1887
Volume 17, Issue 1
  • ISSN 1871-1340
  • E-ISSN: 1871-1375
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

In an experiment, Spanish speakers assigned gender to nouns. Some nouns had biological referents. Others had a mismatch between their gender and their final phones (e.g. ). Nouns with biological referents and nouns with matching gender and phonology were responded to faster suggesting that gender does not depend solely on a noun’s gender. Gender was also assigned to dual-gendered nouns, which are feminine nouns that take the masculine article (e.g. ). Most participants assigned them masculine gender.

Dual-gendered nouns are often preceded by masculine modifiers which is due to analogy to . The idea is explored that the gender of , along with all modifiers a noun has been experienced with, explains gender assignment. Computational simulations were carried out to test this using exemplar, naive Bayes, and decision tree algorithms. They made accurate predictions without referencing the noun’s gender. In dual-gendered nouns, a shift towards preposed masculine modifiers was observed. A simulation predicted the gender of bare dual-gendered nouns which mirrored the masculine gender the experimental participants provided. These results suggest a usage-based model in which a noun’s gender is determined by the modifiers it has been experienced with.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ml.21011.ell
2022-04-08
2025-02-16
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Aha, D. W., Kibler, D., & Albert, M. K.
    (1991) Instance-based learning algorithms. Machine Learning, 6(1), 37–66. 10.1007/BF00153759
    https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00153759 [Google Scholar]
  2. Alarcón, I.
    (2009) The processing of gender agreement in L1 and L2 Spanish: Evidence from reaction time data. Hispania, 814–828.
    [Google Scholar]
  3. (2020) Early and late bilingual processing of Spanish gender, morphology and gender congruency. Borealis–An International Journal of Hispanic Linguistics, 9(2), 175–208. 10.7557/1.9.2.5523
    https://doi.org/10.7557/1.9.2.5523 [Google Scholar]
  4. Álvarez de Miranda, P.
    (1993) El alomorfo de’la’y sus consecuencias. LEA: Lingüística Española Actual, 15(1), 5–44.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Ambridge, B.
    (2020a) Against stored abstractions: A radical exemplar model of language acquisition. First Language, 40(5–6), 509–559. 10.1177/0142723719869731
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723719869731 [Google Scholar]
  6. (2020b) Abstractions made of exemplars or ‘You’re all right, and I’ve changed my mind’: Response to commentators. First Language, 40(5–6), 640–659. 10.1177/0142723720949723
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0142723720949723 [Google Scholar]
  7. Arnon, I., & Ramscar, M.
    (2012) Granularity and the acquisition of grammatical gender: How order-of-acquisition affects what gets learned. Cognition, 122(3), 292–305. 10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.10.009 [Google Scholar]
  8. Barber, H., & Carreiras, M.
    (2005) Grammatical gender and number agreement in Spanish: An ERP comparison. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 17(1), 137–153. 10.1162/0898929052880101
    https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929052880101 [Google Scholar]
  9. Bates, E., Devescovi, A., Pizzamiglio, L., D’amico, S., & Hernandez, A.
    (1995) Gender and lexical access in Italian. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(6), 847–862. 10.3758/BF03206800
    https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03206800 [Google Scholar]
  10. Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Bruni, M. R., Bajo, M. T., & Dussias, P. E.
    (2021) Brain potentials reveal differential processing of masculine and feminine grammatical gender in native Spanish speakers. Psychophysiology, 58(3), e13737. 10.1111/psyp.13737
    https://doi.org/10.1111/psyp.13737 [Google Scholar]
  11. Beatty-Martínez, A. L., & Dussias, P. E.
    (2019) Revisiting masculine and feminine grammatical gender in Spanish: Linguistic, psycholinguistic, and neurolinguistic evidence. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 751. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00751
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00751 [Google Scholar]
  12. Bull, W. E.
    (1965) Spanish for Teachers: Applied Linguistics. New York: Ronald Press Co.
    [Google Scholar]
  13. Bybee, J.
    (2001) Phonology and language useCambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511612886
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511612886 [Google Scholar]
  14. (2006) From usage to grammar: The mind’s response to repetition. Language, 711–733. 10.1353/lan.2006.0186
    https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2006.0186 [Google Scholar]
  15. (2010) Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511750526
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511750526 [Google Scholar]
  16. (2013) Usage-based theory and exemplar representations of constructions. InT. Hoffmann and G. Trousdale (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Construction Grammar, 49–69. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Caffarra, S., Janssen, N., & Barber, H. A.
