1887
Volume 13, Issue 3
  • ISSN 1871-1340
  • E-ISSN: 1871-1375
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

To date, normative psycholinguistics research has mainly focused on establishing norms for producing databases for concrete words using standardized pictures, while abstract words have been subject to much less attention. Understandably, the fact that the first can be represented visually helps in formulating picture-naming tasks to elicit verbal identification for pictures representing nouns and verbs, which greatly contributes to language experiments in both theoretical and clinical studies. The present study argues for the equal importance of studies that aim to develop databases for abstract words, as language use is not restricted to picturable/concrete concepts. We provide norms for a set of 165 abstract nouns, 56 abstract verbs and 109 abstract adjectives, collected from healthy speakers of Arabic. Using rating tasks, norms for imageability, age of acquisition, and familiarity are established. Linguistic factors such as syllable length and phoneme length are also accounted for. We also include orthographic frequency values (extracted from AraLex; Boudelaa and Marslen-Wilson, 2010). The norms for the processing of abstract words collected in the current study present a valuable resource for researchers and clinicians working with speakers of Arabic. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first dataset of abstract words for the Arabic language.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ml.18016.khw
2019-05-14
2024-12-03
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Akinina, Y. , Malyutina, S. , Ivanova, M. , Iskra, E. , Mannova, E. , & Dragoy, O.
    (2015) Russian normative data for 375 action pictures and verbs. Behavior Research Methods, 47, 691–707. 10.3758/s13428‑014‑0492‑9
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0492-9 [Google Scholar]
  2. Alario, F.-X. , Ferrand, L. , Laganaro, M. , New, B. , Frauenfelder, U. H. , & Segui, J.
    (2004) Predictors of naming speed. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 140–155. 10.3758/BF03195559
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195559 [Google Scholar]
  3. Albirini, A.
    (2016) Modern Arabic Sociolinguistics. Routledge, London & New York.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Alonso, M. A. , Fernandez, A. & Diez, E.
    (2015) Subjective age-of-acquisition norms for 7,039 Spanish words. Behavior research methods, 47, 268–274. 10.3758/s13428‑014‑0454‑2
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-014-0454-2 [Google Scholar]
  5. Barber, H. A. , Otten, L. J. , Kousta, S. T. , & Vigliocco, G.
    (2013) Concreteness in word processing: ERP and behavioral effects in a lexical decision task. Brain and Language, 125(1), 47–53. 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.01.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2013.01.005 [Google Scholar]
  6. Barca, L. , Burani, C. , & Arduino, S. L.
    (2002) Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms for Italian nouns. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34, 424–434. 10.3758/BF03195471
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195471 [Google Scholar]
  7. Barry, C. , Morrison, C. M. , & Ellis, A. W.
    (1997) Naming the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures: Effects of age of acquisition, frequency, and name agreement. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A, 560–585. 10.1080/783663595
    https://doi.org/10.1080/783663595 [Google Scholar]
  8. Bird, H., Franklin, S. & Howard, D.
    (2001) Age of acquisition and imageability ratings for a large set of words, including verbs and function words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 33(1), 73–79. 10.3758/BF03195349
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195349 [Google Scholar]
  9. Binder, J. , Westbury, C. , Possing, E. , Mckiernean, K. & Medler, D.
    (2005) Distinct brain systems for processing concrete and abstract concepts, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 905–917. 10.1162/0898929054021102
    https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929054021102 [Google Scholar]
  10. Bonin, P. , Méot, A. , Chalard, M. , & Fayol, M.
    (2002) The determinants of spoken and written picture naming latencies. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 89–114. 10.1348/000712602162463
    https://doi.org/10.1348/000712602162463 [Google Scholar]
