1887
Volume 16, Issue 2-3
  • ISSN 1871-1340
  • E-ISSN: 1871-1375
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

The main aims of the present study are, first, to extend the current cognitive research on affective word processing in bilinguals by analyzing affective words in minimal linguistic contexts and, second, to explore the potential impact of the affective valence of prime nouns on the affective valence of target adjectives. To fulfill these aims, a semantic decision task was employed in which the Persian-English bilinguals saw a pair of words one after another, and were asked to decide whether or not the target word, which was an adjective loaded with positive or negative valence, was related in meaning to the preceding word, which was a noun. Mixed factorial repeated measure ANOVA was run on reaction times and error rates data. The results showed that bilinguals’ responses were slower and less accurate to negative target adjectives in comparison to positive target adjectives. The data further revealed that bilinguals were faster but less accurate when they were responding to related target adjectives compared to unrelated target adjectives. The results provide evidence for a dynamic interaction between cognitive and affective language processing in bilinguals.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ml.20026.doq
2022-03-08
2025-04-23
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Altarriba, J. , & Basnight-Brown, D. M.
    (2011) The representation of emotion vs. emotion-laden words in English and Spanish in the affective Simon task. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15 (3), 310–328. 10.1177/1367006910379261
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006910379261 [Google Scholar]
  2. Barrett, L. F.
    (2006a) Are emotions natural kinds?Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1 (1), 28–58. 10.1111/j.1745‑6916.2006.00003.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00003.x [Google Scholar]
  3. (2006b) Valence is a basic building block of emotional life. Journal of Research in Personality, 40 (1), 35–55. 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2005.08.006 [Google Scholar]
  4. (2013) Psychological construction: The Darwinian approach to the science of emotion. Emotion Review, 5 (4), 379–389. 10.1177/1754073913489753
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073913489753 [Google Scholar]
  5. Berridge, K. , & Winkielman, P.
    (2003) What is an unconscious emotion? (The case for unconscious “liking”). Cognition & Emotion, 17 (2), 181–211. 10.1080/02699930302289
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930302289 [Google Scholar]
  6. Blanchette, I. , & Richards, A.
    (2010) The influence of affect on higher level cognition: A review of research on interpretation, judgment, decision making and reasoning. Cognition & Emotion, 24 (4), 561–595. 10.1080/02699930903132496
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903132496 [Google Scholar]
  7. Bloom, L. , & Beckwith, R.
    (1989) Talking with feeling: Integrating affective and linguistic expression in early language development. Cognition & Emotion, 3, 315–342. 10.1080/02699938908412711
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938908412711 [Google Scholar]
  8. Brysbaert, M. , New, B. , & Keuleers, E.
    (2012) Adding Part-of-Speech information to the SUBTLEX-US word frequencies. Behavior Research Methods, 44 , 991–997. 10.3758/s13428‑012‑0190‑4
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0190-4 [Google Scholar]
  9. Brysbaert, M. , Warriner, A. B. , & Kuperman, V.
    (2014) Concreteness ratings for 40 thousand generally known English word lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 46 (3), 904–911. 10.3758/s13428‑013‑0403‑5
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0403-5 [Google Scholar]
  10. Cacioppo, J. T. , Gardner, W. L. , & Berntson, G. G.
    (1999) The affect system has parallel and integrative processing components: Form follows function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76 (5), 839–855. 10.1037/0022‑3514.76.5.839
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.5.839 [Google Scholar]
  11. Caldwell-Harris, C. L.
    (2014) Emotionality differences between a native and foreign language: Theoretical implications. Frontiers in Psychology, 5 , 1–4. 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01055
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01055 [Google Scholar]
  12. Colbeck, K. L. , & Bowers, J. S.
    (2012) Blinded by taboo words in L1 but not L2. Emotion, 12 (2), 217–222. 10.1037/a0026387
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026387 [Google Scholar]
  13. Conrad, M. , Recio, G. , & Jacobs, A. M.
    (2011) The time course of emotion effects in first and second language processing: A cross cultural ERP study with German-Spanish bilinguals. Frontiers in Psychology, 2 , 1–16. 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00351
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00351 [Google Scholar]
  14. Costa, A. , Foucart, A. , Arnon, I. , Aparici, M. , & Apesteguia, J.
    (2014) “Piensa” twice: On the foreign language effect in decision making. Cognition, 130 (2), 236–254. 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.010 [Google Scholar]
  15. Degner, J. , Doycheva, C. , & Wentura, D.
    (2012) It matters how much you talk: On the automaticity of affective connotations of first and second language words. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 15 (1), 181–189. 10.1017/S1366728911000095
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728911000095 [Google Scholar]
  16. De Houwer, J. , Hermans, D. , Rothermund, K. , & Wentura, D.
    (2002) Affective priming of semantic categorization responses. Cognition and Emotion, 16 (5), 643–666. 10.1080/02699930143000419
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000419 [Google Scholar]