    (2014) Two sides of gender: ERP evidence for the presence of two routes during gender agreement processing. Neuropsychologia, 63, 124–134. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.016
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.08.016 [Google Scholar]
  18. Caramazza, A.
    (1997) How many levels of processing are there in lexical access?. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14(1), 177–208. 10.1080/026432997381664
    https://doi.org/10.1080/026432997381664 [Google Scholar]
  19. Chandler, S.
    (2017) The analogical modeling of linguistic categories. Language and Cognition, 9(1), 52–87. 10.1017/langcog.2015.24
    https://doi.org/10.1017/langcog.2015.24 [Google Scholar]
  20. Corbett, G. G.
    (1991) Gender. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9781139166119
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139166119 [Google Scholar]
  21. Cubelli, R., Lotto, L., Paolieri, D., Girelli, M., & Job, R.
    (2005) Grammatical gender is selected in bare noun production: Evidence from the picture–word interference paradigm. Journal of Memory and Language, 53(1), 42–59. 10.1016/j.jml.2005.02.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2005.02.007 [Google Scholar]
  22. Daelemans, W., Zavrel, J., Van Der Sloot, K., & Van den Bosch, A.
    (2004) Timbl: Tilburg memory-based learner. Tilburg University.
    [Google Scholar]
  23. Davies, M.
    (2002) Corpus del Español, 100 Million Words, 1200s-1900s. https://www.corpusdelespanol.org/hist-gen/
  24. (2019) Corpus del Español, News on the Web Corpus. https://www.corpusdelespanol.org/now/
  25. Domínguez, A., Cuetos, F., & Segui, J.
    (1999) The processing of grammatical gender and number in Spanish. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 28(5), 485–498. 10.1023/A:1023216326448
    https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023216326448 [Google Scholar]
  26. Duchon, A., Perea, M., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Martí, A., & Carreiras, M.
    (2013) EsPal: One-stop shopping for Spanish word properties. Behavior research methods, 45(4), 1246–1258. 10.3758/s13428‑013‑0326‑1
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-013-0326-1 [Google Scholar]
  27. Eddington, D.
    (2002) Spanish Gender Assignment in an Analogical Framework. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics9, 49–75. 10.1076/jqul.9.1.49.8482
    https://doi.org/10.1076/jqul.9.1.49.8482 [Google Scholar]
  28. Eddington, D., & Hualde, J. I.
    (2008) El abundante agua fría: Hermaphroditic Spanish Nouns. Studies in Hispanic & Lusophone Linguistics, 1(1). 10.1515/shll‑2008‑1004
    https://doi.org/10.1515/shll-2008-1004 [Google Scholar]
  29. Elmougy, S., Taher, H., & Noaman, H.
    (2008) Naïve Bayes classifier for Arabic word sense disambiguation. InProceeding of the 6th International Conference on Informatics and SystemsShanghai, China: School of Electronics & Information Shanghai Dianji University, 16–21.
    [Google Scholar]
  30. Faussart, C., Jakubowicz, C., & Costes, M.
    (1999) Gender and number processing in spoken French and Spanish. Rivista di Linguistica, 11(1), 75–101.
    [Google Scholar]
  31. Frauenfelder, U. H., & Tyler, L. K.
    (1987) The process of spoken word recognition: An introduction. Cognition, 25(1–2), 1–20. 10.1016/0010‑0277(87)90002‑3
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(87)90002-3 [Google Scholar]
  32. Goldinger, S. D.
    (1996) Words and voices: episodic traces in spoken word identification and recognition memory. Journal of experimental psychology: Learning, memory, and cognition, 22(5), 1166.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. (1998) Echoes of echoes? An episodic theory of lexical access. Psychological review, 105(2), 251. 10.1037/0033‑295X.105.2.251
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.2.251 [Google Scholar]
  34. Hay, J. & Bresnan, J.
    (2006) Spoken syntax: The phonetics of giving a hand in New Zealand English. The Linguistic Review, 23, 321–349. 10.1515/TLR.2006.013
    https://doi.org/10.1515/TLR.2006.013 [Google Scholar]
