc/18.1.23 [Google Scholar]
  30. Ferguson, C.A
    (1994) Dialect, register, and genre: Working assumptions about conventionalization. In D. Biber & E. Finegan (Eds.), Sociolinguistic perspectives on register (pp.15–30). New York: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Gai, A
    (1995) The category ‘adjective’ in Semitic languages. Journal of Semitic Studies, 1, 1–9. doi: 10.1093/jss/XL.1.1
    https://doi.org/10.1093/jss/XL.1.1 [Google Scholar]
  32. Gadet, F
    (2009) Stylistic and syntactic variation: Introduction. In K. Beeching , N. Armstrong , & F. Gadet (Eds.)Sociolinguistic variation in contemporary French (pp.115–120). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/impact.26.09gad
    https://doi.org/10.1075/impact.26.09gad [Google Scholar]
  33. Gries, S.T. & Divjak, D
    (Eds.) (2012) Frequency effects in language learning and processing. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton. doi: 10.1515/9783110274059
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110274059 [Google Scholar]
  34. Hora, A. , Avivi-Ben Zvi, G. , Levie, R. , & Ravid, D
    (2006) Acquiring diminutive structures and meanings in Hebrew: an experimental study. In I. Savickiene & W.U. Dressler (Eds.), The acquisition of diminutives (pp.295–317). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/lald.43.13hor
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.43.13hor [Google Scholar]
  35. Itai, A. , & Wintner, S
    (2008) Language resources for Hebrew. Language Resources and Evaluation, 42, 75–98. doi: 10.1007/s10579‑007‑9050‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10579-007-9050-8 [Google Scholar]
  36. Juhasz, B.J
    (2005) Age-of-acquisition effects in word and picture identification. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 684–712. doi: 10.1037/0033‑2909.131.5.684
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.684 [Google Scholar]
  37. Jurafsky, D
    (2003) Probabilistic modeling in psycholinguistics: Linguistic comprehension and production. In R. Bod , J. Hay , & S. Jannedy (Eds.), Probability theory in linguistics (pp.39–96). Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Kennedy, G
    (2014) An introduction to corpus linguistics. London & New York: Routledge.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Kuperman, V. , & Van Dyke, J.A
    (2013) Reassessing word frequency as a determinant of word recognition for skilled and unskilled readers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Performance and Perception, 39, 802–823. doi: 10.1037/a0030859
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030859 [Google Scholar]
  40. Levin, I. , Ravid, D. , & Rappaport, S
    (2001) Morphology and spelling among Hebrew-speaking children: From kindergarten to first grade. Journal of Child Language, 28, 741–769. doi: 10.1017/S0305000901004834
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000901004834 [Google Scholar]
  41. Lo, Y. , Mendell, N. , & Rubin, D
    (2001) Testing the number of components in a normal mixture. Biometrika, 88, 767–778. doi: 10.1093/biomet/88.3.767
    https://doi.org/10.1093/biomet/88.3.767 [Google Scholar]
  42. MacWhinney, B
    (2000) The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk. Vol. 2: The database (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Nippold, M.A
    (2016) Later language development: School-age children, adolescents, and young adults (4th ed.). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Nippold, M.A. , & Duthie, J.K
    (2003) Mental imagery and idiom comprehension: A comparison of school-age children and adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 788–799. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2003/062)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/062) [Google Scholar]
  45. Nylund, K.L. , Asparouhov, T. & Muthen, B
    (2007) Deciding on the number of classes in latent class analysis and growth mixture modeling: A Monte Carlo simulation. Structural Equation Modeling, 14, 535–569. doi: 10.1080/10705510701575396
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10705510701575396 [Google Scholar]
  46. Pitchford, N.J. & Mullen, K.T
    (2001) Conceptualization of perceptual attributes: A special case for color?Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 80, 289–314. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2001.2634
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2001.2634 [Google Scholar]
  47. Ravid, D
    (1990) Internal structure constraints on new-word formation devices in Modern Hebrew. Folia Linguistica, 24, 289–346. doi: 10.1515/flin.1990.24.3‑4.289
    https://doi.org/10.1515/flin.1990.24.3-4.289 [Google Scholar]
  48. (1995) Language change in child and adult Hebrew: A psycholinguistic perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  49. (2003) A developmental perspective on root perception in Hebrew and Palestinian Arabic. In Y. Shimron (Ed.), Language processing and acquisition in languages of Semitic, root-based morphology(pp.293–319). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/lald.28.14rav
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lald.28.14rav [Google Scholar]
  50. (2004) Later lexical development in Hebrew: Derivational morphology revisited. In R.A. Berman (Ed.), Language development across childhood and adolescence: Psycholinguistic and crosslinguistic perspectives (pp.53–82). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/tilar.3.07rav
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.3.07rav [Google Scholar]
  51. (2006) Word-level morphology: A psycholinguistic perspective on linear formation in Hebrew nominals. Morphology, 16, 127–148. doi: 10.1007/s11525‑006‑0006‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-006-0006-2 [Google Scholar]
  52. (2012) Spelling morphology: The psycholinguistics of Hebrew spelling. New York: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978‑1‑4419‑0588‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4419-0588-8 [Google Scholar]
