1887
Volume 12, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1871-1340
  • E-ISSN: 1871-1375
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

In some languages the grammatical gender of nouns can be probabilistically detected using formal cues; for instance, in Italian, the majority of feminine nouns end in ‘-a’(e.g., , ‘home’) and the majority of masculine nouns end in ‘-o’ (e.g., , ‘tree’). It has been hypothesized that the match/mismatch between the formal information of the suffix and the abstract grammatical information on gender affects lexical processing of nouns. An alternative account is that a default option available for gender poses constraints to mechanisms of lexical access for words exhibiting gender markers in the surface form.

In the present study, nouns with highly predictive gender suffix (regular), nouns whose gender cannot be recovered from surface form (opaque) and nouns with misleading gender suffix (irregular) were compared in two reading aloud and two lexical decision experiments. Results confirmed that regular nouns are processed better than irregular nouns. No difference was detected between masculine and feminine opaque nouns.

The results allow the conclusion that a formal gender feature (the gender orthographic regularity) is more likely to affect lexical processing of bare nouns than the activation of a gender default option.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ml.12.1.05dem
2017-06-18
2025-02-10
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Bates, E. , Devescovi, A. , Hernandez, A. , & Pizzamiglio, L.
    (1996) Gender priming in Italian. Perception & Psychophysics, 58, 992–1004. doi: 10.3758/BF03206827
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206827 [Google Scholar]
  2. Bertinetto, P. M. , Burani, C. , Laudanna, A. , Marconi, L. , Ratti, D. , Rolando, C. , & Thornton, A. M.
    (2005) Corpus e Lessico di Frequenza dell’Italiano Scritto (CoLFIS). alphalinguistica.sns.it/CoLFIS/CoLFIS_Presentazione.htm
  3. Bordag, D. , & Pechmann, T.
    (2007) Factors influencing L2 gender processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 299–314. doi: 10.1017/S1366728907003082
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728907003082 [Google Scholar]
  4. Bordag, D. , Opitz, A. , & Pechmann, T.
    (2006) Gender processing in first and second languages: The role of noun termination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1090–1101.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Caramazza, A.
    (1997) How many levels of processing are there in lexical access?. Cognitive neuropsychology, 14, 177–208. doi: 10.1080/026432997381664
    https://doi.org/10.1080/026432997381664 [Google Scholar]
  6. Caramazza, A. , & Miozzo, M.
    (1997) The relation between syntactic and phonological knowledge in lexical access: evidence from the tip-of-the-tongue’ phenomenon. Cognition, 64, 309–343. doi: 10.1016/S0010‑0277(97)00031‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00031-0 [Google Scholar]
  7. Carstairs-McCarthy, A.
    (1998) How lexical semantics constrains inflectional allomorphy. InYearbook of Morphology 1997, 1–24. Springer Netherlands. doi: 10.1007/978‑94‑011‑4998‑3_1
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-4998-3_1 [Google Scholar]
  8. Caselli, M. C. Leonard, L. , Volterra, V. , & Campagnoli, M. G.
    (1993) Toward mastery of Italian morphology: A cross-sectional study. Journal of Child Language, 20, 377–393. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900008333
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900008333 [Google Scholar]
  9. Chini, M.
    (1995) Genere grammaticale e acquisizione. Aspetti della morfologia nominale in italiano L2. Milano: Franco Angeli.
    [Google Scholar]
  10. Colé, P. , Pynte, J. & Andriamamonjy, P.
    (2003) Effect of grammatical gender on visual word recognition: Evidence from lexical decision and eye movement experiments. Perception & Psychophysics, 65, 407–419. doi: 10.3758/BF03194572
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194572 [Google Scholar]
  11. Cubelli, R. , Lotto, L. , Paolieri, D. , Girelli, M. , & Job, R.
    (2005) Grammatical gender is selected in bare noun production: Evidence from the picture – word interference paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 42–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2005.02.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.02.007 [Google Scholar]
  12. Dardano, M. , & Trifone, M.
    (1985) La lingua italiana. Bologna: Zanichelli.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. De Martino, M. , & Laudanna, A.
    (2012) Il genere dei nomi italiani: dati sperimentali e distribuzionali, in S. Ferreri (Ed.), Lessico e Lessicologia (pp.343–354). Roma: Bulzoni.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. De Martino, M. , Bracco, G. , & Laudanna, A.
    (2011) The activation of grammatical gender information in processing Italian nouns. Language and Cognitive Processes, 26, 745–776. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2010.491977
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2010.491977 [Google Scholar]
  15. Di Domenico, E.
    (1997) Per una teoria del genere grammaticale. Padova: Unipress.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Franceschina, F.