  35. Heitmeier, M., Chuang, Y. Y., & Baayen, R. H.
    (2021) Modeling morphology with Linear Discriminative Learning: considerations and design choices. arXiv preprint arXiv:2106.07936. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.720713
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2021.720713 [Google Scholar]
  36. Hernández, Arturo E., Sonja A. Kotz, Juliane Hofmann, Vivian V. Valentin, Mirella Dapretto, and Susan Y. Bookheimer
    2004 The neural correlates of grammatical gender decisions in Spanish. NeuroReport, 15, 863–866. 10.1097/00001756‑200404090‑00026
    https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200404090-00026 [Google Scholar]
  37. Harris, J. W.
    (1987) Disagreement rules, referral rules, and the Spanish feminine article el. Journal of Linguistics, 23(1), 177–183. 10.1017/S0022226700011087
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226700011087 [Google Scholar]
  38. Igoa, J. M., García-Albea, J. E., & Sánchez-Casas, R.
    (1999) Gender-number dissociations in sentence production in Spanish. Rivista di Linguistica, 11(1), 163–196.
    [Google Scholar]
  39. Paolieri, D., Lotto, L., Morales, L., Bajo, T., Cubelli, R., & Job, R.
    (2010) Grammatical gender processing in romance languages: Evidence from bare noun production in Italian and Spanish. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22(3), 335–347. 10.1080/09541440902916803
    https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440902916803 [Google Scholar]
  40. The Jamovi Project
    The Jamovi Project (2021) jamovi. (Version 1.6) [Computer Software]. Retrieved fromhttps://www.jamovi.org
    [Google Scholar]
  41. Janda, R. D., & Varela-García, F.
    (1991) On Lateral Hermaphroditism and Other Variation in Spanish « Feminine » el. InLise M. Dobrin, Lynn Nichols, Rosa M. Rodríguez (eds.). Papers from the Chicago Linguistics Society (Vol.27, No.1, pp.276–290).
    [Google Scholar]
  42. Johns, B. T., Jamieson, R. K., Crump, M. J., Jones, M. N., & Mewhort, D. J. K.
    (2020) Production without rules: Using an instance memory model to exploit structure in natural language. Journal of Memory and Language, 115, 104165. 10.1016/j.jml.2020.104165
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2020.104165 [Google Scholar]
  43. Kempen, G., & Hoenkamp, E.
    (1987) An incremental procedural grammar for sentence formulation. Cognitive science, 11(2), 201–258. 10.1207/s15516709cog1102_5
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog1102_5 [Google Scholar]
  44. John, G. H., & Langley, P.
    (2013) Estimating continuous distributions in Bayesian classifiers. arXiv preprint arXiv:1302.4964.
    [Google Scholar]
  45. Levelt, W. J., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S.
    (1999) A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Cciences, 22, 1–38. 10.1017/S0140525X99001776
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99001776 [Google Scholar]
  46. Love, B. C.
    (2013) Categorization. In (Eds.), Oxford handbook of cognitive neuroscience, ed. byK. N. Ochsner & S. M. Kosslyn, 342–358. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199988709.013.0022
    https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199988709.013.0022 [Google Scholar]
  47. Mirkovic, J., MacDonald, M. C., & Seidenberg, M. S.
    (2005) Where does gender come from? Evidence from a complex inflectional system. Language and Cognitive Processes, 20(1–2), 139–167. 10.1080/01690960444000205
    https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960444000205 [Google Scholar]
  48. Nosofsky, R. M.
    (2011) The generalized context model: An exemplar model of classification. Formal approaches in categorization, 18–39. 10.1017/CBO9780511921322.002
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511921322.002 [Google Scholar]
  49. Ogneva, A.
    (2020) Gender agreement hierarchy in common gender and epicene nouns in Spanish. Borealis–An International Journal of Hispanic Linguistics, 9(1), 279–292. 10.7557/1.9.1.4839
    https://doi.org/10.7557/1.9.1.4839 [Google Scholar]
  50. Pérez-Pereira, M.
    (1991) The acquisition of Gender: What Spanish tells us. Journal of Child Language, 18, 571–590. 10.1017/S0305000900011259
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000900011259 [Google Scholar]
  51. Pharies, D.A.
    (2007) A Brief History of the Spanish Language. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press. 10.7208/chicago/9780226666846.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226666846.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  52. Pierrehumbert, J.
