1887
Volume 26, Issue 2-3
  • ISSN 0929-0907
  • E-ISSN: 1569-9943
USD
Buy:$35.00 + Taxes

Abstract

Abstract

Previous research suggests that both patterns in orthography and cultural-specific associations of space-time affect how people map space onto time. In the current study, we focused on Chinese Buddhists, an understudied population, investigating how religious experiences influence their mental representations of time. Results showed that Chinese Buddhists could represent time spatially corresponding to left-to-right, right-to-left and top-to-bottom orientations in their religious scripts. Specifically, they associated earlier events with the starting point of the reading and later times with the endpoint. We also found that Chinese Buddhists were more likely to represent time in a clockwise way than Chinese atheists. This is because Buddhism regards time as cyclic and consisting of repeating ages (i.e. Wheel of Time). Taken together, we provide first psychological evidence that Chinese Buddhists’ spatial representations of time are different from atheists’, due to their religious experiences, namely, both the reading direction in Buddhist texts and Buddhist concepts of time.

Loading

Article metrics loading...

/content/journals/10.1075/pc.19002.li
2021-02-12
2025-02-13
Loading full text...

Full text loading...

References

  1. Anderson, Tenshin
    2001Being upright: Zen meditation and the Bodhisattva precepts. Berkeley, CA: Rodmell.
    [Google Scholar]
  2. Ariel, Robert , Ibrahim Al-Harthy , Christopher Was & John Dunlosky
    2011 Habitual reading biases in the allocation of study time. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review18(5). 1015–1021. 10.3758/s13423‑011‑0128‑3
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-011-0128-3 [Google Scholar]
  3. Bergen, Benjamin & Ting Ting Chan Lau
    2012 Writing direction affects how people map space onto time. Frontiers in Cultural Psychology3. 109.
    [Google Scholar]
  4. Berzin, Alexander
    1997Taking the Kalachakra initiation. New York: Snow Lion Publications, Incorporated.
    [Google Scholar]
  5. Boroditsky, Lera
    2000 Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition75(1). 1–28. 10.1016/S0010‑0277(99)00073‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00073-6 [Google Scholar]
  6. 2001 Does language shape thought? English and Mandarin speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology43(1). 1–22. 10.1006/cogp.2001.0748
    https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.2001.0748 [Google Scholar]
  7. Casasanto, Daniel
    2009 Embodiment of abstract concepts: Good and bad in right- and left-handers. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General138(3). 351–367. 10.1037/a0015854
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015854 [Google Scholar]
  8. 2017 The hierarchical structure of mental metaphors. In Beate Hampe (ed.), Metaphor: Embodied cognition and discourse, 46–61. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/9781108182324.004
    https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108182324.004 [Google Scholar]
  9. Casasanto, Daniel & Robert Bottini
    2014 Mirror reading can reverse the flow of time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General143(2). 473–479. 10.1037/a0033297
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033297 [Google Scholar]
  10. Casasanto, Daniel & Kyle Jasmin
    2012 The hands of time: Temporal gestures in English speakers. Cognitive Linguistics23(4). 643–674. 10.1515/cog‑2012‑0020
    https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2012-0020 [Google Scholar]
  11. Chang, Wonsuk
    2009 Reflections on time and related ideas in the Yijing. Philosophy East and West59(2). 216–229. 10.1353/pew.0.0047
    https://doi.org/10.1353/pew.0.0047 [Google Scholar]
  12. Chen, Jenn-Yeu
    2007 Do Chinese and English speakers think about time differently? Failure of replicating Boroditsky (2001). Cognition104(2). 427–436. 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.012
    https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2006.09.012 [Google Scholar]
  13. Clark, Herbert
    1973 Space, time, semantics, and the child. In Timothy Moore (ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language, 27–63. New York: Academic Press. 10.1016/B978‑0‑12‑505850‑6.50008‑6
    https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-505850-6.50008-6 [Google Scholar]
  14. Duffy, Sarah
    2014 The role of cultural artifacts in the interpretation of metaphorical expressions about time. Metaphor and Symbol29(2). 94–112. 10.1080/10926488.2014.889989
    https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2014.889989 [Google Scholar]
  15. Eiki, Hoshino & Ian Reader
    1997 Pilgrimage and peregrination: Contextualizing the Saikoku Junrei and the Shikoku Henro. Japanese Journal of Religious Studies24(3–4). 271–299.
    [Google Scholar]
  16. Eikmeier, Verena , Simone Alex-Ruf , Claudia Maienborn , & Rolf Ulrich
    2015 How strongly linked are mental time and space along the left–right axis?Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 41(6), 1878–1883.
    [Google Scholar]
  17. Eliot, Thomas Stearns
    2010Notes towards the definition of culture. London, England: Faber & Faber.
    [Google Scholar]
  18. Evans, Vyvyan
    2013Language and time: A cognitive linguistics approach. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/CBO9781107340626