  17. Eilola, T. M. , & Havelka, J.
    (2011) Behavioral and physiological responses to the emotional and taboo Stroop tasks in native and non-native speakers of English. International Journal of Bilingualism, 15 (3), 353–369. 10.1177/1367006910379263
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1367006910379263 [Google Scholar]
  18. Eilola, T. M. , Havelka, J. , & Sharma, D.
    (2007) Emotional activation in the first and second language. Cognition & Emotion, 21 (5), 1064–1076. 10.1080/02699930601054109
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930601054109 [Google Scholar]
  19. Fan, L. , Xu, Q. , Wang, X. , Xu, F. , Yang, Y. , & Lu, Z.
    (2018) The automatic activation of emotion words measured using the emotional face-word Stroop task in late Chinese–English bilinguals. Cognition and Emotion, 32 (2), 315–324. 10.1080/02699931.2017.1303451
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1303451 [Google Scholar]
  20. Frings, C. , Englert, J. , Wentura, D. , & Bermeitinger, C.
    (2010) Decomposing the emotional Stroop effect. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63 , 42–49. 10.1080/17470210903156594
    https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210903156594 [Google Scholar]
  21. Geipel, J. , Hadjichristidis, C. , & Surian, L.
    (2015) How foreign language shapes moral judgment. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 59 , 8–17. 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.02.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2015.02.001 [Google Scholar]
  22. Gentsch, K. , Loderer, K. , Soriano, C. , Fontaine, J. R. J. , Eid, M. , Pekrun, R. , & Scherer, K. R.
    (2018) Effects of achievement contexts on the meaning structure of emotion words. Cognition and Emotion, 32 (2), 379–388. 10.1080/02699931.2017.1287668
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2017.1287668 [Google Scholar]
  23. Grandjean, D. , Baenziger, T. , & Scherer, K. R.
    (2006) Intonation as an interference between language and affect. Progress in Brain Research, 156 , 1–13.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. Ito, T. A. , Larsen, J. T. , Smith, N. K. , & Cacioppo, J. T.
    (1998) Negative information weighs more heavily on the brain: The negativity bias in evaluative categorizations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75 (4), 887–900. 10.1037/0022‑3514.75.4.887
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.75.4.887 [Google Scholar]
  25. Jonczyk, R.
    (2016) Affect-Language Interactions in Native and Non-Native English Speakers: A Neuropragmatic Perspective. Cham: Springer International Publishing. 10.1007/978‑3‑319‑47635‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47635-3 [Google Scholar]
  26. Kahneman, D. , & Tversky, A.
    (1984) Choices, values, and frames. American Psychologist, 39 (4), 341–350. 10.1037/0003‑066X.39.4.341
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.4.341 [Google Scholar]
  27. Keysar, B. , Hayakawa, S. L. , & An, S. G.
    (2012) The foreign-language effect thinking in a foreign tongue reduces decision biases. Psychological Science, 23 (6), 661–668. 10.1177/0956797611432178
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611432178 [Google Scholar]
  28. Klauer, K. C. , & Musch, J.
    (2003) Affective priming: Findings and theories. In J. Musch & K. C. Klauer (Eds.), The psychology of evaluation: Affective processes in cognition and emotion (pp.7–49). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Kousta, S. T. , Vinson, D. P. , & Vigliocco, G.
    (2009) Emotion words, regardless of polarity, have a processing advantage over neutral words. Cognition, 112 (3), 473–481. 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.007 [Google Scholar]
  30. Kutas, M. , & Federmeier, K. D.
    (2000) Electrophysiology reveals semantic memory use in language comprehension. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4 (12), 463–470. 10.1016/S1364‑6613(00)01560‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01560-6 [Google Scholar]
  31. Lai, V. T. , Hagoort, P. , & Casasanto, D.
    (2012) Affective primacy vs. cognitive primacy: Dissolving the debate. Frontiers in Psychology, 3 , 1–8. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00243
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00243 [Google Scholar]
  32. Lang, P. J.
    (1995) The emotion probe: Studies of motivation and attention. The American Psychologist, 50 (5), 372–385. 10.1037/0003‑066X.50.5.372
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.50.5.372 [Google Scholar]
  33. Lang, P. J. , Bradley, M. M. , & Cuthbert, B. N.
    (1990) Emotion, attention, and the startle reflex. Psychological Review, 97 (3), 377–395. 10.1037/0033‑295X.97.3.377
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.3.377 [Google Scholar]
  34. Lench, H. C. , Darbor, K. E. , & Berg, L. A.
    (2013) Functional perspectives on emotion, behavior, and cognition. Behavioral Sciences, 3 , 536–540. 10.3390/bs3040536
    https://doi.org/10.3390/bs3040536 [Google Scholar]