  11. Bonin, P. , Peereman, R. , Malardier, N. , Méot, A. , & Chalard, M.
    (2003) A new set of 299 pictures for psycholinguistic studies: French norms for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, age of acquisition, and naming latencies. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 158–167. 10.3758/BF03195507
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195507 [Google Scholar]
  12. Bonin, P. , Boyer, B. , Méot, A. , Fayol, M. , & Droit, S.
    (2004) Psycholinguistic norms for action photographs in French and their relationships with spoken and written latencies. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 127–139. 10.3758/BF03195558
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195558 [Google Scholar]
  13. Boudelaa, S. , & Marslen-Wilson, W. D.
    (2010) Aralex: A lexical database for Modern Standard Arabic. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 481–487. 10.3758/BRM.42.2.481
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.2.481 [Google Scholar]
  14. Boukadi, M. , Zouaidi, C. , & Wilson, M. A.
    (2016) Norms for name agreement, familiarity, subjective frequency, and imageability for 348 object names in Tunisian Arabic. Behavior Research Methods, 48, 585–599. 10.3758/s13428‑015‑0602‑3
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0602-3 [Google Scholar]
  15. Brown, G. D. A., & Watson, F. L.
    (1987) First in, first out: Word learning age and spoken word frequency as predictors of word familiarity and word naming latency. Memory and Cognition, 15, 208–216. 10.3758/BF03197718
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197718 [Google Scholar]
  16. Brysbaert, M.
    (1996) Word frequency affects naming latency in Dutch when age of acquisition is controlled. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 8, 185–193. 10.1080/095414496383149
    https://doi.org/10.1080/095414496383149 [Google Scholar]
  17. Cameirão, M. L. , & Vicente, S. G.
    (2010) Age-of-acquisition norms for a set of 1,749 Portuguese words. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 474–480. 10.3758/BRM.42.2.474
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.2.474 [Google Scholar]
  18. Caroll, J. B. , & White, M. N.
    (1973) Age-of-acquisition norms for 220 picturable nouns. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 563–576. 10.1016/S0022‑5371(73)80036‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(73)80036-2 [Google Scholar]
  19. Cycowicz, Y. M. , Friedman, D. , Rothstein, M. , & Snodgrass, J. G.
    (1997) Picture naming by young children: norms for name agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65, 171–237. 10.1006/jecp.1996.2356
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1996.2356 [Google Scholar]
  20. Dupoux, E. & Mehler, J.
    (1990) Monitoring the lexicon with normal and compressed speech: Frequency effects and the prelexical code. Journal of Memory & Language29, 316–335. 10.1016/0749‑596X(90)90003‑I
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(90)90003-I [Google Scholar]
  21. Ellis, A. , & Lambon Ralph, M. A.
    (2000) Age of acquisition effects in adult lexical processing reflect loss of plasticity in maturing systems: insights from connectionist networks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 26(5), 1103–1123.
    [Google Scholar]
  22. Ferguson, C. A.
    (1959) Diglossia. Word, 15, 232–351. 10.1080/00437956.1959.11659702
    https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1959.11659702 [Google Scholar]
  23. Gernsbacher, M.
    (1984) Resolving 20 years of incosistent interactions between lexical familiary and orthography, concreteness and polysemy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General113:256–281. 10.1037/0096‑3445.113.2.256
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.113.2.256 [Google Scholar]
  24. Gilhooly, K. J. , & Gilhooly, M. L.
    (1979) Age-of-acquisition effects in lexical and episodic memory tasks. Memory and Cognition, 7, 214–223. 10.3758/BF03197541
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197541 [Google Scholar]
  25. Gilhooly, K. J. , & Logie, R. H.
    (1980) Age-of-acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity, and ambiguity measures for 1,944 words. Behavior Research Methods & Instrumentation, 12, 395–427. 10.3758/BF03201693
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03201693 [Google Scholar]
  26. (1981) Word age-of-acquisition, reading latencies and auditory recognition. Current Psychological Research, 1, 251–262. 10.1007/BF03186735
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03186735 [Google Scholar]
  27. Glaser, W. R.
    (1992) Picture naming. Cognition, 42, 61–105. 10.1016/0010‑0277(92)90040‑O
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(92)90040-O [Google Scholar]
  28. Gordon, B.
    (1985) Subjective frequency and the lexical decision latency function: Implications for mechanisms of lexical access. Journal of Memory & Language24, 631–645. 10.1016/0749‑596X(85)90050‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(85)90050-6 [Google Scholar]
  29. Hale, B.
    (1988) Abstract objects. Blackwell.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Holcomb, P. J. , Kounios, J. , Anderson, J. E. & West, W. C.
