1887
image of Polysemies and the one representation hypothesis
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Polysemy has attracted much interdisciplinary interest in recent times. Recent discussions in psycholinguistics focus on the different processing profiles of polysemous and homonymous words, and on how to explain such different profiles. Much current research assumes that while homonymous meanings are stored in different lexical entries in the mental lexicon, polysemous senses relate to just one lexical representation, be this a list of senses or a core meaning formed by features common to all the different senses. However, there is growing skepticism towards such a one-representation hypothesis. After differentiating regular and irregular polysemies along several dimensions (not only in terms of sense representation, but also in terms of sources, acquisition and word class distribution), this paper argues that the variants of the one representation model can meet some of the challenges that have been raised. However, there are further challenges that have not yet been considered. On the one hand, nested polysemies (senses generated on the basis of iterations of metonymies or metaphors) put some pressure on the idea that senses of irregular polysemies share some set of features. On the other hand, sharing some features that could constitute a core meaning may not be sufficient for entering in co-activation patterns. In sum, while the paper defends the one-representation hypothesis in the light of recent skepticism, it also calls for further research and an eventual reformulation of the hypothesis.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/ml.23016.vic
2024-05-21
2024-12-10
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Apresjan, J. D.
    (1974) Regular polysemy. Linguistics, (), –. 10.1515/ling.1974.12.142.5
    https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1974.12.142.5 [Google Scholar]
  2. Asher, N.
    (2011) Lexical meaning in context. Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511793936
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511793936 [Google Scholar]
  3. Azuma, T., & van Orden, G. C.
    (1997) Why safe is better than fast: The relatedness of a word’s meanings affects lexical decision times. Journal of Memory and Language, , –. 10.1006/jmla.1997.2502
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.1997.2502 [Google Scholar]
  4. Beretta, A., Fiorentino, R., & Poeppel, D.
    (2005) The effects of homonymy and polysemy on lexical access: An MEG study. Cognitive Brain Research, , –. 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.12.006
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.12.006 [Google Scholar]
  5. Brocher, A., Koenig, J. P., Mauner, G., & Foraker, S.
    (2018) About sharing and commitment: the retrieval of biased and balanced irregular polysemes. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, (), –. 10.1080/23273798.2017.1381748
    https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2017.1381748 [Google Scholar]
  6. Brugman, C.
    (1988) The story of over: Polysemy, semantics, and the structure of the lexicon. Garland.
    [Google Scholar]
  7. Carston, R.
    (2002) Thoughts and utterances: The pragmatics of explicit communication. Blackwell Publishers. 10.1002/9780470754603
    https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470754603 [Google Scholar]
  8. Castroviejo, E., Ponciano, M., Hernández-Conde, J. V. & Vicente, A.
    (2024) Development of nonliteral interpretations in typically developing Spanish speaking children: light verb constructions and figurative expressions. Studia Linguistica, , –. 10.1111/stul.12222
    https://doi.org/10.1111/stul.12222 [Google Scholar]
  9. Chomsky, N.
    (2000) New Horizons in the Study of Language and Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511811937
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511811937 [Google Scholar]
  10. (2016) What kind of creatures are we?. New York: Columbia University Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  11. Collins, J.
    (2017) The copredication argument. Inquiry, , –. 10.1080/0020174X.2017.1321500
    https://doi.org/10.1080/0020174X.2017.1321500 [Google Scholar]
  12. Damirjian, A.
    (2023) A Puzzle About Mental Lexicons and Semantic Relatedness. Rev.Phil.Psych. 10.1007/s13164‑023‑00710‑z
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-023-00710-z [Google Scholar]
  13. Deane, P. D.
    (1988) Polysemy and cognition. Lingua, , –. 10.1016/0024‑3841(88)90009‑5
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0024-3841(88)90009-5 [Google Scholar]
  14. Dowty, D.
    (1979) Word Meaning and Montague Grammar. The Semantics of Verbs and Times in Generative Semantics and in Montague’s PTQ. Dordrecht: Reidel. 10.1007/978‑94‑009‑9473‑7
    https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-9473-7 [Google Scholar]
  15. Evans, V.
    (2009) How Words Mean: Lexical concepts, cognitive models and meaning construction. Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199234660.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199234660.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  16. Falkum, I. L.
    (2022) The development of non-literal uses of language: Sense conventions and pragmatic competence. Journal of Pragmatics: –. 10.1016/j.pragma.2021.12.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2021.12.002 [Google Scholar]
  17. Falkum, I. L., Recasens, M. & Clark, E. V.
    (2017) ‘The moustache sits down first’: On the acquisition of metonymy. Journal of Child Language: –. 10.1017/S0305000915000720
    https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000915000720 [Google Scholar]
  18. Foraker, S., & Murphy, G. L.
    (2012) Polysemy in sentence comprehension: Effects of meaning dominance. Journal of Memory and Language, , –. 10.1016/j.jml.2012.07.010
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.07.010 [Google Scholar]
  19. Fraser, K. E.
    (2022) The literal/non-literal divide synchronically and diachronically: The lexical semantics of an English posture verb. Doctoral Dissertation. University of the Basque Country – UPV/EHU.
