1887
Volume 18, Issue 2
  • ISSN 1871-1340
  • E-ISSN: 1871-1375
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

A key question in research concerning the typing production of morphologically complex words is whether the whole multimorphemic word is output ballistically or whether individual constituents are accessed during typing. To address this question, we examined keystroke latencies during the production of English compounds (e.g., ) to test whether the initiation and continued typing of each constituent (e.g., and ) are influenced by its linguistic properties (length, frequency, and semantic transparency). Participants identified and then typed a compound word. We found that the initiation and continued typing of each constituent was influenced by the linguistic properties of that constituent. However, the linguistic properties of the second constituent also influenced the typing latency of the final letter of the first constituent, suggesting that production of the first constituent overlapped with accessing and planning the keystrokes of the second constituent. The influence of the linguistic properties of the first constituent on its own initiation and continued typing suggests that accessing and planning the keystrokes of the first constituent occurred as the compound word was being identified. Our findings indicate that individual constituents are accessed during production and influence the typing of compound words.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ml.21005.tai
2023-12-14
2024-10-04
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Aronoff, M.
    (1994) Morphology by Itself: Stems and Inflectional Classes, MIT Press, Cambridge, MA.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Baayen, R. H., Dijkstra, T., & Schreuder, R.
    (1997) Singulars and plurals in Dutch: Evidence for a parallel dual-route model. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(1), 94–117. 10.1006/jmla.1997.2509
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2509 [Google Scholar]
  3. Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J.
    (2003) The length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure: Evidence from eye movements when reading short and long Finnish compounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 48(3), 615–634. 10.1016/S0749‑596X(02)00539‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00539-9 [Google Scholar]
  4. Beyersmann, E., Kezilas, Y., Coltheart, M., Castles, A., Ziegler, J. C., Taft, M., & Grainger, J.
    (2018) Taking the book from the bookshelf: Masked constituent priming effects from compound words and nonwords. Journal of Cognition, 1(1), 1–13. 10.5334/joc.11
    https://doi.org/10.5334/joc.11 [Google Scholar]
  5. Bien, H., Levelt, W. J. M., & Baayen, R. H.
    (2005) Frequency effects in compound production. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(49), 17876–17881. 10.1073/pnas.0508431102
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0508431102 [Google Scholar]
  6. Brysbaert, M., & New, B.
    (2009) Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41(4), 977–990. 10.3758/BRM.41.4.977
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.4.977 [Google Scholar]
  7. Burani, C., Salmaso, D., & Caramazza, A.
    (1984) Morphological structure and lexical access. Visible Language, 181, 342–352.
    [Google Scholar]
  8. Colé, P., Beauvillain, C., & Segui, J.
    (1989) On the representation and processing of prefixed and suffixed derived words: A differential frequency effect. Journal of Memory and Language, 28(1), 1–13. 10.1016/0749‑596X(89)90025‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-596X(89)90025-9 [Google Scholar]
  9. Dohmes, P., Zwitserlood, P., & Bölte, J.
    (2004) The impact of semantic transparency of morphologically complex words on picture naming. Brain and Language, 90(1–3), 203–212. 10.1016/S0093‑934X(03)00433‑4
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00433-4 [Google Scholar]
  10. Feldman, L. B., Dale, R., & van Rij, J.
    (2019) Lexical and frequency effects on keystroke timing: Challenges to a lexical search account from a type-to-copy task. Frontiers in Communication, 41. 10.3389/fcomm.2019.00017
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2019.00017 [Google Scholar]
  11. Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L.
    (2014) Typing time as an index of morphological and semantic effects during English compound processing. Lingue e Linguaggio, 13(2), 241–262. 10.1418/78409
    https://doi.org/10.1418/78409 [Google Scholar]
  12. (2016a) Effects of morphology and semantic transparency on typing latencies in English compound and pseudocompound words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 42(9), 1489–1495. 10.1037/xlm0000258
    https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000258 [Google Scholar]
  13. (2016b) Written production of English compounds: effects of morphology and semantic transparency. Morphology, 26(2), 133–155. 10.1007/s11525‑015‑9265‑0
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11525-015-9265-0 [Google Scholar]
