1887
Volume 12, Issue 3
  • ISSN 1871-1340
  • E-ISSN: 1871-1375
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

The current study examined morphological priming in older individuals using two complex phenomena of German inflection. Study 1 examined inflected adjectives which encode multiple morphosyntactic features using regular affixes. Study 2 targeted inflected verb forms which also encode multiple features, but in this case using idiosyncratic stem variants. Study 1 revealed priming effects indicating efficient access of morphosyntactic features from inflected word forms with regular affixes. Study 2 showed that the same individuals were less efficient at accessing morphosyntactic features from marked stems. We argue that this contrast reflects age-related memory decline, which affects feature access from (lexically conditioned) stem variants more than feature access from lexically unconditioned regular forms.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ml.17008.rei
2018-03-16
2024-10-14
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Allen, P. A. , Madden, D. J. , & Crozier, L. C.
    (1991) Adult age differences in letter-level and word-level processing. Psychology and Aging, 6, 261–271. doi: 10.1037/0882‑7974.6.2.261
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.6.2.261 [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson, S. R.
    (1992) A-morphous morphology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511586262
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511586262 [Google Scholar]
  3. Aronoff, M.
    (1994) Morphology by itself: Stems and inflectional classes. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Atkinson, R. C. , & Shiffrin, R. M.
    (1971) The Control of Short-Term Memory. Scientific American, 224, 82–89. doi: 10.1038/scientificamerican0871‑82
    https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0871-82 [Google Scholar]
  5. Baayen, R. H.
    (2013) languageR: Data sets and functions with “Analyzing Linguistic Data: A practical introduction to statistics”. R package version 1.4.1
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Baayen, R. H. , Piepenbrock, R. , & Gulikers, L.
    (1995) The CELEX lexical database. Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Baayen, R. H. , & Milin, P.
    (2010) Analyzing reaction times. International Journal of Psychological Research, 3, 12–28.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Barbour, J. S.
    (1982) Productive and non-productive morphology: The case of the German strong verbs. Journal of Linguistics, 18, 331–354. doi: 10.1017/S0022226700013633
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226700013633 [Google Scholar]
  9. Barr, D. J. , Levy, R. , Scheepers, C. , & Tily, H. J.
    (2013) Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001 [Google Scholar]
  10. Basnight-Brown, D. M. , Chen, L. , Hua, S. , Kostić, A. , & Feldman, L. B.
    (2007) Monolingual and Bilingual Recognition of Regular and Irregular English Verbs: Sensitivity to Form Similarity Varies with First Language Experience. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 65–80. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.03.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.03.001 [Google Scholar]
  11. Bates, D. , Maechler, M. , Bolker, B. , & Walker, S.
    (2014) lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using Eigen and S4. R package version 1.1–7.
    [Google Scholar]
  12. Beedham, C.
    (1994) The role of consonants in marking strong verb conjugation in German and English. Folia linguistica, 28, 279–296. doi: 10.1515/flin.1994.28.3‑4.279
    https://doi.org/10.1515/flin.1994.28.3-4.279 [Google Scholar]
  13. Berres, M. , Monsch, A. U. , Bernasconi, F. , Thalmann, B. , & Stähelin, H. B.
    (2000) Normal ranges of neuropsychological tests for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Studies in Health Technology and Informatics, 77, 195–199.
    [Google Scholar]
  14. Bierwisch, M.
    (1967) Syntactic features in morphology: General problems of so-called pronominal inflection in German. InTo Honor Roman Jakobson: Essays on the occasion of his seventieth birthday, October 11, 1966. (Bd.I, pp.239–270). The Hague: Mouton.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Birdsong, D. , & Flege, J. E.
    (2001) Regular-Irregular Dissociations in L2 Acquisition of English Morphology. InBUCLD 25: Proceedings of the 25th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp.123–132). Boston, MA: Cascadilla Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Birdsong, D.
    (2004) Second Language Acquisition and Ultimate Attainment. In A. Davies & C. Elder (Eds.), The Handbook of Applied Linguistics (pp.82–105). London: Blackwell. doi: 10.1002/9780470757000.ch3
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470757000.ch3 [Google Scholar]
  17. (2005) Interpreting age effects in second language acquisition. In J. Kroll & A. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches (pp.109–127). New York: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Bittner, A.
    (1996) Starke “schwache” Verben–schwache “starke” Verben. Struktur des deutschen Verbsystems. Tübingen, Germany: Stauffenburg.