  20. Frisson, S.
    (2009) Semantic underspecification in language processing. Language and Linguistics Compass, , –. 10.1111/j.1749‑818X.2008.00104.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00104.x [Google Scholar]
  21. He, A. X. & Wittenberg, E.
    (2020) The acquisition of event nominals and light verbs. Language and Linguistics Compass, : . 10.1111/lnc3.12363
    https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12363 [Google Scholar]
  22. Jackendoff, R.
    (2022) Lexical Semantics, inPapafragou, A., Trueswell, J. & Gleitman, L. (eds), The Oxford Handbook of the Mental Lexicon, Oxford University Press. 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198845003.013.3
    https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198845003.013.3 [Google Scholar]
  23. Katz, J. J.
    (1972) Semantic theory. Harper & Row.
    [Google Scholar]
  24. King, D. & Gentner, D.
    (2022) Verb Metaphoric Extension Under Semantic Strain. Cognitive Science, , e13141. 10.1111/cogs.13141
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13141 [Google Scholar]
  25. Klimek-Jankowska, D., Hwaszcz, K. & Wieczorek, J.
    (2022) The spectrum of sense remoteness in polysemy: Bridging computational and theoretical lexicography with psycholinguistics. Studies in Polish Linguistics, : –. 10.4467/23005920SPL.22.002.15759
    https://doi.org/10.4467/23005920SPL.22.002.15759 [Google Scholar]
  26. Klein, D. E., & Murphy, G. L.
    (2001) The Representation of Polysemous Words. Journal of Memory and Language, (), –. 10.1006/jmla.2001.2779
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2779 [Google Scholar]
  27. Klepousniotou, E.
    (2002) The processing of lexical ambiguity: Homonymy and polysemy in the mental lexicon. Brain and Language, (), –. 10.1006/brln.2001.2518
    https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2001.2518 [Google Scholar]
  28. Klepousniotou, E., & Baum, S. R.
    (2007) Disambiguating the ambiguity advantage effect in word recognition: An advantage for polysemous but not homonymous words. Journal of Neurolinguistics, , –. 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.02.001
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2006.02.001 [Google Scholar]
  29. Klepousniotou, E., Pike, G. B., Steinhauer, K., & Gracco, V.
    (2012) Not all ambiguous words are created equal: An EEG investigation of homonymy and polysemy. Brain and Language, , –. 10.1016/j.bandl.2012.06.007
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.06.007 [Google Scholar]
  30. Klepousniotou, E., Titone, D., & Romero, C.
    (2008) Making sense of word senses: The comprehension of polysemy depends on sense overlap. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, , –. 10.1037/a0013012
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013012 [Google Scholar]
  31. Lakoff, G.
    (1987) Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. The University of Chicago Press. 10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226471013.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  32. Li, L., Buxó-Lugo, A., Jacobs, C. L., & Slevc, L. R.
    (2023) Are lexical representations graded or discrete?Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, (), –. 10.1177/17470218231187027
    https://doi.org/10.1177/17470218231187027 [Google Scholar]
  33. Liu, M.
    (2023) Mental simulation and language comprehension: The case of copredication. Mind and Language, , –. 10.1111/mila.12459
    https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12459 [Google Scholar]
  34. Löhr, G., & Michel, G.
    (2022) Copredication in context: A predicative processing approach. Cognitive Science, , . 10.1111/cogs.13138
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.13138 [Google Scholar]
  35. Lombard, A., Huyghe, R., Barque, L. & Gras, D.
    (2023) Regular polysemy and novel word-sense identification. The Mental Lexicon, ():  – . 10.1075/ml.21002.lom
    https://doi.org/10.1075/ml.21002.lom [Google Scholar]
  36. Long, M., Shukla, V., & Rubio-Fernández, P.
    (2021) The development of simile comprehension: From similarity to scalar implicature. Child Development, (), –. 10.1111/cdev.13507
    https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13507 [Google Scholar]
  37. MacGregor, L. J., Bouwsema, J., & Klepousniotou, E.
    (2015) Sustained meaning activation for polysemous but not homonymous words: Evidence from EEG. Neuropsychologia, , –. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.008
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.01.008 [Google Scholar]
  38. Majid, A., Boster, J. S., & Bowerman, M.
    (2008) The cross-linguistic categorization of everyday events: A study of cutting and breaking. Cognition, , –. 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.009
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.08.009 [Google Scholar]
  39. Martín-González, I., Ronderos, C. R., Castroviejo, E., Schroeder, K. F., Falkum, I. L., & Vicente, A.
    (forth.). That child is a grasshopper (because he jumps a lot): children’s development of novel metaphor comprehension. Journal of Child Language.