  14. Gagné, C. L., Spalding, T. L., Nisbet, K. A., & Armstrong, C.
    (2018) Pseudo-morphemic structure inhibits, but morphemic structure facilitates, processing of a repeated free morpheme. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 33(10), 1252–1274. 10.1080/23273798.2018.1470250
    https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2018.1470250 [Google Scholar]
  15. Gagné, C. L., Spalding, T. L., & Schmidtke, D.
    (2019) LADEC: The Large Database of English Compounds. Behavior Research Methods, 51(5), 2152–2179. 10.3758/s13428‑019‑01282‑6
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-019-01282-6 [Google Scholar]
  16. Gagné, C., Spalding, T. L., & Taikh, A.
    (2023) Impact of morphology on written word production: An overview of empirical evidence and theoretical implications. InD. Crepaldi (Ed.), Current Issues in the Psychology of Language: Linguistic Morphology in the Mind and Brain. Routledge, 96–107. 10.4324/9781003159759‑7
    https://doi.org/10.4324/9781003159759-7 [Google Scholar]
  17. Grainger, J., & Beyersmann, E.
    (2017) Edge-aligned embedded word activation initiations morpho-orthographic segmentation. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 671, 285–317. 10.1016/bs.plm.2017.03.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.plm.2017.03.009 [Google Scholar]
  18. Inhoff, A. W.
    (1991) Word Frequency During Copytyping. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 171, 478–487. 10.1037/0096‑1523.17.2.478
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.17.2.478 [Google Scholar]
  19. Ji, H., Gagné, C. L., & Spalding, T. L.
    (2011) Benefits and costs of lexical decomposition and semantic integration during the processing of transparent and opaque English compounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 65(4), 406–430. 10.1016/j.jml.2011.07.003
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2011.07.003 [Google Scholar]
  20. Jones, M. N., & Mewhort, D. J. K.
    (2004) Case-sensitive letter and bigram frequency counts from large-scale English corpora. InBehavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers (Vol.36, Issue3, pp.388–396). Psychonomic Society Inc. 10.3758/BF03195586
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03195586 [Google Scholar]
  21. Kandel, S., Álvarez, C. J., & Vallée, N.
    (2006) Syllables as processing units in handwriting production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(1), 18–31. 10.1037/0096‑1523.32.1.18
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.32.1.18 [Google Scholar]
  22. Lehtönen, M., Cunillera, T., Rodríguez-Fornells, A., Hultén, A., Tuomainen, J., & Laine, M.
    (2007) Recognition of morphologically complex words in Finnish: Evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Research, 1148(1), 123–137. 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.026
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2007.02.026 [Google Scholar]
  23. Lemhöfer, K., Koester, D., & Schreuder, R.
    (2011) When bicycle pump is harder to read than bicycle bell: effects of parsing cues in first and second language compound reading. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2010 18:2, 18(2), 364–370. 10.3758/s13423‑010‑0044‑y
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-010-0044-y [Google Scholar]
  24. Libben, G., & Weber, S.
    (2014) Semantic transparency, compounding, and the nature of independent variables. InF. Rainer, W. Dressler, F. Gardani, & H. C. Luschutzky (Eds.), Morphology and meaning. Benjamins. 10.1075/cilt.327.14lib
    https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.327.14lib [Google Scholar]
  25. Logan, G. D.
    (2018) Automatic control: How experts act without thinking. Psychological Review, 125(4), 453–485. 10.1037/rev0000100
    https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000100 [Google Scholar]
  26. Logan, G. D., & Crump, M. J. C.
    (2009) The left hand doesn’t know what the right hand is doing: The disruptive effects of attention to the hands in skilled typewriting: Research article. Psychological Science, 20(10), 1296–1300. 10.1111/j.1467‑9280.2009.02442.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02442.x [Google Scholar]
  27. (2011) Hierarchical Control of Cognitive Processes. The Case for Skilled Typewriting. InPsychology of Learning and Motivation – Advances in Research and Theory (Vol.541). Academic Press. 10.1016/B978‑0‑12‑385527‑5.00001‑2
    https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-385527-5.00001-2 [Google Scholar]