    [Google Scholar]
  19. Blevins, J. P.
    (1995) Syncretism and paradigmatic opposition. Linguistics and Philosophy, 18, 113–152. doi: 10.1007/BF00985214
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00985214 [Google Scholar]
  20. Borod, J. C. , Goodglass, H. , & Kaplan, E.
    (1980) Normative Data on the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, Parietal Lobe Battery, and the Boston Naming Test. Journal of Clinical Neuropsychology, 23, 209–215. doi: 10.1080/01688638008403793
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01688638008403793 [Google Scholar]
  21. Bosch, S. , & Clahsen, H.
    (2016) Accessing Morphosyntax in L1 and L2 Word Recognition: A Priming Study of Inflected German Adjectives. The Mental Lexicon, 11, 26–54.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Bowles, N. L. , & Poon, L. W.
    (1981) The effect of age on speed of lexical access. Experimental Aging Research, 7, 417–425. doi: 10.1080/03610738108259822
    https://doi.org/10.1080/03610738108259822 [Google Scholar]
  23. Burke, D. M. , & MacKay, D. G.
    (1997) Memory, language, and ageing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 352, 1845–1856. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1997.0170
    https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1997.0170 [Google Scholar]
  24. Burke, D. M. , & Shafto, M. A.
    (2004) Aging and language production. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 21–24. doi: 10.1111/j.0963‑7214.2004.01301006.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301006.x [Google Scholar]
  25. Chandler, M. J. , Lacritz, L. H. , Hynan, L. S. , Barnard, H. D. , Allen, G. , Deschner, M. , … Cullum, C. M.
    (2005) A total score for the CERAD neuropsychological battery. Neurology, 65, 102–106. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000167607.63000.38
    https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000167607.63000.38 [Google Scholar]
  26. Cheong, A. M. , Legge, G. E. , Lawrence, M. G. , Cheung, S. H. , & Ruff, M. A.
    (2008) Relationship between visual span and reading performance in age-related macular degeneration. Vision Research, 48, 577–588. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.11.022
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.visres.2007.11.022 [Google Scholar]
  27. Clahsen, H.
    (1999) Lexical entries and rules of language: a multidisciplinary study of German inflection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 991–1060. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99002228
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002228 [Google Scholar]
  28. Clahsen, H. , Eisenbeiss, S. , Hadler, M. , & Sonnenstuhl, I.
    (2001) The mental representation of inflected words: An experimental study of adjectives and verbs in German. Language, 77, 510–543. doi: 10.1353/lan.2001.0140
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2001.0140 [Google Scholar]
  29. Clahsen, H. , Prüfert, P. , Eisenbeiss, S. , & Cholin, J.
    (2002) Strong stems in the German mental lexicon: Evidence from child language acquisition and adult processing. In I. Kaufmann & B. Stiebels (Eds.), More than words: A Festschrift for Dieter Wunderlich (pp.91–112). Berlin: Akademie-Verlag.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Clahsen, H. , & Reifegerste, J.
    (2017). Morphological processing in old-age bilinguals. In M. Libben , M. Goral , & G. Libben Eds. Bilingualism: A Framework for Understanding the Mental Lexicon. (pp.217–248). Amsterdam: Benjamins
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Cortese, M. J. , Balota, D. A. , Sergent-Marshall, S. D. , Buckner, R. L. , & Gold, B. T.
    (2006) Consistency and regularity in past-tense verb generation in healthy ageing, Alzheimer’s disease, and semantic dementia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 23, 856–876. doi: 10.1080/02643290500483124
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290500483124 [Google Scholar]
  32. Curcio, C. A. , Owsley, C. , & Jackson, G. R.
    (2000) Spare the rods, save the cones in aging and age-related maculopathy. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 41, 2015–2018.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Davis, C. J.
    (2000) Match calculator. Software. Retrieved fromwww.pc.rhul.ac.uk/staff/c.davis/Utilities/MatchCalc/index.htm
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Davis, S. W. , Zhuang, J. , Wright, P. , & Tyler, L. K.
