1887
Volume 14, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1871-1340
  • E-ISSN: 1871-1375
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Native speakers of Swedish use tones on stems to predict which suffix is to follow. This is seen behaviorally in reduced response times for matching tone-suffix pairs. Neurophysiologically, online prediction is reflected in the event-related potential (ERP) component pre-activation negativity (PrAN) occurring for tones with a higher predictive value. Invalid suffixes relative to the tone produce a left anterior negativity (LAN), or a broadly distributed negativity, and a P600. When native speakers make decisions about the inflection of words, response times are also longer for invalid tone-suffix combinations. In this study, low to intermediate level second language learners with non-tonal native languages trained tone-suffix associations for two weeks. Before and after training, they participated in a perception test where they listened to nouns with valid and invalid tone-suffix combinations and performed a singular/plural judgment task. During the test, electroencephalography (EEG) and response times were measured. After training, the PrAN effect increased, and a LAN emerged for invalid stimuli, indicating that the participants had acquired the tone-suffix association, using the tones as predictors more extensively post-training. However, neither a P600 nor longer response times for invalidity were found, suggesting potential differences in native and second language processing of the tone-suffix association.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ml.17018.hed
2019-11-11
2024-10-10
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., and Tanenhaus, M. K.
    (1998) Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eye movements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419–439. 10.1006/jmla.1997.2558
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2558 [Google Scholar]
  2. Barber, H., & Carreiras, M.
    (2005) Grammatical gender and number agreement in Spanish: An ERP comparison. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17(1), 137–153. doi:  10.1162/0898929052880101
    https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929052880101 [Google Scholar]
  3. Breen, M., Dilley, L. C., McAuley, J. D., & Sanders, L. D.
    (2014) Auditory evoked potentials reveal early perceptual effects of distal prosody on speech segmentation. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 1132–1146. 10.1080/23273798.2014.894642
    https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2014.894642 [Google Scholar]
  4. Bruce, G.
    (1977) Swedish word accents in sentence perspective. Lund: Gleerups.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Bruce, G., & Gårding, E.
    (1978) A prosodic typology for Swedish dialects. InE. Gårding, G. Bruce, & R. Bannert (Eds.), Nordic Prosody (pp.219–228). Lund: Gleerups.
    [Google Scholar]
  6. Coulson, S., King, J. W., & Kutas, M.
    (1998) Expect the unexpected: Event-related brain response to morphosyntactic violations. Language and Cognitive Processes, 13(1), 21–58. doi:  10.1080/016909698386582
    https://doi.org/10.1080/016909698386582 [Google Scholar]
  7. DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M.
    (2005) Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred from electrical brain activity. Natural Neuroscience, 8, 1117–1121. 10.1038/nn1504
    https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1504 [Google Scholar]
  8. Dikker, S., & Pylkkänen, L.
    (2013) Predicting language: MEG evidence for lexical preactivation. Brain and Language, 127, 55–64. 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.08.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.08.004 [Google Scholar]
  9. Dufour, S., Brunellière, A., & Frauenfelder, U. H.
    (2013) Tracking the time course of word-frequency effects in auditory word recognition with event-related potentials. Cognitive Science, 37, 489–507. 10.1111/cogs.12015
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12015 [Google Scholar]
  10. Ejerhed, E., Källgren, G., Wennstedt, O., & Åström, M.
    (1992) The linguistic annotation system of the Stockholm-Umeå Corpus project. Technical Report 33. Umeå: Department of General Linguistics, Umeå University.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Foucart, A., Ruiz-Tada, E., & Costa, A.
    (2015) How do you know I was about to say “book”? Anticipation processes affect speech processing and lexical recognition. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 30, 768–780. 10.1080/23273798.2015.1016047
    https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2015.1016047 [Google Scholar]
  12. Francis, A. L., Ciocca, V., Ma, L., & Fenn, K.
    (2008) Perceptual learning of Cantonese lexical tones by tone and non-tone language speakers. Journal of Phonetics, 36, 268–294. 10.1016/j.wocn.2007.06.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2007.06.005 [Google Scholar]
  13. Gagnepain, P., Henson, R. N., & Davis, M. H.
    (2012) Temporal predictive codes for spoken words in auditory cortex. Current Biology, 22, 615–621. 10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.015
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.015 [Google Scholar]
  14. Gosselke Berthelsen, S., Horne, M., Brännström, J., Shtyrov, Y., & Roll, M.
    (2018) Neural processing of morphosyntactic tonal cues in second-language learners. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 45, 60–78. 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2017.09.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2017.09.001 [Google Scholar]