  35. Li, P. , Zhang, F. , Tsai, E. , & Puls, B.
    (2014) Language history questionnaire (LHQ 2.0): A new dynamic web-based research tool. Bilingualism: Language & Cognition, 17 (3), 673–680. 10.1017/S1366728913000606
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728913000606 [Google Scholar]
  36. McNamara, T. P.
    (2005) Semantic Priming: Perspectives from Memory and Word Recognition. New York: Psychology Press. 10.4324/9780203338001
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203338001 [Google Scholar]
  37. Nygaard, L. C. , & Lunders, E. R.
    (2002) Resolution of lexical ambiguity by emotional tone of voice. Memory & Cognition, 30 , 583–593. 10.3758/BF03194959
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194959 [Google Scholar]
  38. Nygaard, L. C. , & Queen, J. S.
    (2008) Communicating emotion: Linking affective prosody and word meaning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34 , 1017–1030.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Paulmann, S. , & Kotz, S. A.
    (2008) An ERP investigation on the temporal dynamics of emotional prosody and emotional semantics in pseudo- and lexical-sentence context. Brain and Language, 105 , 59–69. 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.11.005
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2007.11.005 [Google Scholar]
  40. Peirce, J. W.
    (2007) PsychoPy – psychophysics software in Python. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 162 (1), 8–13. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017 [Google Scholar]
  41. (2009) Generating stimuli for neuroscience using PsychoPy. Frontiers in Neuroinformatics, 2 , 1–8.
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Ponari, M. , Rodriguez-Cuadrado, S. , Vinson, D. , Fox, N. , Costa, A. , & Vigliocco, G.
    (2015) Processing advantage for emotional words in bilingual speakers. Emotion, 15 (5), 644–652. 10.1037/emo0000061
    https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000061 [Google Scholar]
  43. Pratto, F. , & John, O. P.
    (1991) Automatic vigilance: The attention-grabbing power of negative social information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61 (3), 380–391. 10.1037/0022‑3514.61.3.380
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.61.3.380 [Google Scholar]
  44. Raymond, J. E. , Shapiro, K. L. , & Arnell, K. M.
    (1992) Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: An attentional blink?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18 (3), 849–860.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Russell, J. A.
    (2006) Emotions are not modules. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 32 , 53–71.
    [Google Scholar]
  46. (2009) Emotion, core affect, and psychological construction. Cognition & Emotion, 23 (7), 1259–1283. 10.1080/02699930902809375
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930902809375 [Google Scholar]
  47. Sass, K. , Fetz, K. , Oetken, S. , Habel, U. , & Heim, S.
    (2013) Emotional verbal fluency: A new task on emotion and executive function interaction. Behavioral Sciences, 3 , 372–387. 10.3390/bs3030372
    https://doi.org/10.3390/bs3030372 [Google Scholar]
  48. Sutton, T. M. , Altarriba, J. , Gianico, J. L. , & Basnight-Brown, D. M.
    (2007) The automatic access of emotion: Emotional Stroop effects in Spanish–English bilingual speakers. Cognition & Emotion, 21 , 1077–1090. 10.1080/02699930601054133
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930601054133 [Google Scholar]
  49. van Heuven, W. J. B. , & Dijkstra, T.
    (2010) Language comprehension in the bilingual brain: fMRI and ERP support for psycholinguistic models. Brain Research Reviews, 64 , 104–122. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.03.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2010.03.002 [Google Scholar]
  50. Warriner, A. B. , Kuperman, V. , & Brysbaert, M.
    (2013) Norms of valence, arousal, and dominance for 13,915 English lemmas. Behavior Research Methods, 45 (4), 1191–1207. 10.3758/s13428‑012‑0314‑x
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-012-0314-x [Google Scholar]
  51. Warriner, A. B. , & Kuperman, V.
    (2015) Affective biases in English are bi-dimensional. Cognition and Emotion, 29 (7), 1147–1167. 10.1080/02699931.2014.968098
    https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2014.968098 [Google Scholar]
  52. Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D. , Barrett, L. F. , and Barsalou, L. W.
    (2013) Neural evidence that human emotions share core affective properties. Psychological Science, 24 , 947–956. 10.1177/0956797612464242
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612464242 [Google Scholar]
  53. Winkielman, P. , & Berridge, K.
    (2004) Unconscious emotion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13 (3), 120–123. 10.1111/j.0963‑7214.2004.00288.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00288.x [Google Scholar]
  54. Winkielman, P. , Berridge, K. C. , & Wilbarger, J. L.
    (2005) Unconscious affective reactions to masked happy versus angry faces influence consumption behavior and judgments of value. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 31 (1), 121–135. 10.1177/0146167204271309
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271309 [Google Scholar]
  55. Winskel, H.
    (2013) The emotional Stroop task and emotionality rating of negative and neutral words in late Thai-English bilinguals. International Journal of Psychology, 48 (6), 1090–1098. 10.1080/00207594.2013.793800
    https://doi.org/10.1080/00207594.2013.793800 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.20026.doq
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.20026.doq
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error