    [Google Scholar]
  40. Murphy, E.
    (2021) Predicate order and coherence in copredication. Inquiry, 10.1080/0020174X.2021.1958054
    https://doi.org/10.1080/0020174X.2021.1958054 [Google Scholar]
  41. Ortega-Andrés, M. & Vicente, A.
    (2019) Polysemy and co-predication. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, (), . 10.5334/gjgl.564
    https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.564 [Google Scholar]
  42. Pickering, M. J., & Frisson, S.
    (2001) Processing ambiguous verbs: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, (), –.
    [Google Scholar]
  43. Pietroski, P.
    (2018) Conjoining Meanings: Semantics without Truth Values. Oxford University Press. 10.1093/oso/9780198812722.001.0001
    https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198812722.001.0001 [Google Scholar]
  44. Pouscolous, N. & Tomasello, M.
    (2020) Early birds: Metaphor understanding in 3-year-olds. Journal of Pragmatics: –. 10.1016/j.pragma.2019.05.021
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2019.05.021 [Google Scholar]
  45. Pritchard, T.
    (2019) Analogical cognition: an insight into word meaning. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, , –. 10.1007/s13164‑018‑0419‑y
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-018-0419-y [Google Scholar]
  46. (2022) Proprietary linguistic meaning. Synthese, . 10.1007/s11229‑022‑03776‑x
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-022-03776-x [Google Scholar]
  47. Pustejovsky, J.
    (1995) The generative lexicon. MIT Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  48. Rodd, J., Gaskell, G., & Marslen-Wilson, W.
    (2002) Making sense of semantic ambiguity: Semantic competition in lexical access. Journal of Memory and Language, (), –. 10.1006/jmla.2001.2810
    https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2001.2810 [Google Scholar]
  49. (2004) Modeling the Effects of Semantic Ambiguity in Word Recognition. Cognitive Science, , –. 10.1207/s15516709cog2801_4
    https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2801_4 [Google Scholar]
  50. Quilty-Dunn, J.
    (2021) Polysemy and thought: towards a generative theory of concepts. Mind and Language, : –. 10.1111/mila.12328
    https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12328 [Google Scholar]
  51. Rundblad, G., & Annaz, D.
    (2010) The atypical development of metaphor and metonymy comprehension in children with autism. Autism, (), –. 10.1177/1362361309340667
    https://doi.org/10.1177/1362361309340667 [Google Scholar]
  52. Srinivasan, M. & Snedeker, J.
    (2011) Judging a book by its cover and its contents: The representation of polysemous and homophonous meanings in four-year-old children. Cognitive Psychology, , –. 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.03.002
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2011.03.002 [Google Scholar]
  53. (2014) Polysemy and the Taxonomic Constraint: Children’s Representation of Words that Label Multiple Kinds, Language Learning and Development, :, –, 10.1080/15475441.2013.820121
    https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2013.820121 [Google Scholar]
  54. Srinivasan, M., Al-Mughairy, S., Foushee, R., Barner, D.
    (2017) Learning language from within: Children use semantic generalizations to infer new word meanings. Cognition, , –. 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.019
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.10.019 [Google Scholar]
  55. Verkuyl, H. J.
    (1993) A Theory of Aspectuality: The Interaction between Temporal and Atemporal Structure. Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9780511597848
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511597848 [Google Scholar]
  56. Vicente, A.
    (2018) Polysemy and word meaning: An account of lexical meaning for different kinds of content words. Philosophical Studies, : –. 10.1007/s11098‑017‑0900‑y
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s11098-017-0900-y [Google Scholar]
  57. (2021) Chomskyan arguments against truth-conditional semantics based upon variability and co-predication. Erkenntnis, : –. 10.1007/s10670‑019‑00138‑x
    https://doi.org/10.1007/s10670-019-00138-x [Google Scholar]
  58. Vicente, A. & Falkum, I. L.
    (2017) Polysemy. InOxford Research Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Ed.Mark Aronoff. New York: Oxford University Press. 10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.325
    https://doi.org/10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.325 [Google Scholar]
  59. Vosniadou, S.
    (1987) Children and metaphors. Child Development, : –. 10.2307/1130223
    https://doi.org/10.2307/1130223 [Google Scholar]
  60. Winner, E., Rosenstiel, A. & Gardner, H.
    (1976) The development of metaphoric understanding. Developmental Psychology: –. 10.1037/0012‑1649.12.4.289
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.12.4.289 [Google Scholar]
  61. Yurchenko, A., Lopukhina, A., & Dragoy, O.
    (2020) Metaphor Is Between Metonymy and Homonymy: Evidence From Event-Related Potentials. Frontiers in Psychology, . 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02113
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.02113 [Google Scholar]
  62. Zwicky, A., & Sadock, J.
    (1975) Ambiguity tests and how to fail them. InJ. P. Kimball (Ed.), Syntax and Semantics (Vol. 4) (pp.–). Academic Press. 10.1163/9789004368828_002
    https://doi.org/10.1163/9789004368828_002 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/ml.23016.vic
Loading
  • Article Type: Research Article
Keywords: lexical entry ; polysemy ; copredication ; core meanings
This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error