    (2001) Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition and contrast. Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure, ed. byJoan Bybee and Paul Hopper, 137–57. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 10.1075/tsl.45.08pie
    https://doi.org/10.1075/tsl.45.08pie [Google Scholar]
  53. Plank, F.
    (1984) Romance disagreements: phonology interfering with syntax. Journal of linguistics, 20(2), 329–349. 10.1017/S002222670001392X
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S002222670001392X [Google Scholar]
  54. Posner, R.
    (1985) Non-agreement on Romance disagreements. Journal of Linguistics, 21(2), 437–451. 10.1017/S0022226700010343
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226700010343 [Google Scholar]
  55. Pratama, Y., Tampubolon, A. R., Sianturi, L. D., Manalu, R. D., & Pangaribuan, D. F.
    (2019) Implementation of sentiment analysis on Twitter using Naïve Bayes algorithm to know the people responses to debate of DKI Jakarta governor election. Journal of Physics: Conference Series, 1175, 012102. 10.1088/1742‑6596/1175/1/012102
    https://doi.org/10.1088/1742-6596/1175/1/012102 [Google Scholar]
  56. R Core Team
    R Core Team (2020) R: A Language and environment for statistical computing. (Version 4.0) [Computer software]. Retrieved fromhttps://cran.r-project.org. (R packages retrieved from MRAN snapshot 2020-08-24).
    [Google Scholar]
  57. Real Academia Española
    Real Academia Española (2005) Diccionario panhispánico de dudas. Madrid: Santillana.
    [Google Scholar]
  58. Real Academia Española
    Real Academia Española (2009) Nueva gramática de la lengua española (Vol.2). Espasa Libros.
    [Google Scholar]
  59. Rini, J.
    (2016) Are some Spanish nouns truly grammatical hermaphrodites?Zeitschrift für romanische Philologie, 132(3), 731–754. 10.1515/zrp‑2016‑0049
    https://doi.org/10.1515/zrp-2016-0049 [Google Scholar]
  60. Russell, I., & Markov, Z.
    (2017, March). An introduction to the Weka data mining system. InProceedings of the 2017 ACM SIGCSE Technical Symposium on Computer Science Education (pp.742–742). 10.1145/3017680.3017821
    https://doi.org/10.1145/3017680.3017821 [Google Scholar]
  61. Sagarra, N., & Herschensohn, J.
    (2010) The role of proficiency and working memory in gender and number agreement processing in L1 and L2 Spanish. Lingua, 120(8), 2022–2039. 10.1016/j.lingua.2010.02.004
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.02.004 [Google Scholar]
  62. Skousen, R.
    1989Analogical Modeling of Language. Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht, Netherlands.
    [Google Scholar]
  63. Tagliamonte, S. A., & Baayen, R. H.
    (2012) Models, forests, and trees of York English: Was/were variation as a case study for statistical practice. Language Variation and Change, 24(2), 135–178. 10.1017/S0954394512000129
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394512000129 [Google Scholar]
  64. Varis, E. E.
    (2012) The Spanish feminine el at the syntax-phonology interface. PhD dissertation, University of Southern California.
    [Google Scholar]
  65. Webb, G. I.
    (1999) Decision tree grafting from the all-tests-but-one partition. InProceedings of the Sixteenth International Joint Conference on Artificial Intelligence, vol.2. San Francisco, CA: Morgan Kaufmann, 702–707.
    [Google Scholar]
  66. Wicha, N. Y., Bates, E. A., Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M.
    (2003) Potato not Pope: human brain potentials to gender expectation and agreement in Spanish spoken sentences. Neuroscience letters, 346(3), 165–168. 10.1016/S0304‑3940(03)00599‑8
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0304-3940(03)00599-8 [Google Scholar]
  67. Wicha, N. Y., Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M.
    (2004) Anticipating words and their gender: An event-related brain potential study of semantic integration, gender expectancy, and gender agreement in Spanish sentence reading. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 16(7), 1272–1288. 10.1162/0898929041920487
    https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929041920487 [Google Scholar]
  68. Zwicky, A. M.
    (1985) Rules of allomorphy and phonology-syntax interactions. Journal of Linguistics, 21(2), 431–436. 10.1017/S0022226700010331
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0022226700010331 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.21011.ell
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.21011.ell
Loading

Data & Media loading...

  • Article Type: Research Article
Keyword(s): corpus; experiment; gender; Spanish; usage-based model
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error