    https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781107340626 [Google Scholar]
  19. Fuente, Juanma de la , Julio Santiago , Antonio Román , Cristina Dumitrache & Daniel Casasanto
    2014 When you think about it, your past is in front of you: How culture shapes spatial conceptions of time. Psychological Science25(9). 1682–1690. 10.1177/0956797614534695
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614534695 [Google Scholar]
  20. Fuhrman, Orly & Lera Boroditsky
    2010 Cross-Cultural differences in mental representations of time: Evidence from an implicit nonlinguistic task. Cognitive Science34(8). 1430–1451. 10.1111/j.1551‑6709.2010.01105.x
    https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01105.x [Google Scholar]
  21. Gibbs, Raymond
    1999 Taking metaphor out of our heads and into the cultural world. In Raymond Gibbs & Gerard Steen (eds.), Metaphor in cognitive linguistics, 145–166. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 10.1075/cilt.175.09gib
    https://doi.org/10.1075/cilt.175.09gib [Google Scholar]
  22. 2017Metaphor wars: Conceptual metaphors in human life. New York: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/9781107762350
    https://doi.org/10.1017/9781107762350 [Google Scholar]
  23. Gijssels, Tom & Daniel Casasanto
    2017 Conceptualizing time in terms of space: Experimental evidence. In Barbara Dancygier (ed.), Cambridge handbook of cognitive linguistics, 651–668. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 10.1017/9781316339732.041
    https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316339732.041 [Google Scholar]
  24. Gregory, Peter
    1986Traditions of meditation in Chinese Buddhism, vol.4. Honolulu: University of Hawaii Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  25. Hendricks, Rose & Lera Boroditsky
    2017 New space–time metaphors foster new nonlinguistic representations. Topics in Cognitive Science9. 800–818. 10.1111/tops.12279
    https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12279 [Google Scholar]
  26. Hevia, Maria de , Véronique Izard , Aurélie Coubart , Elizabeth Spelke & Arlette Streri
    2014 Representations of space, time, and number in neonates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences111(13). 4809–4813. 10.1073/pnas.1323628111
    https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1323628111 [Google Scholar]
  27. Huck, Schuyler
    2004Reading statistics and research. San Francisco: Allyn & Bacon.
    [Google Scholar]
  28. Lakoff, George & Mark Johnson
    1980Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  29. Landau, Mark , Brian Meier & Lucas Keefer
    2010 A metaphor-enriched social cognition. Psychological Bulletin136(6). 1045–1067. 10.1037/a0020970
    https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020970 [Google Scholar]
  30. Li, Heng , Van Quynh Bui & Yu Cao
    2018 One country, two cultures: Implicit space-time mappings in southern and northern Vietnamese. European Journal of Social Psychology48(3). 560–565. 10.1002/ejsp.2356
    https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2356 [Google Scholar]
  31. Miles, Lynden , Louise Nind & Neil Macrae
    2010 Moving through time. Psychological Science21(2). 222–223. 10.1177/0956797609359333
    https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797609359333 [Google Scholar]
  32. Ouellet, Marc , Julio Santiago , María Jesús Funes & Juan Lupiáñez
    2010 Thinking about the future moves attention to the right. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance36(1). 17–24.
    [Google Scholar]
  33. Sobbe, Linda , Edith Scheifele , Claudia Maienborn & Rolf Ulrich
    2019 The space–time congruency effect: A meta-analysis. Cognitive Science43(1). e12709.
    [Google Scholar]
  34. Sousa, Hilário de
    2012 Generational differences in the orientation of time in Cantonese speakers as a function of changes in the direction of Chinese writing. Frontiers in Psychology3. 255. 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00255
    https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00255 [Google Scholar]
  35. Triandis, Harry
    1996 The psychological measurement of cultural syndromes. American Psychologist51(4). 407–415. 10.1037/0003‑066X.51.4.407
    https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.51.4.407 [Google Scholar]
  36. Tversky, Barbara , Sol Kugelmass & Atalia Winter
    1991 Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology23(4). 515–557. 10.1016/0010‑0285(91)90005‑9
    https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(91)90005-9 [Google Scholar]
  37. Vinsrygg, Synnove
    1986Time in archaeological thought: China and the West. Amherst: The University of Massachusetts Press.
    [Google Scholar]
  38. Yang, Wen Xing & Ying Sun
    2016 A monolingual mind can have two time lines: Exploring space-time mappings in mandarin monolinguals. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review23(3). 1–8. 10.3758/s13423‑015‑0964‑7
    https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0964-7 [Google Scholar]
/content/journals/10.1075/pc.19002.li
Loading
/content/journals/10.1075/pc.19002.li
Loading

Data & Media loading...

This is a required field
Please enter a valid email address
Approval was successful
Invalid data
An Error Occurred
Approval was partially successful, following selected items could not be processed due to error