    Dual-coding, context-availability, and concreteness effects in sentence comprehension: An electrophysiological investigation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 25, 721–42.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Idrissi, Ali & Eva Kehayia
    (2004) “Morphological units in the Arabic mental lexicon: Evidence from an individual with deep dyslexia.” Brain and Language90, 183–197. 10.1016/S0093‑934X(03)00431‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00431-0 [Google Scholar]
  32. Idrissi, Ali, Jean-François Prunet & Renée Béland
    (2008) “On the abstractness of Arabic roots.” Linguistic Inquiry39(2), 221–259. 10.1162/ling.2008.39.2.221
    https://doi.org/10.1162/ling.2008.39.2.221 [Google Scholar]
  33. Khwaileh, T. , Body, R. & Herbert, R.
    (2014) A normative database and determinants of lexical retrieval for 186 Arabic nouns: Effects of psycholinguistic and morpho-syntactic variables on naming latency. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 43, 749–769. 10.1007/s10936‑013‑9277‑z
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10936-013-9277-z [Google Scholar]
  34. Khwaileh, T. , Body, R. , & Herbert, R.
    (2017) Lexical retrieval after Arabic aphasia: Syntactic access and predictors of spoken naming. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 42, 140–155. 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2017.01.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2017.01.001 [Google Scholar]
  35. Khwaileh, T. , Mustafawi, E. , Herbert, R. & Howard, D.
    (2018) Gulf Arabic nouns and verbs: A standardized set of 319 object pictures and 141 action pictures, with predictors of naming latencies. Behavior Research Methods, 1–18. doi:  10.3758/s13428‑018‑1019‑6
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-1019-6 [Google Scholar]
  36. Kosslyn, S. M. , & Chabris, C. F.
    (1990) Naming pictures. Journal of Visual Languages and Computing, 1, 77–95. 10.1016/S1045‑926X(05)80035‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1045-926X(05)80035-7 [Google Scholar]
  37. Kousta, S. T. , Vigliocco, G. , Vinson, D. P. , Andrews, M. , & Del Campo, E.
    (2011) The representation of abstract words: Why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 14–34. 10.1037/a0021446
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021446 [Google Scholar]
  38. Kreuz, R.
    (1987) The subjective familiarity of English homophones. Memory & Cognition15(2), 154–168. 10.3758/BF03197027
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197027 [Google Scholar]
  39. Lachman, R. , Shaffer, J. P. , & Hennrikus, D.
    (1974) Language and cognition: Effects of stimulus codability, name-word frequency, and age of acquisition on lexical reaction time. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 613–625. 10.1016/S0022‑5371(74)80049‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(74)80049-6 [Google Scholar]
  40. Marslen-Wilson, W.
    (1990) Activation, competition and frequency in lexical access. Cognitive models of speech processing, ed. G. Altmann , 148–172. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Martein, R.
    (1995) Norms for name and concept agreement, familiarity, visual complexity and image agreement on a set of 216 pictures. Psychologica Belgica, 35, 205–225.
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Meschyan, G. & Hernandez, A.
