1887
Volume 13, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1871-1340
  • E-ISSN: 1871-1375
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Previous studies have shown that nonnative phonemic contrasts pose perceptual difficulties for L2 learners, but less is known about how these contrasts affect speech production in L2 learners. In the present study, we elicited speech errors in a tongue twister task investigating L1 Arabic speakers producing L2 English words. Two sets of word productions were contrasted: words with phonemic contrasts existing in both L1 Arabic and L2 English (e.g. vs vs ) or words with phonemic contrasts existing in English alone ( vs vs ). Results showed that phonemic contrasts that do not exist in Arabic induced significantly more speech errors in L2 Arabic speakers of English compared to native English speakers than did phonemic contrasts found in both languages. Implications of these findings for representations in L2 learners are discussed.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ml.17014.alj
2018-08-10
2024-12-07
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Aoyama, K. , Flege, J. , Guion, S. , Akahane-Yamada, R. , and Yamada, T.
    (2004) Perceived phonetic dissimilarity and L2 speech learning: the case of Japanese /r/ and English /l/ and /r/. Journal of Phonetics, 32, 233–50.10.1016/S0095‑4470(03)00036‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0095-4470(03)00036-6 [Google Scholar]
  2. Baars, B. J. , Motley, M. T. & MacKay, D. G.
    (1975) Output editing for lexical status in artificially elicited slips of the tongue. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 382–391.10.1016/S0022‑5371(75)80017‑X
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(75)80017-X [Google Scholar]
  3. Balukas, C. & Koops, C.
    (2015) Spanish-English bilingual voice onset time in spontaneous code-switching. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19, 423–443.10.1177/1367006913516035
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006913516035 [Google Scholar]
  4. Bongaerts, T. , van Summeren, C. , Planken, B. , and Schils, E.
    (1997) Age and ultimate attainment in the pronunciation of a foreign language. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 19, 447–465.10.1017/S0272263197004026
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263197004026 [Google Scholar]
  5. Broersma, M.
    (2002) Comprehension of non-native speech: Inaccurate phoneme processing and activation of lexical competitors. In: Hansen, J. and Pellom, B. L. (eds) Seventh International Conference on Spoken Language Processing. Denver, CO: Center for Spoken Language.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Broersma, M. , and Cutler, A.
    (2011) Competition dynamics of second-language listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 74–95.10.1080/17470218.2010.499174
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2010.499174 [Google Scholar]
  7. Brysbaert, M. & New, B.
    (2009) Moving beyond Kucera and Francis: A Critical Evaluation of Current Word Frequency Norms and the Introduction of a New and Improved Word Frequency Measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977–99010.3758/BRM.41.4.977
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.977 [Google Scholar]
  8. Chan, K. Y. and Vitevitch, M. S.
    (2015) The influence of neighborhood density on the recognition of Spanish-accented words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41, 69–85.
    [Google Scholar]
  9. Cutler, A. , Weber, A. , and Otake, T.
    (2006) Asymmetric mapping from phonetic to lexical representations in second-language listening. Journal of Phonetics, 34, 269–284.10.1016/j.wocn.2005.06.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2005.06.002 [Google Scholar]
  10. Cutler, A. , and Otake, T.
    (2004) Pseudo-homophony in non-native listening. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 115, 2392.10.1121/1.4780547
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4780547 [Google Scholar]
  11. Dell, G. S.
    (1986) A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283–321.10.1037/0033‑295X.93.3.283
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283 [Google Scholar]
  12. Dell, G. S. , Reed, K. D. , Adams, D. R. , and Meyer, A. S.
    (2000) Speech errors, phonotactic constraints, and implicit learning: A study of the role of experience in language production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1355–1367.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Flege, J. E.
    (1980) Phonetic Approximation in Second Language Acquisition. Language Learning, 30, 117–134.10.1111/j.1467‑1770.1980.tb00154.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-1770.1980.tb00154.x [Google Scholar]
  14. Flege, James. E.
    (1987) The production of ‘New’ and ‘Similar’ phones in a foreign language: Evidence for the effect of equivalence classification.” Journal of Phonetics, 15, 47–65.
    [Google Scholar]
  15. Flege, J. E. , and Port, R.
    (1981) Cross-language phonetic interference: Arabic to English. Language and Speech, 24, 125–146.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Goldinger, S. D.