  53. Ravid, D. , & Berman, R.A
    (2009) Developing linguistic register across text types: The case of Modern Hebrew. Pragmatics & Cognition, 17, 108–145. doi: 10.1075/pc.17.1.04rav
    https://doi.org/10.1075/pc.17.1.04rav [Google Scholar]
  54. Ravid, D. & R. Berman
    (2010) Developing noun phrase complexity at school-age: A text-embedded cross-linguistic analysis. First Language, 30, 3–26. doi: 10.1177/0142723709350531
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723709350531 [Google Scholar]
  55. Ravid, D. , & Levie, R
    (2010) Hebrew adjectives in later language text production. First Language, 30, 27–55. doi: 10.1177/0142723709350529
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723709350529 [Google Scholar]
  56. Ravid, D. , & Nir, M
    (2000) On the development of the category of adjective in Hebrew. In M. Beers , B. van den Bogaerde , G. Bol , J. de Jong , & C. Rooijmans (Eds.), From sound to sentence: Studies on first language acquisition (pp.113–124). Groningen: Center for Language and Cognition.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Ravid, D. , & Schiff, R
    (2006) Roots and patterns in Hebrew language development: Evidence from written morphological analogies. Reading and Writing, 19, 789–818. doi: 10.1007/s11145‑006‑9004‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-006-9004-3 [Google Scholar]
  58. Ravid, D. & R. Schiff
    (2012) From dichotomy to divergence: Number/gender marking on Hebrew nouns and adjectives across schoolage. Language Learning, 62, 133–169. doi: 10.1111/j.1467‑9922.2011.00675.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2011.00675.x [Google Scholar]
  59. Ravid, D. , & Shlesinger, Y
    (1987) On the classification and structure of -i suffixed adjectives. Hebrew Linguistics, 25, 59–70. [in Hebrew]
    [Google Scholar]
  60. Ravid, D. , & Vered, L
    . (In press). Learning passive forms as lexical items: Later language development in Hebrew. Journal of Child Language.
    [Google Scholar]
  61. Ravid, D. , & Zilberbuch, S
    (2003) The development of complex nominals in expert and non-expert writing: A comparative study. Pragmatics and Cognition, 11, 267–297. doi: 10.1075/pc.11.2.05rav
    https://doi.org/10.1075/pc.11.2.05rav [Google Scholar]
  62. Ravid, D. , Levie, R. , & Avivi-Ben Zvi, G
    (2003) Morphological disorders. In L. Verhoeven & H. van Balkom (Eds.), Classification of developmental language disorders: Theoretical issues and clinical implications (pp.235–260). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Ravid, D. , Ashkenazi, O. , Levie, R. , Ben Zadok, G. , Grunwald, T. , Bratslavsky, R. , & Gillis, S
    (2016) Foundations of the root category: Analyses of linguistic input to Hebrew-speaking children. In R.A. Berman (Ed.), Acquisition and development of Hebrew: From infancy to adolescence (pp.95–134). Amsterdam: John Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/tilar.19.04rav
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tilar.19.04rav [Google Scholar]
  64. Saiegh-Haddad, E. & Spolsky, B
    (2014) Acquiring literacy in a diglossic context: Problems and prospects. In Saiegh-Haddad, E. & Joshi, M . (Eds.)Handbook of Arabic literacy: Insights and perspectives (pp.225–240). Dordrecht: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978‑94‑017‑8545‑7_10
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-8545-7_10 [Google Scholar]
  65. Salerni, N. , Assanelli, A. , D’Odorico, L. , & Rossi, G
    (2007) Qualitative aspects of productive vocabulary at the 200- and 500-word stages: A comparison between spontaneous speech and parental report data. First Language, 27, 75–87. doi: 10.1177/0142723707067545
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723707067545 [Google Scholar]
  66. Sandhofer, C. , Smith, L.B. , & Luo, J
    (2000) Counting nouns and verbs in the input: Differential frequencies, different kinds of learning?Journal of Child Language, 27, 561–585. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900004256
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900004256 [Google Scholar]
  67. Schachter, P. , & Shopen, T
    (2007) Parts-of-speech systems. In T. Shopen (Ed.), Language typology and syntactic description: Clause structure (2nd ed., vol. 1, pp.1–60). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511619427.001
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511619427.001 [Google Scholar]
  68. Shlesinger, Y
    (2000) Journalistic Hebrew: Stylistic aspects of Israeli newspaper sections. Beer-Sheva: Ben-Gurion University of the Negev Press. [in Hebrew]
    [Google Scholar]
  69. Spencer, A
    (1991) Morphological theory. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Thompson, G.L. , & Desrochers, A
    (2009) Corroborating biased indicators: Global and local agreement among objective and subjective estimates of printed word frequency. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 452–471. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.2.452
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.2.452 [Google Scholar]
  71. Tribushinina, E. , Van den Bergh, H. , Ravid, D. , Aksu-Koç, A. , Kilani-Schoch, M. , Korecky-Kröll, K. , Leibovitch-Cohen, I. , Laaha, S. , Nir, B. , Dressler W.U. , & Gillis, S
    (2014) The first year of adjectives: A growth curve analysis of child speech and parental input. Language, Interaction and Acquisition, 5, 185–226. doi: 10.1075/lia.5.2.02tri
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lia.5.2.02tri [Google Scholar]
  72. Vichner, A
    (2016) Adjectives in young children’s storybooks. Tel Aviv University ms. [in Hebrew]
    [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.11.3.04rav
Loading
  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): adjectives; AoA; Hebrew; morphology; register; subjective ranking
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error