    (2002) Age of acquisition and word frequency: Determinants of object-naming speed and accuracy. Memory & Cognition, 30, 262–269. 10.3758/BF03195287
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195287 [Google Scholar]
  43. Morrison, C. M. , Ellis, A. W. , & Quinlan, P. T.
    (1992) Age of acquisition, not word frequency, affects object naming, not object recognition. Memory and Cognition, 20, 705–714. 10.3758/BF03202720
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03202720 [Google Scholar]
  44. Morrison, C. M., & Ellis, A. W.
    (1995) Roles of word frequency and age of acquisition in word naming and lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(1), 116–133.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Mustafawi, E.
    (2018) Arabic phonology. In E. Benmamoun and R. Bassiouney , The Routledge Handbook of Arabic linguistics, 11–31. Routledge, New York.
  46. Mustafawi, E. & Shaaban, K. Language
    Policies in Qatar: From Local to Global Then Back to Local.Language Policy. Published online: 16 Jul. 2018. doi:  10.1007/s10993‑018‑9483‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10993-018-9483-5 [Google Scholar]
  47. Nickels, L., & Howard, D.
    (1995) Phonological Errors in Aphasic Naming: Comprehension, Monitoring and Lexicality. Cortex, 31(2), 209–237.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Nickels, L.
    (1997) Spoken word production and its breakdown in aphasia. Hove, UK: Psychology Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  49. Nusbaum, H. C. , Pisoni, D. & Davis, C.
    (1984) Sizing up the hoosier mental lexicon: Measuring the familiarity of 20,000 words. Research on speech perception, progress report 10, 357–376. Indiana University.
    [Google Scholar]
  50. Paivio, A.
    (1991) Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 45, 255–287. 10.1037/h0084295
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0084295 [Google Scholar]
  51. Paivio, A.
    (2007) Mind and its evolution: A dual coding theoretical approach. Mahwah, NJ, US: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
    [Google Scholar]
  52. Paivio, A.
    (2013) Dual coding theory, word abstractness, and emotion: a critical review of Kousta et al. (2011). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(1), 282–287. [PubMed] 10.1037/a0027004
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027004 [Google Scholar]
  53. (2014) Intelligence, dual coding theory, and the brain. Intelligence, 47, 141–158. 10.1016/j.intell.2014.09.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.09.002 [Google Scholar]
  54. Paivio, A. , Clark, J. M. , Digdon, N. , & Bons, T.
    (1989) Referential processing: Reciprocity and correlates of naming and imaging. Memory & Cognition, 17, 163–174. 10.3758/BF03197066
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197066 [Google Scholar]
  55. Paivio, A. , Yuille, J. C. & Madigan, S.
    (1968) Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 76, (1, Pt. 2) 10.1037/h0025327
    https://doi.org/10.1037/h0025327 [Google Scholar]
  56. Pind, J., & Tryggvadottir, H. B. J.
    (2002) Determinants of picture naming times in Icelandic. Scandanavian Journal of Psychology, 43, 221–226. 10.1111/1467‑9450.00290
    https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9450.00290 [Google Scholar]
  57. Plaut, D. C. , & Shallice, T.
    (1993) Perseverative and semantic influences on visual object naming errors in optic aphasia: A connectionist account. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 89–117. 10.1162/jocn.1993.5.1.89
    https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.1993.5.1.89 [Google Scholar]
  58. Prunet, J. , Béland, R. & Idrissi, A.
    (2000) The mental representation of Semitic words. Linguistic Inquiry, 31, 609–648. 10.1162/002438900554497
    https://doi.org/10.1162/002438900554497 [Google Scholar]
  59. Quadflieg, S. , Michel, C. , Bukowski, H. , & Samson, D.
    (2014) A database of psycholinguistic and lexical properties for French adjectives referring to human and/or nonhuman attributes. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 67–76. 10.1037/cep0000001
    https://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000001 [Google Scholar]
  60. Raman, I. , Raman, E. , & Mertan, B.
    (2014) A standardized set of 260 pictures for Turkish: Norms of name and image agreement, age of acquisition, visual complexity, and conceptual familiarity. Behavior Research, 46, 588–595. 10.3758/s13428‑013‑0376‑4
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0376-4 [Google Scholar]