    (2014) Age-related sensitivity to task-related modulation of language-processing networks. Neuropsychologia, 63, 107–115. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.017
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.017 [Google Scholar]
  35. Dobbs, A. R. , & Rule, B. G.
    (1989) Adult age differences in working memory. Psychology and Aging, 4, 500–503. doi: 10.1037/0882‑7974.4.4.500
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.4.4.500 [Google Scholar]
  36. Duñabeitia, J. A. , Marin, A. , Avilés, A. , Perea, M. , & Carreiras, M.
    (2009) Constituent priming effects: Evidence for preserved morphological processing in healthy old readers. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21, 283–302. doi: 10.1080/09541440802281142
    https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440802281142 [Google Scholar]
  37. Durrell, M.
    (1980) Morphophonologische und morpholexische Regelmäßigkeiten im deutschen Ablautsystem. Jahrbuch für internationale Germanistik, 8, 19–28.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. (2001) Strong verb ablaut in the West Germanic languages. In S. Watts , J. West , & H. Solms (Eds.), Zur Verbmorphologie germanischer Sprachen (pp.5–18). Tübingen: Niemeyer. doi: 10.1515/9783110918656.5
    https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110918656.5 [Google Scholar]
  39. Feldman, L. B. , & Larabee, J.
    (2001) Morphological facilitation following prefixed but not suffixed primes: lexical architecture or modality-specific processes?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 680–691.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Fleischman, D. A.
    (2007) Repetition priming in aging and Alzheimer’s disease: an integrative review and future directions. Cortex, 43, 889–897. doi: 10.1016/S0010‑9452(08)70688‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70688-9 [Google Scholar]
  41. Folstein, M. F. , Folstein, S. E. , & McHugh, P. R.
    (1975) “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. doi: 10.1016/0022‑3956(75)90026‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6 [Google Scholar]
  42. Foos, P. W.
    (1989) Adult age differences in working memory. Psychology and Aging, 4, 269–275. doi: 10.1037/0882‑7974.4.3.269
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.4.3.269 [Google Scholar]
  43. Forster, K. I. , & Davis, C.
    (1984) Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10, 680–698.
    [Google Scholar]
  44. Forster, K. I. , & Forster, J. C.
    (2003) DMDX: A windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 35, 116–124. doi: 10.3758/BF03195503
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195503 [Google Scholar]
  45. Günther, H.
    (1988) Oblique word forms in visual word recognition. Linguistics, 26, 583–600. doi: 10.1515/ling.1988.26.4.583
    https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1988.26.4.583 [Google Scholar]
  46. Hasher, L. , & Zacks, R. T.
    (1988) Working memory, comprehension, and aging: A review and a new view. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol.22, pp.193–225). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  47. Hippisley, A.
    (2016) Network Morphology. In A. Hippisley & G. Stump (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Morphology (pp.816–851). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Isaacs, B. , & Kennie, A. T.
    (1973) The Set test as an aid to the detection of dementia in old people. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 123, 467–470. doi: 10.1192/bjp.123.4.467
    https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.123.4.467 [Google Scholar]
  49. Kavé, G. , & Levy, Y.
    (2005) The Processing of Morphology in Old Age: Evidence From Hebrew. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1442–1451. doi: 10.1044/1092‑4388(2005/100)
    https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/100) [Google Scholar]
  50. Kemtes, K. A. , & Kemper, S.
    (1997) Younger and older adults’ on-line processing of syntactically ambiguous sentences. Psychology and Aging, 12, 362–371. doi: 10.1037/0882‑7974.12.2.362
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.12.2.362 [Google Scholar]
  51. Kiparsky, P.
    (1998) Paradigm effects and opacity. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Köpcke, K. -M.
    (1998) Prototypisch starke und schwache Verben der deutschen Gegenwartssprache. Germanistische Linguistik, 141/142, 45–60.
    [Google Scholar]
  53. Krause, H. , Bosch, S. , & Clahsen, H.
    (2015) Morphosyntax in the bilingual mental lexicon. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 37, 597–621. doi: 10.1017/S0272263114000564
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263114000564 [Google Scholar]
  54. Kynette, D. , & Kemper, S.
    (1986) Aging and the loss of grammatical forms: A cross-sectional study of language performance. Language & Communication, 6, 65–72. doi: 10.1016/0271‑5309(86)90006‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0271-5309(86)90006-6 [Google Scholar]
  55. Leminen, A. , & Clahsen, H.
    (2014) Brain potentials to inflected adjectives: beyond storage and decomposition. Brain Research, 1543, 223–234. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.10.038
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.10.038 [Google Scholar]
  56. Light, L. L. , & Capps, J. L.
    (1986) Comprehension of pronouns in young and older adults. Developmental Psychology, 22, 580–585. doi: 10.1037/0012‑1649.22.4.580
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.22.4.580 [Google Scholar]