  15. Gross, M., Say, T., Kleingers, M., Clahsen, H., & Münte, T. F.
    (1998) Human brain potentials to violations in morphologically complex Italian words. Neuroscience Letters, 241, 83–86. 10.1016/S0304‑3940(97)00971‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(97)00971-3 [Google Scholar]
  16. Gunter, T. C., Friederici, A. D., & Schriefers, H.
    (2000) Syntactic gender and semantic expectancy: ERPs reveal early autonomy and late interaction. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(4), 556–568. doi:  10.1162/089892900562336
    https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900562336 [Google Scholar]
  17. Hopp, H.
    (2016) Learning (not) to predict: Grammatical gender processing in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 32, 277–307. 10.1177/0267658315624960
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658315624960 [Google Scholar]
  18. Hunter, C. R.
    (2013) Early effects of neighborhood density and phonotactic probability of spoken words on event-related potentials. Brain and Language, 127, 463–474. 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.09.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.09.006 [Google Scholar]
  19. Jung, T.-P., Makeig, S., Humphries, C., Lee, T.-W., McKeown, M. J., Iragui, V., Sejnowski, T. J.
    2000 Removing electroencephalographic artifacts by blind source separation. Psychophysiology37, 163–178. 10.1111/1469‑8986.3720163
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.3720163 [Google Scholar]
  20. Kaan, E.
    (2014) Predictive sentence processing in L2 and L1: What is different?Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 4, 257–282. 10.1075/lab.4.2.05kaa
    https://doi.org/10.1075/lab.4.2.05kaa [Google Scholar]
  21. Kaan, E., Wayland, R., Bao, M., & Barkley, C. M.
    (2007) Effects of native language and training on lexical tone perception: An event-related potential study. Brain Research, 1148, 113–122. 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.019
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.019 [Google Scholar]
  22. Kaiser, R.
    (2011) Do Germans produce and perceive the Swedish word accent contrast? A cross-language analysis. TMH-QPSR, 51, 51, 93–96.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Lehtonen, M., & Laine, M.
    (2003) How word frequency affects morphological processing in monolinguals and bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(3), 213–255. 10.1017/S1366728903001147
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728903001147 [Google Scholar]
  24. Lehtonen, M., Niska, H., Wande, E., Niemi, J., & Laine, M.
    (2006) Recognition of inflected words in a morphologically limited language: Frequency effects in monolinguals and bilinguals. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35(2), 121–146. 10.1007/s10936‑005‑9008‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-005-9008-1 [Google Scholar]
  25. Lehtonen, M., Vorobyev, V., Soveri, A., Hugdahl, K., Tuokkolae, T., & Laine, M.
    (2009) Language-specific activations in the brain: Evidence from inflectional processing in bilinguals. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 22, 495–513. 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.05.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.05.001 [Google Scholar]
  26. Lu, S., Wayland, R., Kaan, E.
    (2015) Effects of production training and perception training on lexical tone perception – A behavioral and ERP study. Brain Research, 1624, 28–44. 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.07.014
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2015.07.014 [Google Scholar]
  27. Lück, M., Hahne, A., & Clahsen, H.
    (2006) Brain potentials to morphologically complex words during listening. Brain Research, 1077, 144–152. 10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.030
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2006.01.030 [Google Scholar]
  28. Morris, J., & Holcomb, P.
    (2005) Event-related potentials to violations of inflectional verb morphology in English. Cognitive Brain Research, 25, 963–981. 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.021
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.021 [Google Scholar]
  29. Newman, A. J., Ullman, M. T., Pancheva, R., Waligura, D. L., & Neville, H. J.
    (2007) An ERP study of regular and irregular English past tense inflection. Neuroimage, 34(1), 435–445. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.007 [Google Scholar]
  30. Ojima, S., Nakata, H., & Kakigi, R.
    2005 An ERPs study of second language learning after childhood: Effects of proficiency. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1212–1228. 10.1162/0898929055002436
    https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929055002436 [Google Scholar]
  31. Osterhout, L., & Holcomb, P.
    (1992) Event-related brain potentials elicited by syntactic anomaly. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 785–806. 10.1016/0749‑596X(92)90039‑Z
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(92)90039-Z [Google Scholar]
  32. Osterhout, L., & Mobley, L. A.
    (1995) Event-related brain potentials elicited by failure to agree. Journal of Memory and Language, 34(6), 739–773. doi:  10.1006/jmla.1995.1033
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1995.1033 [Google Scholar]