  28. Nottbusch, G., Grimm, A., Weingarten, R., & Will, U.
    (2005) Syllabic structures in typing: Evidence from deaf writers. Reading and Writing, 181, 497–526. 10.1007/s11145‑005‑3178‑y
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-005-3178-y [Google Scholar]
  29. Pinet, S., Ziegler, J. C., & Alario, F. X.
    (2016) Typing is writing: Linguistic properties modulate typing execution. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 231, 1898–1906. 10.3758/s13423‑016‑1044‑3
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1044-3 [Google Scholar]
  30. Roelofs, A.
    (1998) Rightward incrementality in encoding simple phrasal forms in speech production: Verb-particle combinations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 24(4), 904–921. 10.1037/0278‑7393.24.4.904
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.24.4.904 [Google Scholar]
  31. Roelofs, A., & Baayen, H.
    (2002) Morphology by itself in planning the production of spoken words. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9(1), 132–138. 10.3758/BF03196269
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196269 [Google Scholar]
  32. Sahel, S., Nottbusch, G., Grimm, A., & Weingarten, R.
    (2008) Written production of German compounds: Effects of lexical frequency and semantic transparency. Written Language & Literacy, 11(2), 211–227. 10.1075/wll.11.2.06sah
    https://doi.org/10.1075/wll.11.2.06sah [Google Scholar]
  33. Salthouse, T. A.
    (1986) Perceptual, cognitive, and motoric aspects of transcription typing. Psychological Bulletin, 99(3), 303–319. 10.1037/0033‑2909.99.3.303
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.99.3.303 [Google Scholar]
  34. Scaltritti, M., Arfé, B., Torrance, M., & Peressotti, F.
    (2016) Typing pictures: Linguistic processing cascades into finger movements. Cognition, 1561, 16–29. 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.07.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.07.006 [Google Scholar]
  35. Seidenberg, M. S.
    (1987) Sublexical structures in visual word recognition: Access units or orthographic redundancy?InM. Coltheart (Ed.), Attention and performance 12: The psychology of reading (pp.245–263). Lawrence Earlbaum Associates, Inc.https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1987-98557-011
    [Google Scholar]
  36. (1989) Reading Complex Words. InG. N. Carlson & M. K. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Linguistic Structure in Language Processing. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics (7th ed., pp.53–105). Springer, Dordrecht. 10.1007/978‑94‑009‑2729‑2_3
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-2729-2_3 [Google Scholar]
  37. Taft, M.
    (1979) Recognition of affixed words and the word frequency effect. Memory & Cognition, 7(4), 263–272. 10.3758/BF03197599
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197599 [Google Scholar]
  38. Taft, M., & Ardasinski, S.
    (2006) Obligatory decomposition in reading prefixed words. The Mental Lexicon, 1(2), 183–199. 10.1075/ml.1.2.02taf
    https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.1.2.02taf [Google Scholar]
  39. Taikh, A., Gagne, C., & Spalding, T. L.
    (in press). Influence of the constituent morpheme boundary on compound word access. Memory and Cognition.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Weingarten, R., Nottbusch, G., & Will, U.
    (2004) Morphemes, syllables, and graphemes in written word production. Trends in lingustics studies and monographs, 1571, 529–572.
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Will, U., Nottbusch, G., & Weingarten, R.
    (2006) Linguistic units in word typing. Written Language & Literacy Written Language and Literacy, 9(1), 153–176. 10.1075/wll.9.1.10wil
    https://doi.org/10.1075/wll.9.1.10wil [Google Scholar]
  42. Weingarten, R.
    (2005) Subsyllabic units in written word production. Written Language & Literacy, 8(1), 43–61. 10.1075/wll.8.1.03wei
    https://doi.org/10.1075/wll.8.1.03wei [Google Scholar]
  43. Yamaguchi, M., & Logan, G. D.
    (2014) Pushing typists back on the learning curve: Revealing chunking in skilled typewriting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(2), 592–612. 10.1037/a0033809
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033809 [Google Scholar]
  44. (2016) Pushing typists back on the learning curve: Memory chunking in the hierarchical control of skilled typewriting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 42(12), 1919–1936. 10.1037/xlm0000288
    https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000288 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.21005.tai
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.21005.tai
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error