  57. Lukatela, G. , Carello, C. , & Turvey, M.
    (1987) Lexical representation of regular and irregular inflected nouns. Language and Cognitive Processes, 2, 1–17. doi: 10.1080/01690968708406349
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690968708406349 [Google Scholar]
  58. Lustig, C. , Hasher, L. , & Zacks, R. T.
    (2007) Inhibitory deficit theory: Recent developments in a “new view”. Inhibition in Cognition, 17, 145–162. doi: 10.1037/11587‑008
    https://doi.org/10.1037/11587-008 [Google Scholar]
  59. MacKay, D. G. , & Burke, D. M.
    (1990) Cognition and aging: a theory of new learning and the use of old connections. In T. M. Hess (Ed.), Aging and Cognition: Knowledge, Organization, and Utilization (pp.213–263). Amsterdam: North-Holland. doi: 10.1016/S0166‑4115(08)60159‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4115(08)60159-4 [Google Scholar]
  60. Mortensen, L. , Meyer, A. S. , & Humphreys, G. W.
    (2006) Age-related effects on speech production: A review. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 238–290. doi: 10.1080/01690960444000278
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960444000278 [Google Scholar]
  61. Moscoso del Prado Martín, F.
    (2017) Vocabulary, Grammar, Sex, and Aging. Cognitive Science, 41, 950–975.
    [Google Scholar]
  62. Newman, R. S. , & German, D. J.
    (2005) Life span effects of lexical factors on oral naming. Language and Speech, 48, 123–156. doi: 10.1177/00238309050480020101
    https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309050480020101 [Google Scholar]
  63. Opitz, A. , Regel, S. , Müller, G. , & Friederici, A. D.
    (2013) Neurophysiological evidence for morphological underspecification in German strong adjective inflection. Language, 89, 231–264. doi: 10.1353/lan.2013.0033
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2013.0033 [Google Scholar]
  64. Park, D. C. , Lautenschlager, G. , Hedden, T. , Davidson, N. , Smith, A. D. , & Smith, P.
    (2002) Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 16, 299–320. doi: 10.1037/0882‑7974.17.2.299
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.17.2.299 [Google Scholar]
  65. Paster, M.
    (2016) Alternations: Stems and Allomorphy. In A. Hippisley & G. Stump (Eds.), The Cambridge Handbook of Morphology (pp.174–214). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Penke, M. , Janssen, U. , & Eisenbeiss, S.
    (2004) Psycholinguistic evidence for the underspecification of morphosyntactic features. Brain and Language, 90, 423–433. doi: 10.1016/S0093‑934X(03)00453‑X
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00453-X [Google Scholar]
  67. Prasada, S. , & Pinker, S.
    (1993) Generalisation of regular and irregular morphological patterns. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 1–56. doi: 10.1080/01690969308406948
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690969308406948 [Google Scholar]
  68. Ramscar, M. , Hendrix, P. , Shaoul, C. , Milin, P. , & Baayen, R. H.
    (2014) The myth of cognitive decline: Non-linear dynamics of lifelong learning. Topics in Cognitive Science, 6, 5–42. doi: 10.1111/tops.12078
    https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12078 [Google Scholar]
  69. Reifegerste, J. , Hauer, F. , & Felser, C.
    (2017) Agreement processing and attraction errors in aging: Evidence from subject-verb agreement in German. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 24, 672–702.
    [Google Scholar]
  70. Riley, J. C.
    (2005) Estimates of Regional and Global Life Expectancy. Population and Development Review, 31, 537–543. doi: 10.1111/j.1728‑4457.2005.00083.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1728-4457.2005.00083.x [Google Scholar]
  71. Rosen, W. G. , Mohs, K. L. , & Davis, R. C.
    (1984) A New Rating Scale for Alzheimer’s Disease. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 1356–1364. doi: 10.1176/ajp.141.11.1356
    https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.141.11.1356 [Google Scholar]
  72. Salthouse, T. A.
    (1994) The aging of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8, 535–543. doi: 10.1037/0894‑4105.8.4.535
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.535 [Google Scholar]
  73. Schriefers, H. , Friederici, A. , & Graetz, P.
    (1992) Inflectional and derivational morphology in the mental lexicon: Symmetries and asymmetries in repetition priming. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44, 373–390. doi: 10.1080/02724989243000073
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02724989243000073 [Google Scholar]
  74. Singer, T. , Verhaeghen, P. , Ghisletta, P. , Lindenberger, U. , & Baltes, P. B.
    (2003) The fate of cognition in very old age: six-year longitudinal findings in the Berlin Aging Study (BASE). Psychology and Aging, 18, 318–331. doi: 10.1037/0882‑7974.18.2.318
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.18.2.318 [Google Scholar]