  33. Penke, M., Weyerts, H., Gross, M., Zander, E., Münte, T., & Clahsen, H.
    (1997) How the brain processes complex words: An event-related potential study of German verb inflections. Cognitive Brain Research, 6, 37–52. 10.1016/S0926‑6410(97)00012‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(97)00012-8 [Google Scholar]
  34. Pinker, S.
    (1991) Rules of language. Science, 253, 530–535. 10.1126/science.1857983
    https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1857983 [Google Scholar]
  35. Pulvermüller, F., & Shtyrov, Y.
    (2003) Automatic processing of grammar in the human brain as revealed by the mismatch negativity. Neuroimage, 20, 159–172. 10.1016/S1053‑8119(03)00261‑1
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00261-1 [Google Scholar]
  36. Regel, S., Kotz, S. A., Henseler, I., & Friederici, A. D.
    (2017) Left inferior frontal gyrus mediates morphosyntax: ERP evidence from verb processing in left-hemisphere damaged patients. Cortex, 86, 156–171. 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.11.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.11.007 [Google Scholar]
  37. Riad, T.
    (2013) The phonology of Swedish. Oxford: Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199543571.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199543571.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  38. (2016) Prosodin i svenskans morfologi. Stockholm: Morfem.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Righi, G., Blumstein, S. E., Mertus, J., & Worden, M. S.
    (2010) Neural systems underlying lexical competition: an eyetracking and fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 213–224. 10.1162/jocn.2009.21200
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21200 [Google Scholar]
  40. Rischel, J.
    (1963) Morphemic tone and word tone in Eastern Norwegian. Phonetica, 10, 154–164. 10.1159/000258166
    https://doi.org/10.1159/000258166 [Google Scholar]
  41. Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Clahsen, H., Lleó, C., Zaake, W., & Münte, T. F.
    (2001) Event-related brain responses to morphological violations in Catalan. Cognitive Brain Research, 11(1), 47–58. doi:  10.1016/S0926‑6410(00)00063‑X
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00063-X [Google Scholar]
  42. Roll, M.
    (2015) A neurolinguistic study of South Swedish word accents: Electrical brain potentials in nouns and verbs. Nordic Journal of Linguistics, 38, 149–162. 10.1017/S0332586515000189
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0332586515000189 [Google Scholar]
  43. Roll, M., Horne, M., & Lindgren, M.
    (2010) Word accents and morphology – ERPs of Swedish word processing. Brain Research, 1330, 114–123. 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.020
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.020 [Google Scholar]
  44. Roll, M., Horne, M., & Söderström, P.
    (2013) Word-stem tones cue suffixes in the brain. Brain Research, 1520, 116–120. 10.1016/j.brainres.2013.05.013
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.05.013 [Google Scholar]
  45. Roll, M., Söderström, P., Frid, J., Mannfolk, P., & Horne, M.
    (2017) Forehearing words: Pre-activation of word endings at word onset. Neuroscience Letters, 658, 57–61. doi:  10.1016/j.neulet.2017.08.030
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2017.08.030 [Google Scholar]
  46. Roll, M., Söderström, P., Mannfolk, P., Shtyrov, Y., Johansson, M., van Westen, D., & Horne, M.
    (2015) Word tones cueing morphosyntactic structure: Neuroanatomical substrates and activation time-course assessed by EEG and fMRI. Brain and Language, 150, 14–21. 10.1016/j.bandl.2015.07.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2015.07.009 [Google Scholar]
  47. Rossi, S., Gugler, M. F., Friederici, A. D., & Hahne, A.
    2006 The impact of proficiency on syntactic second-language processing of German and Italian: evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 2030–2048. 10.1162/jocn.2006.18.12.2030
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2006.18.12.2030 [Google Scholar]
  48. Schremm, A., Söderström, P., Horne, M., & Roll, M.
    (2016) Implicit acquisition of tone-suffix connections in L2 learners of Swedish. The Mental Lexicon11(1), 55–75. 10.1075/ml.11.1.03sch
    https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.11.1.03sch [Google Scholar]
  49. Schremm, A., Hed, A., Horne, M., & Roll, M.
    (2017) Training predictive L2 processing with a digital game: prototype promotes acquisition of anticipatory use of Swedish tone-suffix associations. Computers and Education, 114, 206–221. 10.1016/j.compedu.2017.07.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2017.07.006 [Google Scholar]
  50. Schremm, A., Novén, M., Horne, M., & Roll, M.
    (2019) Brain responses to morphologically complex verbs: An electrophysiological study of Swedish regular and irregular past tense forms. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 51, 76–83. 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2019.01.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2019.01.006 [Google Scholar]