    (2001) Morphological or syntactic deficits in near-native speakers? An assessment of some current proposals. Second Language Research, 17, 213–247. doi: 10.1191/026765801680191497
    https://doi.org/10.1191/026765801680191497 [Google Scholar]
  17. Gollan, T. H. , & Frost, R.
    (2001) Two routes to grammatical gender: Evidence from Hebrew. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 30, 627–651. doi: 10.1023/A:1014235223566
    https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1014235223566 [Google Scholar]
  18. Grosjean, F. , Dommergues, .J. Y. , Cornu, E. , Guillelmon, D. , & Besson, C.
    (1994) The gender-marking effect in spoken word recognition. Perception and Psychophysics, 56, 590–598. doi: 10.3758/BF03206954
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206954 [Google Scholar]
  19. Guillelmon, D. , & Grosjean, F.
    (2001) The gender marking effect in spoken word recognition: The case of bilinguals. Memory and Cognition, 29, 503–511. doi: 10.3758/BF03196401
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196401 [Google Scholar]
  20. Halle, M. , & Marantz, A.
    (1994) Some key features of Distributed Morphology. MIT working papers in linguistics, 21, 275–288.
    [Google Scholar]
  21. Harley, H. , & Ritter, E.
    (2002) Person and number in pronouns: A feature-geometric analysis. Language, 78, 482–526. doi: 10.1353/lan.2002.0158
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2002.0158 [Google Scholar]
  22. Harris, J. W.
    (1991) The exponence of gender in Spanish. Linguistic Inquiry, 22, 27–62.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Hernandez, A. E. , Kotz, S. A. , Hofmann, J. , Valentin, V. V. , Dapretto, M. , & Bookheimer, S. Y.
    (2004) The neural correlates of grammatical gender decisions in Spanish. Neuroreport, 15, 863–866. doi: 10.1097/00001756‑200404090‑00026
    https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200404090-00026 [Google Scholar]
  24. Matthews, C.
    (2010) On the nature of phonological cues in the acquisition of French gender categories: Evidence from instance-based learning models. Lingua, 120, 879–900. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2009.06.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2009.06.007 [Google Scholar]
  25. McCarthy, C.
    (2008) Morphological variability in the comprehension of agreement: An argument for representation over computation. Second Language Research, 24, 459–486. doi: 10.1177/0267658308095737
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658308095737 [Google Scholar]
  26. Padovani, R. , & Cacciari, C.
    (2003) Il ruolo della trasparenza morfologica nel riconoscimento di parole in italiano. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 4, 749–771.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Paolieri, D. , Lotto, L. , Leoncini, D. , Cubelli, R. , & Job, R.
    (2011) Differential effects of grammatical gender and gender inflection in bare noun production. British Journal of Psychology, 102, 19–36. doi: 10.1348/000712610X496536
    https://doi.org/10.1348/000712610X496536 [Google Scholar]
  28. Pérez-Pereira, M.
    (1991) The acquisition of gender: What Spanish children tell us. Journal of Child Language, 18, 571–590. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900011259
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900011259 [Google Scholar]
  29. Pinker, S.
    (1998) Words and rules. Lingua, 106, 219–242. doi: 10.1016/S0024‑3841(98)00035‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00035-7 [Google Scholar]
  30. Postiglione, F. , De Martino, M. , Bracco, G. , & Laudanna, A.
    (2014) Frequency distribution of inflectional classes and grammatical gender: a lexical database for Italian nouns. Poster presented atSecondo Convegno Interannuale della Società di Linguistica Italiana (SLI): Teorie e approcci usage-based in linguistica.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Serianni, L.
    (1988) Grammatica italiana. Italiano comune e lingua letteraria. Suoni, forme, costrutti. Torino: Utet.
    [Google Scholar]
  32. Spinelli, E. , Meunier, F. & Seigneuric, A.
    (2006) Spoken word recognition with gender-marked context. The Mental Lexicon, 1, 277–297. doi: 10.1075/ml.1.2.06spi
    https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.1.2.06spi [Google Scholar]
  33. Taft, M. , & Meunier, F.
    (1998) Lexical representation of gender: A quasi-regular domain. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27, 23–45. doi: 10.1023/A:1023270723066
    https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023270723066 [Google Scholar]
  34. Tsimpli, I. & Hulk, A.
    (2013) Grammatical gender and the notion of default: Insights from language acquisition. Lingua, 137, 128–144. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2013.09.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2013.09.001 [Google Scholar]
  35. White, L. , Valenzuela, E. , Kozlowsk-Macgregor, M. , & Leung, Y. K. I.
    (2004) Gender and number agreement in non native Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 105–133. doi: 10.1017/S0142716404001067
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716404001067 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.12.1.05dem
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.12.1.05dem
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error