    (1996) Words and voices: Episodic traces in spoken word identification and recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 1166–1183.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Goldrick, M. , Runnqvist, E. and Costa, A.
    (2014) Language switching makes pronunciation less nativelike. Psychological Science, 25, 1031–1036.10.1177/0956797613520014
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613520014 [Google Scholar]
  18. Gollan, T. H. and Goldrick, M.
    (2012) Does bilingualism twist your tongue?Cognition, 125, 491–497.10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.08.002 [Google Scholar]
  19. Greenberg, J.
    (1966) Language Universals. The Hague: Mouton.
    [Google Scholar]
  20. Hyltenstam, K. , and Abrahamsson, N.
    (2003) Maturational constraints in SLA. In Doughty and Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition. Rowley, MA: Blackwell.10.1002/9780470756492.ch17
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470756492.ch17 [Google Scholar]
  21. Jakobson, R.
    (1971) Selected Writings Volume 1: Phonological Studies. The Hague: Mouton.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Kučera, H. , and Francis, W. N.
    (1967) Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Li, C. , Goldrick, M. & Gollan, T. H.
    (2017) Bilinguals’ Twisted Tongues: Frequency Lag or Interference?Memory & Cognition, 45, 600–610.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Lombardi, L.
    (2002) Coronal epenthesis and markedness. Phonology, 19, 219–251.10.1017/S0952675702004323
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0952675702004323 [Google Scholar]
  25. Luce, P. A. , and Pisoni, D. B.
    (1998) Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19, 1–36.10.1097/00003446‑199802000‑00001
    https://doi.org/10.1097/00003446-199802000-00001 [Google Scholar]
  26. Nusbaum, H. C. , Pisoni, D. B. , and Davis, C. K.
    (1984) Sizing up the Hoosier mental lexicon: Measuring the familiarity of 20,000 words. Research on Speech Perception Progress Report, 10, 357–376.
    [Google Scholar]
  27. Olson, D. J.
    (2013) Bilingual language switching and selection at the phonetic level: Asymmetrical transfer in VOT production. Journal of Phonetics, 41, 407–420.10.1016/j.wocn.2013.07.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2013.07.005 [Google Scholar]
  28. Pallier, C. , Colome´, A. , and Sebastián-Gallés, N.
    (2001) The influence of native-language phonology on lexical access: Exemplar-based versus abstract lexical entries. Psychological Science, 12, 445–449.10.1111/1467‑9280.00383
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00383 [Google Scholar]
  29. Sebastián-Gallés, N. , Echeverría, S. , and Bosch, L.
    (2005) The influence of initial exposure on lexical representation: Comparing early and simultaneous bilinguals. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 240–255.10.1016/j.jml.2004.11.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.11.001 [Google Scholar]
  30. Shattuck-Hufnagel, S.
    (1992) The role of word structure in segmental serial ordering. Cognition, 42, 213–259.10.1016/0010‑0277(92)90044‑I
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90044-I [Google Scholar]
  31. Swan, M. , and Smith, B.
    (2001) Learner English. A teacher’s guide to interference and other problems.10.1017/CBO9780511667121
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511667121 [Google Scholar]
  32. Vitevitch, M. S.
    (2002) The influence of phonological similarity neighborhoods on speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(4), 735.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Vitevitch, M. S. , Armbrüster, J. , and Chu, S.
    (2004) Sublexical and lexical representations in speech production: effects of phonotactic probability and onset density. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30(2), 514.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Wang, Y. , Jongman, A. , and Sereno, J. A.
    (2003) Acoustic and perceptual evaluation of Mandarin tone productions before and after perceptual training. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 113, 1033–1044.10.1121/1.1531176
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1531176 [Google Scholar]
  35. Warker, J. A. & Dell, G. S.
    (2006) Speech errors reflect newly learned phonotactic constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 32, 387–398.
    [Google Scholar]
  36. Weber, A. , and Cutler, A.
    (2004) Lexical competition in non-native spoken-word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 50, 1–25.10.1016/S0749‑596X(03)00105‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00105-0 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.17014.alj
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.17014.alj
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): Arabic; phoneme inventory; speech errors; speech production; tongue twister
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error