  51. Schremm, A., Novén, M., Horne, M., Söderström, P., Westen, D. v., & Roll, M.
    (2018) Cortical thickness of planum temporale and pars opercularis in native language tone processing. Brain and Language, 176, 42–47. doi:  10.1016/j.bandl.2017.12.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2017.12.001 [Google Scholar]
  52. Söderström, P., Roll, M., & Horne, M.
    (2012) Processing morphologically conditioned word accents. The Mental Lexicon, 7(1), 77–89. 10.1075/ml.7.1.04soe
    https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.7.1.04soe [Google Scholar]
  53. Söderström, P., Horne, M., Frid, J., & Roll, M.
    (2016) Pre-activation negativity (PrAN) in brain potentials to unfolding words. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 512. 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00512
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00512 [Google Scholar]
  54. Söderström, P., Horne, M., & Roll, M.
    (2017a) Stem tones pre-activate suffixes in the brain. Journal of Psycholinguistic research, 46(2), 271–280. 10.1007/s10936‑016‑9434‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-016-9434-2 [Google Scholar]
  55. Söderström, P., Horne, M., Mannfolk, P., van Westen, D., & Roll, M.
    (2017b) Tone-grammar association within words: Concurrent ERP and fMRI show rapid neural pre-activation and involvement of left inferior frontal gyrus in pseudoword processing. Brain and Language, 174, 119–126. 10.1016/j.bandl.2017.08.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2017.08.004 [Google Scholar]
  56. Teleman, U., Hellberg, S., & Andersson, E.
    (1999) Svenska Akademiens grammatik. Stockholm: Svenska Akademien.
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Tronnier, M., & Zetterholm, E.
    (2013) Tendencies of Swedish word accent production by L2 Learners with tonal and non-tonal L1. InE. L. Asu & P. Lippus (Eds.), Nordic Prosody, Proceedings of the XIth Conference, Tartu 2012 (pp.391–400).
    [Google Scholar]
  58. van Berkum, J. J., Brown, C. M., Zwitserlood, P., Kooijman, V., & Haagort, P.
    (2005) Anticipating upcoming words in discourse: evidence from ERPs and reading times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition31, 443–467.
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Van Dommelen, W., & Husby, O.
    (2009) Perception of Norwegian word tones by Chinese and German listeners. InM. A. Watkins, A. S. Rauber & B. O. Babtista (Eds.), Recent research in second language phonetics/phonology: Perception and production (pp.308–321). Newcastle upon Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.
    [Google Scholar]
  60. van Hell, J. G., & Tokowicz, N.
    (2010) Event-related brain potentials and second language learning: syntactic processing in late L2 learners at different L2 proficiency levels. Second Language Research, 26, 43–74. 10.1177/0267658309337637
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0267658309337637 [Google Scholar]
  61. Vitevitch, M. S., & Luce, P. A.
    (1998) When words compete: Levels of processing in perception of spoken words. Psychological Science, 9, 325–329. 10.1111/1467‑9280.00064
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00064 [Google Scholar]
  62. (1999) Probabilistic phonotactics and neighborhood activation in spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 374–408. 10.1006/jmla.1998.2618
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1998.2618 [Google Scholar]
  63. Wang, Y., Spence, M. M., Jongman, A., & Sereno, J.
    (1999) Training American listeners to perceive Mandarin tones. Journal of the Acoustic Society of America, 106(6), 3649–3658. 10.1121/1.428217
    https://doi.org/10.1121/1.428217 [Google Scholar]
  64. Wang, Y., Jongman, A., Sereno, J., & Hirsch, J.
    (2003) FMRI evidence for cortical modification during learning of Mandarin lexical tone. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 1–9. 10.1162/089892903770007407
    https://doi.org/10.1162/089892903770007407 [Google Scholar]
  65. Wayland, R. P., & Guion, S. G.
    (2004) Training English and Chinese listeners to perceive Thai tones: a preliminary report. Language Learning, 54, 681–712. 10.1111/j.1467‑9922.2004.00283.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2004.00283.x [Google Scholar]
  66. Weyerts, H., Penke, M., Dohrn, U., Clahsen, H., & Münte, T. F.
    (1997) Brain potentials indicate differences between regular and irregular German plurals. Neuroreport, 8(4). doi:  10.1097/00001756‑199703030‑00028
    https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-199703030-00028 [Google Scholar]
  67. Williams, J., Segalowitz, N., & Leclair, T.
    (2014) Estimating second language productive vocabulary size. The Mental Lexicon, 9(1), 23–47. 10.1075/ml.9.1.02wil
    https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.9.1.02wil [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.17018.hed
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.17018.hed
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): ERP; LAN; morphology; PrAN; prosody; second language acquisition
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error