  61. Rosa, P. , Catricala, E. , Vigliocco, G. & Cappa, S.
    (2010) Beyond the abstract – concrete dichotomy: Mode of acquisition, concreteness, imageability, familiarity, age of acquisition, context availability, and abstractness norms for a set of 417 Italian words. Behavior research methods, 42(4), 1042–1048. 10.3758/BRM.42.4.1042
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.42.4.1042 [Google Scholar]
  62. Shao, Z. , Roelofs, A. , & Meyer, A. S.
    (2013) Predicting naming latencies for action pictures: Dutch norms. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 274–283. 10.3758/s13428‑013‑0358‑6
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0358-6 [Google Scholar]
  63. Shao, Z. , Roelofs, A. , Martin, R. C. , & Meyer, A. S.
    (2015) Selective inhibition and naming performance in semantic blocking, pictureword interference, and color-word Stroop tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(6), 1806–20.
    [Google Scholar]
  64. Schwanenflugel, P. J. , Akin, C. , & Luh, W.
    (1992) Context availability and the recall of abstract and concrete words. Memory & Cognition, 20, 96–104. 10.3758/BF03208259
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03208259 [Google Scholar]
  65. Schwanenflugel, P. J. , & Stowe, R. W.
    (1989) Context availability and the processing of abstract and concrete words in sentences. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 114–126. 10.2307/748013
    https://doi.org/10.2307/748013 [Google Scholar]
  66. Schwanenflugel, P. J. , Harnishfeger, K. K. , & Stowe, R. W.
    (1988) Context avail ability and lexical decisions for abstract and concrete words. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 499–520. 10.1016/0749‑596X(88)90022‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(88)90022-8 [Google Scholar]
  67. Schwitter, V. , Boyer, B. , Méot, A. , Bonin, P. , & Laganaro, M.
    (2004) French normative data and naming times for action pictures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 564–576. 10.3758/BF03195603
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195603 [Google Scholar]
  68. Segui, J. , Mehler, J. , Frauenfelder, U. & Morton, J.
    (1982) The word frequency effect and lexical access. Neuropsychologica, 20(6), 615–627. 10.1016/0028‑3932(82)90061‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(82)90061-6 [Google Scholar]
  69. Severens, E. , Van Lommel, S. , Ratinckx, E. , & Hartsuiker, R. J.
    (2005) Timed picture naming norms for 590 pictures in Dutch. Acta Psychologica, 119, 159–187. 10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.01.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2005.01.002 [Google Scholar]
  70. Snodgrass, J. G. , & Yuditsky, T.
    (1996) Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 516–536. 10.3758/BF03200540
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200540 [Google Scholar]
  71. Snodgrass, J. G. , & Corwin, J.
    (1988) Perceptual identification thresholds for 150 fragmented pictures from the Snodgrass and Vanderwart picture set. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 67, 3–36. 10.2466/pms.1988.67.1.3
    https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1988.67.1.3 [Google Scholar]
  72. Snodgrass, J. G. , & Vanderwart, M.
    (1980) A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning & Memory, 6, 174–215.
    [Google Scholar]
  73. Stadthagen-Gonzalez, H., & Davis, C. J.
    (2006) The Bristol norms for age of acquisition, imageability, and familiarity. Behavior Research Methods, 38, 598–605.
    [Google Scholar]
  74. Tanaka-Ishii, K. & Terada, H.
    (2011) Word familiarity and frequency. Studia Linguistica, 65(1), 96–116.
    [Google Scholar]
  75. Vigliocco, G. , Vinson, D. P. , Druks, J. , Barber, H. , & Cappa, S. F.
    (2011) Nouns and verbs in the brain: A review of behavioural, electrophysiological, neuropsychological and imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 407–426. 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.007 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.18016.khw
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.18016.khw
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): abstract; adjectives; age of acquisition; Arabic; familiarity; imageability; norms; nouns; verbs
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error