  75. Smolka, E. , Zwitserlood, P. , & Rösler, F.
    (2007) Stem access in regular and irregular inflection: Evidence from German participles. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 325–347. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.04.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.04.005 [Google Scholar]
  76. Spear, P. D.
    (1993) Neural bases of visual deficits during aging. Vision Research, 33, 2589–2609. doi: 10.1016/0042‑6989(93)90218‑L
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(93)90218-L [Google Scholar]
  77. Treitz, F. H. , Heyder, K. , & Daum, I.
    (2007) Differential course of executive control changes during normal aging. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14, 370–393. doi: 10.1080/13825580600678442
    https://doi.org/10.1080/13825580600678442 [Google Scholar]
  78. Tyler, L. K. , Shafto, M. A. , Randall, B. , Wright, P. , Marslen-Wilson, W. D. , & Stamatakis, E. A.
    (2010) Preserving syntactic processing across the adult life span: The modulation of the frontotemporal language system in the context of age-related atrophy. Cerebral Cortex, 20, 352–364. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp105
    https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhp105 [Google Scholar]
  79. Ullman, M. T. , Corkin, S. , Coppola, M. , Hickok, G. , Growdon, J. H. , Koroshetz, W. J. , & Pinker, S.
    (1997) A neural dissociation within language: Evidence that the mental dictionary is part of declarative memory, and that grammatical rules are processed by the procedural system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 266–276. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1997.9.2.266
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1997.9.2.266 [Google Scholar]
  80. Verhaeghen, P.
    (2011) Aging and executive control: reports of a demise greatly exaggerated. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 174–180. doi: 10.1177/0963721411408772
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411408772 [Google Scholar]
  81. Verhaeghen, P. , & Cerella, J.
    (2002) Aging, executive control, and attention: A review of meta-analyses. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 849–857. doi: 10.1016/S0149‑7634(02)00071‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00071-4 [Google Scholar]
  82. Verhaeghen, P. , Marcoen, A. , & Goossens, L.
    (1993) Facts and fiction about memory aging: A quantitative integration of research findings. Journal of Gerontology, 48, P157–P171. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.4.P157
    https://doi.org/10.1093/geronj/48.4.P157 [Google Scholar]
  83. Verhaeghen, P. , & Salthouse, T. A.
    (1997) Meta-analyses of age–cognition relations in adulthood: estimates of linear and nonlinear age effects and structural models. Psychological Bulletin, 122, 231–249. doi: 10.1037/0033‑2909.122.3.231
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.122.3.231 [Google Scholar]
  84. Wecker, N. S. , Kramer, J. H. , Wisniewski, A. , Delis, D. C. , & Kaplan, E.
    (2000) Age effects on executive ability. Neuropsychology, 14, 409–414. doi: 10.1037/0894‑4105.14.3.409
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.14.3.409 [Google Scholar]
  85. Wiese, B.
    (1996) Iconicity and syncretism: On pronominal inflection in Modern German. In R. Sackmann (Ed.), Theoretical Linguistics and Grammatical Description (pp.323–344). Amsterdam: Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/cilt.138.25wie
    https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.138.25wie [Google Scholar]
  86. (1999) Unterspezifizierte Paradigmen: Form und Funktion in der pronominalen Deklination. Linguistik Online, 4(3). doi: 10.13092/lo.4.1034
    https://doi.org/10.13092/lo.4.1034 [Google Scholar]
  87. (2008) Form and function of verbal ablaut in contemporary standard German. In R. Sackmann (Ed.), Explorations in Integrational Linguistics. Four essays on German, French, and Guaraní (pp.97–151). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: Benjamins. doi: 10.1075/cilt.285.03wie
    https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.285.03wie [Google Scholar]
  88. Wingfield, A. , Stine, E. A. , Lahar, C. J. , & Aberdeen, J. S.
    (1988) Does the capacity of working memory change with age?. Experimental Aging Research, 14, 103–107. doi: 10.1080/03610738808259731
    https://doi.org/10.1080/03610738808259731 [Google Scholar]
  89. Wunderlich, D.
    (1996) Minimalist morphology: The role of paradigms. In G. Booji & J. van Marle (Eds.), Yearbook of morphology 1995 (pp.93–114). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: Kluwer. doi: 10.1007/978‑94‑017‑3716‑6_6
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-3716-6_6 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.17008.rei
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.17008.rei
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): affixation; allomorphy; memory; morphological processing; morphosyntactic features
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error